Download study - Association for Transpersonal Psychology

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Team composition wikipedia , lookup

Communication in small groups wikipedia , lookup

False consensus effect wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
THE EFFECT OF CONTEMPLATIVE PRACTICE
ON INTRAPERSONAL, INTERPERSONAL, AND
TRANSPERSONAL DIMENSIONS OF THE SELF-CONCEPT
Charlotte J. Haimerl
Elizabeth R.Valentine
Egham, Surrey, United Kingdom
ABSTRACT: This study investigated the effect of Buddhist meditation on self-directedness,
cooperativeness, and self-transcendence, representing the intrapersonal, interpersonal, and
transpersonal levels of the self-concept, respectively. On empirical and theoretical grounds,
the authors predicted that meditation experience would lead to development in each of these
dimensions. Prospective meditators (n = 28) with no experience, beginner meditators (n = 58)
with less than 2 years' experience, and advanced meditators (n = 73) with more than 2 years'
experience of meditation were compared on the intrapersonal, interpersonal, and transpersonal subscales of the Temperament and Character Inventory. The hypothesis was fully supported for all three dimensions of the self-concept, level of development being a positive
function of meditation experience. Implications and limitations of the study are discussed.
INTRODUCTION
Since the Western world became acquainted with Eastern worldviews in the 1960s
(Suzuki, 1960), the practice of meditation-the main characteristic of the Buddhist
way of life-has been increasingly applied within the clinical sector to supplement
conventional psychotherapies (Carrington & Ephron, 1975; Kabat-Zinn, Lipworth,
Burney, & Sellers, 1986; Schuster, 1979; Shapiro, 1982). As such, meditation is
mainly applied as an eclectic tool for the client's self-regulation and self-exploration
(Shapiro, 1992). The highly intellectual, contemporary writer and vigorous advocate of the perennial philosophy, Ken Wilber (1983), even claims: "meditation--or
a similar and truly contemplative practice-becomes an absolute ethical imperative
. . . if we are to contribute to evolution and not merely siphon it off' (p. 321).
Likewise, within its religious background, meditation aims for self-development.
However, ultimately the cultivation of a nondualistic perception of existence is
sought, transcending the experience of an independently existing self, thus supporting compassion and sensitivity to the welfare of others (Krishnamurti Foundation,
1996). Although the variety of meditation methods that have evolved is extremely
large, Walsh (1983) summarizes them as "a family of practices that train attention
in order to heighten awareness and bring mental processes under greater voluntary
control" (p. 19). Hence, through observing and training the workings of one's mind,
different states of self-consciousness are said to be reached.
Empirical research has been conducted to reveal the specific phenomenological,
physiological, and psychological correlates of meditation (see Farthing, 1992, for a
review). The most common phenomenological effect reported has been relaxation.
The Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 2001. Vol. 33. No. I
37
Physiological studies have identified changes in physiological activity as well as in
cortical activity during meditative states. These bodily changes support the view
that the described phenomenological, as well as the following psychological, effects
of meditation are truly causal phenomena.
Within the psychological domain, research has related the practice of meditation at
the intrapersonal level to an increment in internal locus of control (Hjelle, 1974),
overall domain-specific sense of control (Astin, 1997), and self-determination
(Penner, Zingle, Dyck, & Truch, 1974). At the interpersonal level, relations to the
positive development of ego distance (Pelletier, 1974), altruism (Penner et al.,
1974), social-psychological attitudes (Hanley & Spates, 1978), and empathy (Lesh,
1970), as well as a decrement in hostility (Abrams & Siegel, 1978), have been
reported.
Psychological studies have suggested that meditation encourages self-actualization
(Hjelle, 1974; Nidich, Seeman, & Dreskin, 1973; Seeman, Nidich, & Banta, 1972),
a concept assessed through a measurement scale designed by Shostrom (1964) that
taps 12 dimensions of personal orientation. The domains that were shown to be significantly, positively affected by meditation practice in these three investigations
were inner directedness, spontaneity, self-regard, and self-actualizing value, classifiable as intrapersonal aspects; and capacity for intimate contact, clearly belonging
to the interpersonal dimension. On the basis of a thorough meta-analysis, these
changes were recently linked with meditators' increased self-transcendental experiences (Alexander, Rainforth, & Gelderloos, 1991).
Past research has already focused specifically on the self-concept construct, identifying a relationship between meditation practice and a more positive and strong definition of self-concept at the intrapersonal, interpersonal, and transpersonal levels
(Turnbull & Norris, 1982; Nystul & Garde, 1977). Nystul and Garde's (1977)
results depict meditators as judging their relation to themselves, to others, and to
God more positively. Turnbull and Norris's (1982) study, on the other hand, illustrates that within the process of meditative practice, people's self-concepts approximate toward what they want to be like (i.e., their ideal self) and how they perceive
others (i.e., their social self).
These questionnaire-based studies were truly experimental in nature, establishing
treatmentho-treatment or before/after treatment conditions, focusing on one specific method of meditation (i.e., concentrative or mindfulness meditation [see below]).
However, various methodological shortcomings, such as small number of participants, student samples, non-random allocation of participants to condition an/or
absence of a control group, decrease their scientific value. Considering that probably a specific psychological type of person is attracted to meditation (Shapiro,
1987), the fact that only one study controlled for such group specificity bias by
matching control and experimental groups for interest in the areas of Buddhism and
meditation (Hjelle, 1974) threatens their validity still further. Moreover, Hjelle's
study applied a cross-sectional design, thereby reducing the high-demand characteristics associated with longitudinal designs which require multiple administration
38
The Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 2001, Vol. 33, No. 1
of a self-report reliant measurement tool that is, in any case, highly susceptible to
such biases. Proceeding otherwise risks attributing meditation effects that might be
either an intrinsic quality of the kind of people who approach this activity in the first
place (Shapiro, 1987) or may have arisen from participants' compliance with
guessed outcome expectations on behalf of the experimenter (Hjelle, 1974). Taking
these methodological pitfalls into account, it would be preferable to investigate the
subject matter under naturalistic conditions-using a quasi-experimental designwith a nonstudent sample and an attempt to restrict the key confounding factors,
group specificity bias and demand characteristics.
The Objectives of the Present Study
The current study set out to extend the insight into the effects of meditation on people's self-conceptualizations. The primary focal point was whether or not changes
occur in people's self-concept at different stages of development within their regular, self-selected practice of Buddhist meditation. Here it must be stressed that within the Buddhist tradition it is believed that two kinds of meditation methods, concentrative and mindfulness meditation, have to be trained in parallel. Concentrative
meditation methods teach the mind to create space for the truly meaningful matters
in life by practicing the removal of thought processes through focusing one's entire
attention on one specific stimulus. Mindfulness meditation methods teach sensitive
awareness to that which is meaningful within and around us by directing one's
entire attention to all thought processes (Goleman, 1988). The flexibility of the
mind to apply either of them when necessary seems to be what is aimed for. As
Geshe Tashi Tsering, a Tibetan lama and leader of the London Yamjang Buddhist
center, explained to the first author: "The least we want is a dull, relaxed mind. We
want sharp, alert, and receptive minds. The exclusive training of one type of meditation makes the mind dull." The study's naturalistic premise thus excludes the possibility of differentiating between these two types of meditation methods, as has
been done in past research.
The second major objective was to extend the understanding of the effect of meditative practice by using a more recent and integrative model of the self-concept, taking the intrapersonal, interpersonal, and transpersonal dimensions equally into
account. This seems particularly crucial, since the transpersonal has so far been little considered in a scientific manner. In particular, the only directly relevant discoveries made have been (a) meditators' self-reports claim a significantly more positive relationship between their selves and God (Nystul & Garde, 1977) and (b) the
link between self-transcendental experiences and self-actualization (Alexander et
al., 1991).
The neglect of this area in psychology has often been criticized (e.g., Bergin, 1991).
Indeed, within the area of personality, the transpersonal dimension has only recently been acknowledged as being of significance. According to MacDonald and his
colleagues (MacDonald, Tsagarakis, & Holland, 1994), there are only two personality measurements available that transcend conventional models, such as the "Big
w
Five -factor model of Costa and McCrea (1992), by including this aspect. One of
The Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 2001, Vol. 33, No. I
39
them is Friedman's (1983) Self-Expansiveness Level Form (SELF); the other is
Cloninger, Svrakic, and Przybeck's (1993) Temperament and Character Inventory
(TCI). Since the SELF appears generally less comprehensive and supposedly bears
weaknesses in terms of operationalization and definition as well as a gender bias
(MacDonald, LeClair, & Holland, 1995), the TCI was judged to be more adequate
for scientific inquiry.
In the TCI-associated psychobiological model of personality, Cloninger and his colleagues (1993) differentiate two distinct personality structures: temperament and
character. Personality development is assumed to occur via the dynamic interaction
between these two factors according to the epigenetic principle, reciprocal feedback. The temperament factors are assumed to "[be] independently heritable, manifest early in life, and involve preconceptual biases in perceptual memory and habit
formation." Character factors "mature in adulthood and influence personal and
social effectiveness by insight learning about self-concepts" (p. 975). In particular,
temperament denotes traits having to do with associative learning in response to
novelty, punishment, reward, and persistence, while the character dimension
includes response biases in relation to three different facets of the self-concept that
are the focus of this investigation: intrapersonal, interpersonal, and transpersonal.
Specifically, how people identify their selves on these levels significantly defines
their personality make-up.
According to Cloninger et al. (1993), people's self-concepts vary in their degree of
self-identification within three dimensions: self-directedness (intrapersonal level),
cooperativeness (interpersonal level), and self-transcendence (transpersonal level).
Self-directedness is referred to as a developmental process, concerning a person's
"self-determination and 'willpower', or the ability . . . to control, regulate and adapt
behaviour to fit the situation in accord with individually chosen goals and values"
(p. 979). Cooperativeness is defined as "Individual differences in identification with
and acceptance of other people" (p. 980). Finally, self-transcendence is encapsulated as "[the] identification with everything conceived as essential and consequential
parts of a unified whole" (p. 981).
On the basis of the empirical findings of meditation's psychological correlates, positive development should be expected within the intrapersonal, interpersonal, and
transpersonal domains of the self-concept. Although the most appropriate method
of studying this phenomenon would certainly be to accompany participants through
this process (i.e., a longitudinal design), in the light of the outlined difficulties with
demand characteristics it appears advisable to compare different individuals who
are at different stages in their meditative practice at one point in time (i.e., a crosssectional design). The control of this potentially confounding variable can be seen
as the study's third key point. The fourth empirical objective is the restriction of
another bias, namely group specificity bias, by matching the control group (made
up of individuals who are planning to start practicing Buddhist meditation, or
"prospective meditators") and the experimental groups ("active meditators") for
their interest in Buddhist meditation.
40
The Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 2001, Vol. 33, No. 1
\
Furthermore, as the effect of meditation is clearly not an all-or-nothing process
(Krishnamurti Foundation, 1996), it is necessary to distinguish between individuals
who have become involved recently (beginner meditators) and individuals who
have been involved for a longer period of time (advanced meditators). To acknowledge the effects of meditation as a continual process, involving the acquisition of
skills that need to be learned and which are not simply picked up at once, can be
seen as the fifth focus of the study.
On the basis of past research, we hypothesized that there would be significant
development in all three aspects of the self-concept-the intrapersonal (self-directedness), interpersonal (cooperativeness), and transpersonal (self-transcendence)
dimensions-as a positive linear function of meditation experience. We therefore
predicted that scores on each of the corresponding subscales of the TCI would be
higher for advanced meditators than for beginner meditators and higher for beginner meditators than for prospective meditators.
Design
A cross-sectional design was employed, with one independent variable and three
dependent variables. The independent variable was meditation experience, encompassing three levels. The first sample was composed of participants who had the
intention to start practicing meditation but no experience yet. This group, labelled
prospective meditators, was the baseline comparison group, controlling for group
specificity of people drawn to Buddhist meditation. The second sample, called
beginner meditators, consisted of participants who had meditated regularly (i.e., at
least once a week) for less than 2 years. The third group, advanced meditators, consisted of participants who had meditated at least once a week for more than 2 years.
The participants' performances in these last two samples should be relatively equal
if they were based on motivational factors such as experimenter bias and demand
characteristics (Alexander et al., 1991). The cut-off of 2 years followed the advice
given by several spiritual leaders of the Buddhistcenters and has, furthermore, been
applied in past meditation research (Shapiro, 1992). Within each sample there were
two subgroups, divided according to whether participants were attending Western
or Tibetan Buddhist meditation centers.
The three dependent variables were the participants' scores on the intrapersonal,
interpersonal, and transpersonal subscales of the self-concept inventory. To control
for the existing mean age difference between groups, this factor was included in the
design as a covariate.
Participants
Participants were drawn from people attending six Buddhist meditation centers in
London. Three of these centers were adherents of the Tibetan Buddhist tradition,
two of the centres being mahayanist and one being vajrayanist. The remaining three
The Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 2001, Vol. 33, No. 1
41
centers followed a Westernized version of Buddhism designated Friends of the
Western Buddhist Order. These centers were selected on the basis of availability;
study participants taken from them hence represented an opportunity sample of the
population of people who are interested in the practice of meditation. A total of 300
questionnaires was distributed, with an overall response rate of 53%.
Approximately equal numbers of questionnaires were returned for the three samples
from both Tibetan and Westernized Buddhist centers. The total sample consisted of
159 participants, of which 17.6% were prospective, 36.5% were beginner, and
45.9% were advanced practitioners of Buddhist meditation. Within these three
experimental samples, gender was relatively equally represented, and age was positively related to meditation experience (see Table 1). The three samples were
almost exclusively Caucasian: 100% of the prospective meditators, 87% of the
TABLE 1
BASIC DETAILS OF SAMPLES
-
n
(females)
n (males)
n (Western)
n (Tibetan) Mean age in years
(range)
Prospective meditators
17
11
15
13
3 1.68 (20 - 68)
Beginner meditators
28
30
34
24
33.78 (19 - 64)
Advanced meditators
?A
39
36
17
40.64 ( 1 8 - 73)
beginner meditators, and 98% of the advanced meditators. The participants' occupational backgrounds were so wide ranging (e.g., students, builders, charity workers, musicians, teachers, clerks, writers, designers, lecturers, lawyers, therapists)
that no categorization was attempted.
Materials
One hundred and two items were drawn from three of the subscales of the
Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI) (Cloninger et al., 1993): 38 items from
the intrapersonal subscale, 38 items from the interpersonal subscale, and 26 items
from the transpersonal subscale. The TCI, a paper-and-pencil self-assessment questionnaire containing seven subscales relating to the four temperament and three
character factors, has generally received adequate support in terms of reliability and
validity (MacDonald et al., 1995). Nevertheless, more research is required for a
definitive judgement of its psychometric value. Each subscale represents separate
aspects of the respective subdimension. The intrapersonal, self-directedness scale
encompasses five different aspects, represented as continua: personal responsibility
taking vs. blaming, purposefulness vs. goal-undirectedness, resourcefulness vs. apathy, self-acceptance vs. self-striving, and congruent second nature vs. personal distrust. The interpersonal, cooperativeness scale is specified by five basic continuous
developmental qualities: social acceptance vs. intolerance, empathy vs. social disinterest, helpfulness vs. unhelpfulness, compassion vs. revengefulness, and pureheartedness vs. selfishness. The transpersonal, self-transcendence scale is divided
into three distinct, key, developmental continua: self-forgetfulness vs. self-con-
42
The Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 2001, Vol. 33, No. 1
sciousness, transpersonal identification vs. self-isolation, and spiritual acceptance
vs. rational materialism.
The items were administered as a single questionnaire, in random order. However,
the following changes were made: the original subscales were shortened somewhat,
the wording refined, and the response pattern changed. Within the intrapersonal
subscale, items referring to self-acceptance were reduced from 11 to eight, and only
nine items relating to congruent second nature were included instead of 12. Within
the interpersonal subscale, four items were omitted: one from the helpfulness
domain, two from the compassion domain, and one from the pure-hearted conscience domain. The transpersonal subscale was reduced from 33 items to 26, three
items being removed from the spiritual acceptance section, three items from the
self-forgetful section, and one from the transpersonal identification section. Items
were eliminated following advice from spiritual leaders at the Buddhist centers,
who judged particular items as inadequate, intrusive, or repetitive. Cronbach's alpha
was performed on the resulting subscales, in all cases exceeding the generally
accepted criterion for internal consistency of 0.7. The values obtained were 0.86 for
the intrapersonal subscale, 0.78 for the interpersonal subscale, and 0.78 for the
transpersonal subscale. To prevent response bias, the wording of the items was frequently reversed, resulting in 51 negative and 5 1 positive statements. To expand the
participants' choice, a four-point Likert-type scale (strongly agree, slightly agree,
slightly disagree, strongly disagree) was chosen instead of the pre-given yes/no format. No neutral response option was allowed, due to the problems of interpreting a
midway position, which is ambiguous as to whether the participant is opinionless or
simply indecisive. The participants' answers were scored as follows: For the positive statements, "strongly agree" was given a value of 3, "slightly agree" a value of
1, "slightly disagree" a value of -1, and "strongly disagree" a value of -3. For the
negative statements, this schema was reversed. These scores were totalled for the
individual subscales, yielding three individual scores for each participant relating
respectively to the intra-, inter- and transpersonal self-concept dimensions. Possible
scores ranged from -1 14 to +114 for the intrapersonal and the interpersonal subscales and from -78 to +78 for the transpersonal subscale.
Prior to completing the questionnaire, participants were asked to provide information on the following demographic characteristics: age, gender, occupation, ethnicity, actively practiced religion, duration of meditation experience, frequency of
meditation experience, and method of meditation practiced.
Procedure
Participants were recruited over a period of 7 weeks. They were approached on open
days at the meditation centers, during introductory courses on meditation practice,
or at regular meditative meetings. Participants were not told about the true purpose
of the study in order to reduce potential social desirability. The study was introduced as a research project investigating general attitudes of Western people interested in Eastern philosophies. Participants were supplied with questionnaires and
allowed to answer them in their own time, returning them to a third party or an
The Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 2001, Vol. 33, No. I
available box in the meditation center or via a stamped, addressed envelope directly to the investigator.
Total scores on each of the three subscales of the TCI were computed. In the case
of two participants, missing values on 11 and 7 items respectively were scored as
zero. Since there were fewer items on the transpersonal subscale than on the intrapersonal and interpersonal subscales, scores were converted to proportions of the
maximum possible, making direct comparisons possible between scales. An arcsin
transformation was performed on the data prior to statistical analysis. Means and
standard deviations of the resulting scores on the three subscales for each group are
shown in Table 2.
A repeated measures analysis of variance was carried out with meditation experience (prospective, beginner, advanced) as a between-participants variable, scale
(intrapersonal, interpersonal, transpersonal) as a within-participants variable, score
as the dependent variable, and age as a covariate. Post hoc comparisons were conducted using Tukey's honestly significant difference test for the between-participants variable and Bonferroni tests for the within-participants variable. The significant results obtained are set out in Table 3.
Age was not significantly correlated with score (B = 0.00037, t = 0.39, p = 0.696).
The contribution of age to the prediction of score is negligible. There were highly
significant main effects of both meditation experience and scale (see Figure 1). The
results of the post hoc tests show that advanced meditators scored significantly
higher than prospective meditators on all three subscales, advanced meditators
scored significantly higher than beginner meditators on the interpersonal subscale,
and beginner meditators scored significantly higher than prospective meditators on
the transpersonal subscale (this last being the largest of these effects). All three
groups scored significantly higher on the interpersonal than on the transpersonal
subscale, and beginner and advanced meditators scored significantly higher on the
interpersonal than on the intrapersonal subscale, but only the advanced meditators
scored significantly higher on the transpersonal than on the intrapersonal susbcale.
The results of the current investigation demonstrated that scores on the intrapersonal, interpersonal, and transpersonal levels of the TCI were a positive function
of meditation experience, suggesting that progress in Buddhist meditation leads to
significant growth in these components of personality. Overall, scores were highest
on the interpersonal scale and lowest on the intrapersonal scale. The greatest
changes as a function of meditation experience were on the transpersonal scale.
These results are consistent with the empirical research reviewed in the Introduction
indicating that the practice of meditation leads to positive development of people's
self-concept definition at the intrapersonal level (Nystul & Garde, 1977; Turnbull &
44
The Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 2001, Vol. 33, No. I
TABLE 2
MEAN SCORES
( A N D S T A N D A RD DEVIATIONS) O N THE INTRAPERSONAL, INTERPERSONAL, A N D TR A N S P ER S O N A L
SUBSCALES OF THE TCI FOR PROSPECTIVE, BEGINNER, AND ADVANCED MEDITATORS
Prospective
(n = 28)
Beginner
(n = 58)
Advanced
(n = 73)
Combined
(n = 159)
Intrapersonal
1.710 (0.162)
1.779 (0.179)
1.83 1 (0.187)
1.79 1 (0.184)
Interpersonal
I ,900 (0.1 17)
1.919 (0.144)
1.990 (0.139)
1.948 (0.142)
Transpersonal
1.703 (0.2 1 1 )
1.834 (0.169)
1.890 (0.175)
1.837 (0.191)
Combined
1.77 1 (0.168)
1.844 (0.165)
1.904 (0.168)
S IGNIFICANT
RESULTS OF
TABLE 3
ANOVA INCLUDING
POST HOC TESTS
Effect
Experience
Prospective
Beginner
Advanced
Post
hoc
Mean
SD
Mean
SD
Mean
SD
Statistic
Combined*
1.77
0.17
1.84
0.16
1.90
0.17
F(2.155)
=9.4 1
Intrapersonal
1.71
0.16
1.78
0.18
1.83
0.19
Interpersonal
1.90
0.12
1.92
0.14
1.99
0.14
Transpersonal
1.70
0.21
1.83
0.17
1.89
0.18
Scale
Intrapersonal
Interpersonal Transpersonal
Mean
SD
Mean
SD
Mean
SD
Combined*
1.79
0.18
1.95
0.14
1.84
0.19
Prospective
1.71
0.16
1.90
0.12
1.70
0.21
Inter >Trans (<0.0005)
Beginner
1.78
0.18
1.92
0.14
1.83
0.17
Inter >Intra (<0.0005)
Inter >Trans (<0.001)
Advanced
1.83
0.19
1.99
0.14
1.89
0.18
Inter >Intra (<0.0005)
Inter >Trans (<0.0005)
Trans>Intra (0.007)
A>P (0.007)
A>P (0.0001)
B>P (0.004)
Bonferroni
F(2,312)
=60.97
*p < 0.00 1
The Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 2001, Vol. 33, No. I
tests
Tukye
HSD
Norris, 1982), involving an increment in locus of control (Astin, 1997; Hjelle,
1974), inner directedness (Hjelle, 1974; Nidich et al., 1973; Seeman et al., 1972),
and self-determination (Penner et al., 1974); and positive development of people's
self-concept definition at the interpersonal level (Nystul & Garde, 1977; Turnbull &
Norris, 1982), involving increments in altruism (Penner et al., 1974), ego distance
(Pelletier, 1974), intimacy capacity (Hjelle, 1974; Nidich et al., 1973; Seeman et al.,
1972), empathy (Lesh, 1970), and positive psychosocial attitudes (Hanley & Spates,
1978), as well as a decrement in hostility (Abrams & Siegel, 1978). More importantly, our results extend the findings of these authors to the transpersonal domain,
as incorporated in the sophisticated theoretical model of the self-concept developed
by Cloninger et al. (1993), building on the limited research in this area which indicates positive development of people's self-concept definition at the transpersonal
level (Nystul & Garde, 1977), involving an increment in self-actualization (Hjelle,
1974; Nidich et al., 1973; Seeman et al, 1972), with increased self-transcendental
experiences (Alexander et al., 1991), as a correlate of the practice of meditation.
Moreover, the current investigation implemented methodological improvements
over most previous studies by employing a sizeable, representative sample in a natural environment and by attempting to reduce demand characteristics and minimize
group specificity bias. Construct validity could have been further enhanced by
FIGURE 1
M E AN
ARCSIN SCORES O N THE INTRAPERSONAL , INTERPERSONAL , A N D TRANSPERSONAL
SUBSCALES OF THE TCI FOR PROSPECTIVE, BEGINNER, AND ADVANCED MEDITATORS
-0-
lntrapersonal
Interpersonal
Transpersonal
I
I
Prospective
46
I
I
Beginner
The Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 2001, Vol. 33, No. 1
Advanced
inclusion of different tests measuring similar constructs, such as the Self
Expansiveness Level Form (Friedman, 1983), Ego Grasping Orientation questionnaire (Knoblauch & Falconer, 1986) or East West Questionnaire (Gilgen & Cho,
1979). No comparison was made between different methods of meditation, nor was
the possible effect of frequency of meditation practice explored. However, the nondifferentiation of meditative methods is not as substantive as it might first appear,
as discussed below.
At first glance, and assuming the validity of a cross-sectional design, it might be
concluded that Buddhist meditation is an effective tool for developing self-control
with respect to one's life goals, social skills, and increased freedom from self-orientation. Furthermore, within Cloninger et al.'s (1993) framework, meditation
could be suggested as an aid to character formation in particular and personality
development in general, as changes in character also affect the other basic personality component, temperament. Before accepting such a causal hypothesis, potential
problems with the study need to be addressed which may affect the interpretation of
the results.
Firstly, reliance on questionnaires as a measuring instrument raises the possibility
of false self-reports resulting from participants' social desirability. However, the
different performance of the two groups of active meditators appears to counter
such criticism (Alexander et al., 1991). This difference also suggests that the
attempt to conceal the true purpose of the investigation was successful.
Furthermore, Foulds and Warehime (1971) have demonstrated that it is not easy for
meditators to fake " good responses on personality tests.
Secondly, there is the issue of response rate, which was 53%. Hence, the people
who actually returned the questionnaires may represent only a specific subpopulation of the people willing to initiate Buddhist meditative practices, differing in crucial aspects that brought about the displayed performance. However, the response
rates appeared to be fairly equal across the three groups, the number of questionnaires returned within each sample reflecting the representation of the relevant participants within each Buddhist center. The fact that participants were not required to
answer and return the questionnaires immediately (since meetings at the centers
were often late in the evening and the questionnaire took about 30 minutes to complete) resulted in variable conditions in which participants completed the questionnaires, possibly introducing additional sources of bias. However, this was considered preferable and more likely to give valid results than requiring immediate completion.
The Effect of Meditation Practice vs.
the Effect of Beliefs, Expectations, and Values
It was not possible in this study to investigate religious participation and its effects
on self-perception, interpersonal interactions, and values, particularly relationship
to the transcendent. There is undoubtedly a complex interaction between practice,
doctrine, beliefs, values, and behaviors. Religiosity has been shown to relate to par-
The Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 2001, Vol. 33, No. 1
47
ticipants' meditative experience (Shapiro, 1992). Religious participation and meditation are often confounded; future work should be directed toward disentangling
these factors. It is possible that this was compounded in the present study by the
removal of certain items from the TCI at the suggestion of spiritual leaders on
whose cooperation data collection depended. Although we have no particular reason to believe this was the case, it is possible that this affected our results.
Despite the attempt to control for group specificity bias through the inclusion of
prospective meditators, this confounding factor was not completely eliminated. This
is due to an inescapable obstacle within meditation research termed the attrition
problem (Shapiro, 1987), which refers to the probability that continuing as well as
quitting the practice of meditation is linked to specific personality make-ups.
Dropouts have been identified as more psychologically disturbed and less critical
(Smith, 1978), as well as more neurotic (West, 1980). With the high dropout rates
noted in initiators of meditation (Smith, 1976; West, 1980), this constitutes a major
problem for valid scientific investigations in this area.
Likewise, it was impossible to eliminate demand characteristics, despite the crosssectional design. Again, the reason is not a difficulty specific to the current study,
but rather a common problem in this research area, namely participants' selfimposed expectations. If meditation turns out to be ineffective, the individual may
give falsified reports based on attitudes formed to relieve the cognitive dissonance,
arising from the discrepancy between personal expectations and personal experience and the frustration due to the fruitless time investment (Farthing, 1992).
Evidence for the importance of expectations has been obtained by Smith (1976),
who established that belief in effectiveness is a necessary condition for actual effectiveness of meditation. This is a special case of the placebo effect, where expectations are sufficient to bring about the respective outcome, which sceptics similarly
raise against claims of meditation practice's efficacy (Shapiro, 1987).
These considerations appear even more problematic in the light of the findings of
Shapiro (1992). He identified significant relationships between different effects of
meditation and changing effect-expectations across time. In particular, it was
demonstrated that meditators move, during the course of their meditative training,
from the initial aim of self-regulation, across the intermediate aim of self-exploration, to the final aim of self-liberation and compassionate service. Hence, rather
than increased skills in the practice of the method, change in expectations might be
the primary cause of development in the different levels of self-concept. But what
promotes this change in expectations?
If it is important that meditators believe in meditation effects to experience these
effects, the way of teaching, or should one say the degree of persuasion, is clearly
as crucial as the student's expectations. Therefore, one might rather speak of "guru"
effects instead of meditation effects (Farthing, 1992). The current study failed to
control for this confounding variable. However, such influences cannot explain the
direction of progress in expectations.
48
The Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 2001. Vol. 33. No. I
Progressive Shifts in Meditative Projects
A possible interpretation is as follows. If the Buddhist ideology appeals, the practitioner starts with what appears most important from a subjective viewpoint: selfregulation. This is the first meditative project, clearly related to the intrapersonal
self-concept domain. The higher one's sense of control, the greater the prospect of
engaging in the second meditative project: self-exploration. But how can the self be
explored without any relation to other objects? Only through analysis of our
responses to what exists around us can we know our selves (Gazzaniga & LeDoux,
1978). Hence, through this process we also need to know other selves, certainly relevant to the interpersonal self-concept level. The greater the capacity to regulate and
understand our selves, the greater the space to start the final meditative project related to one's transpersonal self-concept: self-liberation and compassionate service.
These hypotheses could be empirically investigated by exploring the relationships
between the individual self-concept dimensions.
Given the influence of the individual's personality, expectations, and received
teachings, the necessary ingredients for generating this complex process may lie primarily within the student and the instructor as well as within their interaction, rather
than within the meditation method as such. This view matches Delmonte's (1987)
conclusion:
Meditation is a self-directed and active process in which a technique is used by a person
(and not on a person) in the context of particular subjective expectations and objectives.
For this reason meditation may not be readily dispensed, like medication, to anxious or
depressed patients if they show little motivation to practice. The value of meditation may
be greater for those who wish to be involved in directing their own development than for
those wanting to be "cured" passively. (p. 132)
It also sets in perspective Ken Wilber's previously quoted claim and demand for
humans' active engagement in the processes of evolution through true contemplative practice.
Wilber (1983) also concurs with the view that it does not matter what methods are
applied for contemplation so long as the result is true contemplation. What is important is the raising of awareness of the individual self and, thus, to all that exists in
relation to it. Different individuals may need different strategies taught by different
instructors. These strategies could be different forms of psychotherapy or meditation or a combination of such methods. This hypothesis also makes sense of the socalled equivalent paradox, a source of great headaches for many dedicated
researchers. This refers to the fact that so far it has proven impossible to distinguish
the outcome of different types of meditation, as well as of different types of psychotherapy, according to their respective contents (Shapiro, 1987).
However, when comparing psychotherapy and meditation, the latter may appear
favorable as a method for two reasons: its simplicity and its dualistic stance on problems of human existence, emphasizing the training of both physical and mental
awareness. Meditation may therefore be particularly useful as a secular tool for self-
The Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 2001, Vol. 33, No. I
regulation, self-exploration, and self-liberation, best taught at an early age, when
cognitive and personality factors are not fully developed and hence do not represent
an obstacle to experiencing the method's usefulness. This is an issue for future
research.
Finally, the following directions for future research aimed at the analysis of meditation effects are suggested. Studies should focus on the systematic comparison of
different people, distinguished particularly in terms of age, personality, and cognitive factors, in relation to different settings, investigated with multiple quantitative
as well as qualitative assessment techniques, using randomized (in the case of truly
experimental designs) as well as even-numbered and large enough samples, with
cross-sectional as well as longitudinal analyses, exploring issues related to psychological well-being that have been suggested to be significantly affected by the practice of meditation, such as the intrapersonal, interpersonal, and transpersonal dimensions of the self-concept. Looking at this latter theoretical construct, the relationship
between the different domains of self-conceptualizations as well as the effect of the
newly gained self-concept on the temperament factors would also be issues worth
exploring.
Unless such future empirical investigation unequivocally pinpoints the causal function of the different factors involved within the process of meditation that affect the
self, researchers should be particularly carefu1about drawing conclusions and making recommendations like the one given by Hjelle (1974) in the context of his study:
"An added benefit of the therapeutic application of Transcendental Meditation is
that its effects are presumed to come about rapidly and do not depend on new beliefs
or altered styles of living" (p. 627). It would be a truly paradisiacal prospect if identifying the catalyst for individuals' personal self-development would be that
straightforward within the multicomplexity of contemporary human life.
Abrams, A. I., & Siegel, L. M. (1978). The transcendental meditation program and rehabilitation at Folsom State Prison: A cross-validation study. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 5 ,
3-20.
Alexander, C. N.. Rainforth, M. V., & Gelderloos, P. (1991). Transcendental meditation, selfactualisation, psychological health: a conceptual overview and statistical metaanalysis.
Journal of Social Behaviour and Personality, 6, 189-247.
Astin, J. (1997). Stress-reduction through mindfulness meditation: Effects on psychological
symptomatology, sense of control, and spiritual experience. Psychotherapy and
Psychosomatics, 66.97- 106.
Bergin, A. E. (1991). Values and religious issues in psychotherapy and mental health.
American Psychologist, 46, 394-403.
Canington, P., & Ephron, H. (1975). Meditation as an adjunct to psychotherapy. In S. Arieti
& G. Chrzanowski (Eds.), The world biennial of psychotherapy and psychiatry. Near
York: Wiley.
Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, D. M., & Przybeck, T. R. (1993). A psychobiological model of
temperament and character. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 975-999.
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Four ways five factors are basic. Personaliiy and
Individual Differences, 13, 653-665.
50
The Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 2001, Vol. 33, No. I
Delmonte, M. (1987). Personality and meditation. In M. West (Ed.), The psychology of meditation (pp. 1 18- 132). Oxford: Clarendon.
Farthing, G. W. (1992). The psychology of consciousness. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice
Hall.
Foulds, M. L., & Warehime, R. G. (1971). Effects of a "fake g o o d response set on a measure of self-actualization. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 18, 279-280.
Friedman, H. L. (1983). The Self-Expansiveness Level Form: a conceptualization and measurement of a transpersonal construct. Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 15,37-50.
Gazzaniga, M . S., & LeDoux, J. E. (1978). The integrated mind. New York: Plenum Press.
Gilgen, A. R.. & Cho, J. H. (1979). Questionnaire to measure Eastern and Western thought.
Psychological Reports, 44. 835-841.
Goleman, D. (1988). The meditative mind. Los Angeles: Jeremy P. Tarcher. .
Hanley, C. P., & Spates, J. L. (1978). Transcendental meditation and social psychological
attitudes. Journal of Psychology, 99, 12 1- 127.
Hjelle, L. A. (1974). Transcendental meditation and psychological health. Perceptual and
Motor Skills, 39,623-628.
Kabat-Zinn, J., Lipworth, L., Bumey, R., & Sellers, W. (1986). Four year follow up of a meditation-based program for the self-regulation of chronic pain. Clinical Journal of Chronic
Pain, 2, 150-173.
Knoblauch, D. L., & Falconer, J. A. (1986). The relationship of a measured Taoist orientation to Western personality dimensions. Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 18,73-83.
Krishnamurti Foundation. (1996). Questioning Krishnamurti: J. Krishnamurti in dialogue.
London: Thorsons.
Lesh, T. V. (1970). Practice of Zazen increased empathy in student counselors. Journal of
Humanistic Psychology, 10, 37-74.
MacDonald, D: A., Tsagarakis, C. I., & Holland, C. J. (1994). Validation of a measure of
transpersonal self-concept and its relationship to Jungian and five-factor model conceptions of personality. Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 26, 175-201.
MacDonald, A. D., LeClair, L., Holland, C. J., Alter, A., & Friedman, H. L. (1995). A survey of measures of transpersonal constructs. Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 27,
171-235.
Nidich, S., Seeman, W., & Dreskin, T. (1973). Influence of transcendental meditation: A
replication. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 20, 565-566.
Nystul, M. S., & Garde, M. (1977). Comparison of self-concepts of transcendental meditators and nonmeditators. Psychological Reports, 41, 303-306.
Pelletier, K. R. (1974). Influence of transcendental meditation upon autokinetic perception.
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 39, 103 1- 1034.
Penner, W. J., Zingle, H. W., Dyck, R., & Truch, S. (1974). Does an in-depth transcendental
meditation course effect change in personalities of participants? Western Psychologist, 4,
104-111.
Schuster, R. (1979). Empathy and mindfulness. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 1.71-77.
Seeman, W., Nidich, S., & Banta, S. (1972). Influence of transcendental meditation on a
measure of self-actualization. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 19, 184- 187.
Shapiro, D. A. (1987). Implications of psychotherapy research for the study of meditation. In
M. West (ed.), The psychology of meditation. Oxford: Clarendon.
Shapiro, D. H. (1982). Overview: Clinical and physiological comparison of meditation with
other self-control strategies. American Journal of Psychiatry, 139, 267-274.
Shapiro, D. H. (1992). A mode of control and self-control profile of long-term meditators.
Psychologia, 35, 1 - 11.
Shostrom, E. L.(1964). An inventory for the measurement of self-actualization. Educational
and Psychological Measurement, 24(2), 207-2 18.
Smith, J. C. (1976). Psychotherapeutic effects of transcendental meditation with controls for
The Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 2001, Vol. 33, No. I
51
expectation of relief and daily sitting. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 44,
630-637.
Smith, J. C. (1978). Personality correlates of continuation and outcome in meditation and
erect sitting control treatments. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46, 272279.
Suzuki, D. T. (1960). Essays in Zen Buddhism. In D. T. Suzuki, E. Fromm, & R. de Martino
(Eds.), Zen Buddhism and psychoanalysis. New York: Harper & Row.
Turnbull, M. J., & Norris, H. (1982). Effects of transcendental meditation on self-identity
indices and personality. British Journal of Psychology, 73, 57-68.
Walsh, R. N. (1983). Meditation practice and research. Journal of Humanistic Psychology,
23, 18-50.
West, M. A. (1980). Meditation, personality and arousal. Personality and lndividutrl
Differences, I , 135-142.
Wilber, K. (1983). Up from Eden: a transpersonal view of human evolution. London:
Routledge & Kegan Paul.
The Authors
Charlotte J. Haimerl, B.Sc., graduated with a first class degree from Royal Holloway,
University of London, and is currently completing her M.Sc. in science, culture, and communication at the University of Bath.
Elizabeth R. Valentine, B.A., Ph.D., is a Reader in psychology at Royal Holloway,
University of London, and a member of the Consciousness and Experiential Psychology
and Transpersonal Psychology sections of the British Psychological Society. She is the
author of nearly 100 publications in theoretical and experimental psychology, including
Conceptual Issues in Psychology (Routledge, 1992). She is a coeditor of Phenomenology
and Cognitive Science: Handbook (Josef Roell Verlag, 1997). Her research interests
include philosophical psychology, memory, and music performance.
52
The Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 2001, Vol. 33, No. I