* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Example Critical Review Paper
Survey
Document related concepts
Transcript
Running head: MOVEMENT IN INTELLIGENCE 1 Shouldn’t There Be Some Movement in the Intelligence Movement? Dwight Hennessy York University Excerpts from the Minor Area paper submitted to the Graduate Faculty in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Graduate Program in Psychology York University For example purposes in PSY450 only. Any other use for academic or other purposes is prohibited and a violation of copyright laws. Shouldn’t There Be Some Movement in the Intelligence Movement ? Running head: MOVEMENT IN INTELLIGENCE 2 The intelligence movement, which began around the turn of the century, was based on a few basic principles: that intelligence is inborn and unalterable, that it can be represented by a single number, that it is a defining factor in overall personality characteristics, and that there are basic group and/or racial differences (Fancher, 1985). In recent years the intelligence movement has reemerged in psychological and popular literature. However, since the word "movement" means to progress or change, and the latest version of the intelligence movement does neither, it represents more of an intelligence "stagnation". Despite saying nothing new, the popularity of the latest intelligence concepts is based on the same political and social conscience that facilitated the original ideas: if negative characteristics are biologically rather than socially determined, then the time, effort, money, and responsibility for improvement is on deviant individuals rather than on society itself. Within this framework, the present paper will follow the movement (or stagnation) and its main criticisms which advocate more environmental influences in intelligence. The Beginning--Eugenics Galton (1869) proposed that the poor were inherently less intelligent than the well off and that social positions were unchangeable. Low intelligence caused people to be lazy and unmotivated which led to poverty and sickness. Galton felt that the number of poor would eventually be so enormous that the fittest and superior breed of humans would disappear. Thus his eugenic solution was to selectively breed those who possessed the best characteristics and prevent those who were unfit from procreating (Buss, 1976). He believed that the root of intelligence was in the brain and, since the brain controlled the body, intelligence could be measured through body functions such as the senses and reaction times. He devised a complex series of tests specifically to measure sensory acuity and reaction times in order to identify those who should procreate and those who should not (Fancher, 1985). Unfortunately for him he could find no relationships between his measures and actual success in life. Not all were as impressed with eugenics as Galton. Mill firmly believed that intelligence was more a product of good upbringing and quality education than heredity (Mill, 1971). The antieugenic explanation was that the environment was shared or passed on between generations. It was becoming well known that the filthy, disease ridden conditions of the poor in England was a major reason they accomplished little in life (Chase, 1980). The poor were more likely to developed eye Running head: MOVEMENT IN INTELLIGENCE 3 and ear problems and lacked the monetary or the medical resources to correct such problems. Despite evidence that proper medical care, better diet, adequate sewage, and a good education could improve the dilemma of the poor, eugenic ideas were better suited to the powerful and biological determinism of intelligence was advocated (Chase, 1980). The World War I Era One of the earliest American advocates of the eugenic conception of intelligence was Goddard, who traced the lineage of Martin Kalikak down through two different paths: from a child born to his mistress (a simple bar maid) and from the children born to his Quaker wife (Goddard, 1912). The descendants of the bar maid were found to be unintelligent, deviant, criminal, impoverished individuals and the descendants of the quaker wife to be intelligent, law abiding, somewhat successful individuals. He concluded that intelligence must be inherited and unchangeable. In this respect, the majority of social ills in America could be remedied simply by preventing the unintelligent from reproducing. To further his point Goddard measured the intelligence of new immigrants entering America on Ellis Island. He found that 87% of Russians, 83% of Jews, 80% of Hungarians, and 79% of Italians immigrants were feeble-minded or "morons" (Goddard, 1917). Not only was intelligence biologically determined but there were clear national and racial group differences. The idea of inborn group differences in intelligence was advanced by the publication of the Army Intelligence data following World War I. During the war, Yerkes, then the president of APA and an advocate of Galton's biologically determined intelligence, gathered together a group of like minded psychologists, including Goddard and Terman, to develop an intelligence test to be used by the military (Kamin, 1974). Terman had recently become known for his American revision of the Binet-Simon test, the Stanford-Binet, which included American items and age related standards (Terman, 1920). Each score was converted to an Intelligence Quotient (Stern, 1914) which provided a single number indicating a child's intelligence for his/her age. This fit well with the growing idea that there was a single inborn general intelligence (called "g") that could be accurately measured and represented as a single score (Spearman, 1904). They proclaimed to the military and the media that recruits could be accurately ranked based on intelligence scores and could be placed in the most appropriate jobs to help end the war early (Kevles, 1968). Running head: MOVEMENT IN INTELLIGENCE 4 Two Army Intelligence tests were constructed: the Alpha and Beta tests. The Alpha test was designed for those recruits who could read, write, and understand English while the Beta test was designed for those who could not (Yerkes, 1921). Both provided a single score indicating mental age (i.e. intelligence). They were believed to measure natural inborn intelligence and not school learning or cultural influences (Samelson, 1977), therefore, training those with low scores in certain areas was a considered a waste of time and money. The most impressive aspect, and the intended "selling point", at that time was that a great deal of time and money could be saved since large numbers of recruits could be evaluated at once. This began the era of group testing in psychology. According to the army test data, the average mental age of the American adult was determined to be around 13 years (Yerkes, 1921). Immigrants from norther Europe were slightly lower while those from southern and eastern Europe were below 12 years (the upper limit of the "moron" classification). The least intelligent group was the American blacks with an average mental age of 10 years. This was startling news since the average adult mental age had been estimated, prior to the implementation of the tests, to be 16 years (Brigham, 1923). The suggested solution was to prevent the black and inferior European blood from contaminating the Nordic American breed. This data is believed to have contributed to the subsequent implementation of the 1924 Immigration Act which limited the number of immigrants from specific "low intelligence" countries (Kamin, 1974). There were some who doubted the validity and usefulness of the Army tests (Samelson, 1977). The use of a single number to represent intelligence was seen as too simple (McLeish, 1963). Advocates of biological determinism had greater faith in the concept of a single intelligence than did its opponents. Thurstone demonstrated that by simply rotating the factor dimensions in a factor analysis, multiple intelligences emerged (Gould, 1994). The validity of a concept that can be changed simply by modifying its mathematical construction is highly suspect. The idea of multiple intelligences is the position most accepted today. Brigham (1930) later believed that comparisons between groups were not proper since different racial groups had different life experiences, history, and education. The fact the tests were developed based on "Nordic" culture, gave certain groups a distinct advantage. For example, on the Alpha test one question required participants to know the Buick was made in Flint, while on the Running head: MOVEMENT IN INTELLIGENCE 5 Beta test one question required participants to know that a tennis court contained a net (Greene, 1941). Individuals not familiar with middle class American culture (including many blacks) would have had a disadvantage since such questions were based on knowledge of culture rather than innate "intelligence". Similarly, immigrants who had been in America longer would generally score higher due to a greater familiarity with the language and culture (Gould, 1981). The superiority of the Nordic blood is easily explained in that whites (including most Northern Europeans) were more likely to be middle class, attend good schools, and speak English. Knowledge of numbers, writing skills, and standardized school curriculum gave a clear advantage to those who had attended westernized schools (Greene, 1941). For those not familiar with testing situations, the simple experience of being in a crowed room performing a novel task as military instructors shouted instructions must have been very anxiety provoking; particularly those taking the Beta test where the instructions were given in pantomime. Recent immigrants and the poor often did not have the scholastic experience to do well in these tests. American blacks, for instance, could not attend higher level schools and were more often represented in the poor so their scores were very low (Samelson, 1977). When individual tests were conducted on those who scored extremely low, taking into account the entire individual and testing context, they exhibited much higher intelligence than their test scores indicated (Yerkes, 1921). The Civil Rights Movement and “Jensenism” The intelligence movement lost a great deal of momentum following World War II due to the public trepidation over the recent Nazi atrocities committed in the name of individual and group superiority (Chase, 1980). The late 1950s began a more liberal political and public era where individual differences and societal problems were seen more as a function of upbringing and environmental conditions than natural group inferiority. Programs were proposed that would eliminate the social and economic differences in America. Schools were desegregated and blacks were allowed to enter schools and programs from which they were once prohibited. There were some who were opposed to these changes because they were costly. By the mid to late 1960s, these programs were beginning into full operation and coupled with the escalating Vietnam War which was costing billions of dollars per year, the burden on the tax payers was enormous (Chase, 1980). The ideas of personal rather than social responsibility for a person's place in life Running head: MOVEMENT IN INTELLIGENCE 6 began to spring up once again. In 1969, the Harvard Educational Review, in an attempt to start a debate on the I.Q. issue, solicited an article from Jensen refuting environmental explanations of intelligence. Having been a recent convert to the biological determination of intelligence proposed by Galton and Goddard (among others), his article stated very clearly that intelligence was decreasing in America, that some groups were born less intelligent, and that one's intelligence level was not changeable (Jensen, 1969). Although the poor were low in intelligence it was the blacks who were the least intelligent and, as a result, social programs such as Head Start and compensatory education had failed and would continue to fail because they had no possible chance of success. His solution was that the less intelligent should be trained in more appropriate tasks while the more intelligent were trained in tasks requiring complex thinking. The public were outraged at his racially charged propositions. Jensen had ignored the available medical data on the children enrolled in the Head Start program which showed that many had illnesses, eye and ear problems, or mental or physical disabilities, all of which could have easily hindered learning. Rich as well as poor children with uncorrected ear and eye problems were found to perform less well in school (Chase, 1980). Similarly, most children in the programs were poor and\or malnourished. Lowe (1969) found that in America, 29% of children lived in poverty and 33% of all children under the age of six were malnourished (cited in Chase, 1980). Since learning is a process influenced by development (which is influenced by diet and nutrition) and environment, such problems early in childhood could easily have had an effect on learning ability regardless of "intelligence level" (Hunt, 1969). There were also problems within the programs themselves that had little to do with the children or their race. Since Head Start was a new concept, no one really knew how to implement or evaluate it properly. Even more damning was the fact that some school boards did not want to implement Head Start and, as a result, purposely sabotaged the program (Cronbach, 1969). Even in those programs that were earnestly undertaken, the solution was a "mind fix" rather than an "environment fix" or "body fix" (Prilleltensky, 1990). The effects of poverty, malnourishment, and a history of racism and segregation on scholastic success were not being confronted and still exerted a great deal of influence on "intelligence". Finally, Jensen Running head: MOVEMENT IN INTELLIGENCE 7 condemned all compensatory education based solely on eight weeks of Head Start data. Other compensatory education programs had already been shown to be successful in increasing "intelligence" scores and decreasing the student dropout rate by 63% (Cronbach, 1969). The quality of Jensen's intellectual sources were also questioned. Along with the eugenic minded ideas of Galton, Goddard, Brigham and others from the World War I era, he relied heavily on Burt's finding that intelligence is 80% heritable. Burt believed that monozygotic (MZ) twins were genetically equal so any similarities in their I.Q. scores should be a direct indication of biological influences on intelligence. Over the course of many years, he complied a sample of MZ twins reared separately and found that the correlation between their I.Q. scores was approximately 0.80 and concluded that 80% of intelligence in heritable (Burt, 1966). Burt has been discredited on many accounts including the fact that he doctored or fabricated some of his data. For example, some of his correlations were found to be the same to three decimal places even after the number of twin pairs rose from 26 to 58 when one would expect at least a .001 difference (Hearnshaw, 1979; Kamin, 1977). He provided no records on many of the twin pairs, and the raw data or descriptions of his tests were not presented publicly (Hearnshaw, 1979). In a similar manner, the “corrections” to the scores were never clearly explained (Kamin, 1977). Other environmental factors were never considered such as the fact that the twin pairs had met prior to being studied, some several years earlier, and so may have had a chance to notice and comment on similarities (Farber, 1981). Similarly, MZ twins look alike and are the same age which may have resulted in them being treated somewhat similarly even when separated. Kamin (1981) has found that even DZ twins who look very similar have heritability correlations similar to MZ twins. Finally, twins do not constitute a representative sample of the population, especially those reared apart. Adoption is not random since children tend to be placed in homes of the more well off and educated (although Burt (1966) insisted the adoptions in his sample were truly random). Despite all of these flaws (in fairness, some of which Jensen had not yet known) Burt's 80% heritability was still used. The Modern Era Although Jensen's arguments were not new, they were presented in a social and political climate in which they could not be publicly tolerated. These very same ideas have been Running head: MOVEMENT IN INTELLIGENCE 8 presented in recent years, within a more right wing climate conducive to person rather than social responsibility, and have been more widely accepted. The data may be more extensive and complex but the basic ideas are still based on biological determinism and person rather than social responsibility for change. According to Rushton, the propensity of some individuals to be, among other things, low in intelligence is a product of the evolution of races (Rushton, 1988). The environment in which the mongoloid race evolved was scarce in resources, so they had to be more intelligent, have fewer children, and invest much more time and effort into their survival if the race was to survive. Today, as a result, the mongoloid race possesses more "K" traits such as high intelligence, strong parental skills, and group consciousness. The evolutionary environment of the blacks, on the other hand, was resource rich so high intelligence and parental investment were not necessary. Today, as a result, the negroid race possess more "r" traits such as low intelligence, weak parental ties (i.e. they have many children and pay little attention to them), and low motivation. The white race is somewhere between the two, higher in "K" than "r" traits. This has led to stable low intelligence in most blacks and some deviant whites. To support this theory, Rushton relies on racial differences in penis length which he believes is an indication of sexual prowess and parental investment (which is inversely related to intelligence). Those with large penises procreate often and care little for their offsprings. He has reported that blacks have the largest average penis length followed by whites and mongoloids (Rushton & Bogaert, 1987). He has also claimed that cranial capacity (adjusted for height), which he believes provides an indication of brain size and intelligence, is largest for mongoloids followed by whites and blacks. The r/K theory is viewed as outdated and overstated even in the field of ecology in which it was developed. It was never intended to be used for long term analysis or to be taken outside of the short term historical and environmental context in which the behaviours occurred (Anderson, 1991). Since Rushton could not possibly know the true environment of prehistoric people, he could not know their evolutionary adaptation or their genetic variation. There is no way for anyone to know if prehistoric races remained stationary or migrated, thus, Rushton has no proof that the mongoloids evolved completely in arctic climates while blacks evolved completely in the central tropics (Anderson, 1991). Also, his concept of race is rather narrow. Running head: MOVEMENT IN INTELLIGENCE 9 Basing racial boundaries on skin colour does not take into account that skin colour varies considerably and can change over generations. Some have proposed many more than three races while others have argued that the concept of race no longer exists (Wright, 1994). More genetic variability can be found within than between racial groups (Weizmann, Wiener, Wiesenthal, & Ziegler, 1991). A more appropriate approach would have been to separate the three races into more homogeneous races. Questions have been raised concerning the quality of his intellectual sources on penis lenght differences. Weizmann et al., (1991) point out that Rushton and Bogaert (1987) cite Noble (1982) who cited a journal on penis length which turned out to be the Penthouse Forum. His principle source of evidence concerning penis length is the French Army Surgeon, an anonymous book written in the 19th century when mysteries about blacks were rampant. The book amounts to nothing more than anthropologic pornography (Davis & Whitten, 1987). Rushton and Bogaert (1987) claim that Arab men are a mixture of white and black races and have larger penises than white men but smaller than black men. However, they have no proof that Arab men are a product of this racial mixture (Weizmann et al., 1991). They also insist that penis length, sexuality, procreative ability or propensity, and parental style, are all inversely related to intelligence. There is no proof that intelligent people attempt to procreate less often and/or make innately better parents. In his work on cranial size, Rushton's asserts that head size represents brain size (when corrected for by body height) which represents brain weight and intelligence. However, it has been found that head size has no relationship to brain volume and that there is no relationship between body and brain sizes (Reed & Jensen, 1993; Wickett, Vernon, & Lee, 1994). If the body height correction of brain sizes breaks down, then using Rushton's own data, the brain size of whites becomes larger than that of mongoloids (Peters, 1995) which means whites should be more intelligent than mongoloids. Hernstein and Murray (1994) have written arguably the most popular piece of work on the intelligence issue since World War I. Their arguments are not new yet the Bell Curve, despite consisting of 845 pages of charts, graphs, and statistics novel to most lay readers, was the top selling book for many months. Hernstein and Murray's main arguments are that intelligence Running head: MOVEMENT IN INTELLIGENCE 10 in the United States has declined over the past decades (especially among the poor and blacks), that intelligence is inborn and unchangeable, and that social programs designed to increase intelligence are a waste of time and money. The source of the problem is person oriented, thus the solution should be based on those deviant individuals rather than on society. The core arguments are similar to Jensen's (and all who came before) but the data is more extensive and complex. Hernstein's personal bias is clear in his earlier work in which he proposed that the average I.Q. of a nation is a perfect indicator of productivity (Hernstein, 1971). Japan's rise in world economic power can be attributed to the fact that I.Q. is rising in Japan and falling in the United States. To resurrect the United States to dominance once again, the average American intelligence must be increased. Murray and Hernstein's solution, like Jensen's, involves training the less intelligent in menial jobs while the more intelligent improve the quality of life in the United States. The ideas are not much different from those a century ago (Beardsley, 1995). The solutions may vary slightly but the concept remains the same--immobilize the stupid while the intelligent middle class whites repopulate the world. The first criticism of the Bell Curve should come as no surprise. Their main authority on black racial inferiority is Jensen (who was based on Burt) (Lane, 1994). The problems run deeper in both the quality of data and intellectual sources. They have some problems with the current trends in worldwide I.Q. in that if genetic superiority is the reason for higher I.Q., and there is a rise in inferior breeds, then the worldwide I.Q. should not be rising as it is (Beardsley, 1995). Also, if national productivity is dependent solely on I.Q. then those countries with the largest increase should be outperforming previous generations but they do not. The fact is that along with the rise in I.Q. there has been a worldwide rise in years of schooling, quality of education, better diet and medical care, and other environmental factors related to intelligence (Wiener, Weizmann, Wiesenthal, & Ziegler, 1990). Hernstein and Murray's principle authority on the Japanese I.Q. is Lynn who had at one time proclaimed Japanese I.Q. to be around 111 (Lynn, 1982) but re-evaluated it to be around 100 (Lynn, 1987) after it was discovered he used an unrepresentative sample of wealthy urban Japanese children in westernized schools. In fact, more representative samples of Japanese and American children have shown no difference in I.Q. scores (Stevenson & Azuma, 1983). Another of Lynn's Japanese studies was shown to compare Running head: MOVEMENT IN INTELLIGENCE 11 I.Q. scores of Japanese students in 1985 with British students in 1978 and American students in 1972 (Lynn, Hampson, & Iwakawi, 1987). It is not the same to be 15, for example, in 1972 as it is in 1985. Hernstein and Murray do not mention these problems although they soften the difference between American and Japanese I.Q.'s to 3 points but increase it to 5 points in later chapters (Lane, 1994). Lynn is also a key contributor to the idea that blacks have lower I.Q. than whites. Hernstein and Murray state that if I.Q. was influenced by environment rather than genes, then African blacks should score higher than American blacks since African blacks have not had a history of prejudice and racism. Lynn (1991) evaluated 11 African intelligence studies and determined the average I.Q. of African blacks to be around 70-75, which is lower than the score of 84 assigned to American blacks (Kamin, 1995). In Hernstein and Murray's view, this constitutes a genetic explanation of intelligence differences between blacks and whites. Five of the studies Lynn evaluated were conducted in South Africa where the attitude toward blacks has been almost as unfavourable as in America, while most other studies were conducted by those in favour of subjugating blacks in Africa (Kamin, 1995). Many studies also used Raven's Progressive Matrix Test scores which cannot be converted to I.Q. scores (because they are not symmetrical about a mean) although Lynn did just that (Lane, 1994). Lynn also ignored warnings by some original authors like Owen (1989) who stated that the black Africans were not familiar with white culture and language, on which the Progressive Matrix test is based. Hernstein and Murray ignored contradictory studies such as that of Crawford-Nutt (1976) who found that African black students in westernized schools scored higher in progressive matrix scores than American white students. Another major area of problem is with the reported multiple regressions and their concepts of SES. With SES held constant, I.Q. predicted negative behaviours such as criminality, school dropout,and poor job performance. When I.Q. was held constant, no relationships emerged, therefore, they proclaimed I.Q. to be the major cause of social ills. The problem is that they treated their relationships as if they were causal and, as "respected scientists" (Brimelow, 1994), should have know that their statistical method does not allow such statements. They ignore many other studies which show relatioinships between SES or environment and I.Q Running head: MOVEMENT IN INTELLIGENCE 12 (Kamin, 1995). Also, their conceptions of intelligence and SES are very limited. They assume that I.Q. scores represent true intelligence and that intelligence is a single measurable thing (Beardsley, 1995). Such a simple concept is accepted by very few today. When this idea breaks down their whole argument does as well (Gould, 1994). Their divisions of SES are based on job and financial status gathered from self reports which may be questioned in terms of accuracy and truthfulness (Kamin, 1995). The context and history surrounding the reports of job and financial status were not considered. It should not be a surprise that children of poor parents remain poor since the conditions in which lower SES children grow and develop often includes poor diet, poor medical care, low self esteem, and poor education. Many low SES individuals do not have English as a first language which handicaps the parents and the children in learning and advancing socially and economically. It is these factor, not the actual intelligence or SES level of parents that leads to poverty in children (Kamin, 1995). Another major statistical error is that they report the form of the relationships in bright, bold, colourful graphs and charts, yet they only mention the strength of the relationships near the conclusion of the book in an appendix where most would not look (Gould, 1994). The strengths account for only 5% to 10% of the variance (usually closer to 5 than 10). In other words, intelligence (which is only 70% heritable in their view) only accounts for 5% to 10% of the negative behaviours (such as criminality) in the relationships (Gould, 1994). With such weak relationships between their variables, their entire argument must be questioned. Without any strong proof or backing, they are left only with theoretical propaganda. Conclusion The fact that the recent "movement" in intelligence has shown no movement at all should call into question the usefulness of biologically determined intelligence. However, in a political and social climate conducive to individual rather than social responsibility, the concepts of an innate, unchangeable intelligence, which can be used to define all that is wrong with society, is very attractive--regardless of how weak the data or how personally involved the intellectual (i.e. racist) sources of the data may be. The intention of this paper was not to insist that biology plays no part in intelligence, or any other personality characteristic, but that it is only a fraction of the truth. Biology may play a role in determining intelligence but only in combination with a wide Running head: MOVEMENT IN INTELLIGENCE 13 variety of other factors such as diet, medical care, pollution, early childhood experience, education level, and SES to name but a few. It would be easy to conclude this paper calling for more research highlighting the environmental or interactional (person by situation) influences but I feel a more appropriate plea would be to call for an end to the biological exclusiveness found in the intelligence "stagnation". Running head: MOVEMENT IN INTELLIGENCE 14 References Anderson, J. L. (1991). Rushton’s racial comparisons: An ecological critique of theory and method. Canadian Psychology, 32, 51-60. Beardsley, T. (1995). For whom the Bell Curve really tolls. Scientific American, January, 14-18. Binet, A. (1903). Etude Experimentale de l'Intelligence. Paris: Schleicher. Brigham, C. C. (1923). A Study of American Intelligence. Princeton University Press. Brigham, C. C. (1930). Intelligence tests of immigrant groups. Psychological Review, 37, 158-165. Brimelow, P. (1994). For whom the "Bell" tolls. Forbes, October 24, 153-163. Burt, C. (1966). The genetic determination of differences in intelligence: A study of monozy gotic twins reared together and apart. British Journal of Psychology, 57, 137-153. Buss, A. (1976). Galton and the birth of differential psychology and eugenics: Social, political and economic forces. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 12, 47-58. Chase, M. (1980). The Legacy fo Malthus: The Social Costs of the New Scientific Racism. Urbana: University of Illinois Press. Crawford-Nutt. D. (1976). Are black scores on Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrixes an artifact of method of test presentation? Psychologia Africana, 16, 201-206. Cronbach, J. L. (1968). Heredity, environment, and educational policy. Harvard Educational Review, 39, 338-347. Davis, D. L., & Whitten, R. G. (1987). The cross-cultural study of human sexuality. Annual Review of Anthropology, 16, 69-98. Fancher, R. E. (1985). The Intelligence Men: Makers of the I.Q Controversy. New York: Norton. Farber, S. L. (1981). Identical Twins Reared Apart: A Reanalysis. New York: Basic Books. Galton, F. (1869). Hereditary Genius. London: MacMillan. Goddard, H. H. (1912). The Kalikak Family: A Study in the Heredity of Feeble-Mindedness. New York: Macmillan. Goddard, H. H. (1917). Mental tests and the immigrant. Journal of Delinquency, 2, 243-277. Running head: MOVEMENT IN INTELLIGENCE 15 Gould, S. J. (1981). Mismeasure of Man. New York: Norton. Gould, S. J. (1994). The curveball. New Yorker, November 28, 139-149. Greene, E. B. (1941). Measurements of Human Behavior. New York: Oddysey. Hearnshaw, L. S. (1979). Cyril Burt: Psychologist. New York: Cornell University Press. Hernstein, R. J. (1971). IQ. Atlantic Monthly, 228, 43-64. Hernstein, R. J., & Murray, C. (1994). The Bell Curve. New York: Free Press. Hunt, J. McV. (1969). Has compensatory education failed? Has it been attempted? Harvard Educational Review, 39, 278-300. Jensen, A. R. (1968). How much can we boost IQ and scholastic achievement? Harvard Educational Review, 39, 1-123. Kamin, L. J. (1974). The science and politics of I.Q. Social Research, 41, 387-425. Kamin, L. J. (1977). The Science and Politics of I.Q. Harmondswoth, England: Penguin Books. Kamin, L. J. (1981). The Intelligence Controversy. New York: Wiley. Kamin, L. J. (1995). Behind the Bell Curve. Scientific American, February, 99-103. Kevles, D. J. (1968). Testing the Army's intelligence: Psychologists and the military in World War I. Journal of American History, 55, 565-581. Lane, C. (1994). The tainted sources of "The Bell Curve". The New York Review, December 1, 249-287. Lynn, R. (1982). I.Q. in Japan and the United States shows a growing disparity. Nature, 297, 222-223. Lynn, R. (1987). The intelligence of the mongoloids: A psychometric, evolutionary and neurological theory. Personality and Individual Differences, 8, 813-814. Lynn, R. (1991). Race differences in intelligence: A global perspective. Mankind Quarterly, 31, 255-272. Lynn, R., Hampson, S. L., & Iwakawi, S. (1987). Abstract reasoning and spatial abilities among American, British and Japanese adolescents. Mankind Quarterly, 27, 379-402. McLeish, J. (1963). The Science of Behaviour. London: Barrie and Rockliffe. Mill, J. S. (1971). Autobiography. London: Oxford University Press. Owen, K. (1989). Test and Item Bias: The Use of the Junior Aptitude Test as a Common Test Running head: MOVEMENT IN INTELLIGENCE 16 Battery for White, Indian, and Black pupils in Standard 7. Pretoria: Human Science Research Council. Peters, M. (1995). Race differences in brain size. American Psychologist, 50, 947-948. Prilleltensky, I. (1990). On the social and political implications of cognitive psychology. Journal of Mind and Behavior, 11, 127-136. Reed, T. E. & Jensen, A. R. (1993). Cranial capacity: New caucasian data and comments on Rushton's claimed Mongoloid-Caucasoid brain size differences. Intelligence, 17, 423-431. Rushton, J. P., & Bogaert, A. F. (1987). Race differences in sexual behaviour: Testing an evolutionary hypothesis. Journal of Research in Personality, 21, 529-551. Samelson, F. (1977). World War I intelligence testing and the development of psychology. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 13, 274-282. Spearman, C. (1904). 'General Intelligence' objectively determined and measured. American Journal of Psychology, 15, 201-293. Stern, W. (1914). The Psychological Methods of Intelligence Testing. Baltimore: Warwick and York. Stevenson, H. & Azuma, H. (1983). I.Q. in Japan and the United States. Nature, 17, 291-292. Terman, L. (1920). Condensed Guide for the Stanford Revision of the Binet-Simon Intelligence Tests. New York: Houghton-Miffin. Weizmann, F., Wiener, N. I., Wiesenthal, D. L., & Ziegler, M. (1991). Eggs, eggplants, and eggheads: A rejoinder to Rushton. Canadian Psychology, 32, 43-50. Wickett, J. C., Vernon, P. A., & Lee, D. H. (1994). In vivo brain size, head perimeter, and intelligence in a sample of healthy adult females. Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 831-838. Wiener, N. I., Weizmann, F., Wiesenthal, D. L., & Ziegler, M. (1990). I.Q., economic productivity and eugenics. International Journal of Dynamic Assessment and Instruction, 1, 105-115. Wright, L. (1994). One drop of blood. The New Yorker, July 25, 46-55. Yerkes, R. M. (1921). Psychological examining in the United States Army. Memoirs of the Running head: MOVEMENT IN INTELLIGENCE National Academy of Sciences, 15, 1-890. 17