Download Effects of medicinal drugs on actual and simulated driving

Document related concepts

Start School Later movement wikipedia , lookup

Prenatal testing wikipedia , lookup

Harm reduction wikipedia , lookup

Placebo-controlled study wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Project No. TREN-05-FP6TR-S07.61320-518404-DRUID
DRUID
Driving under the Influence of Drugs, Alcohol and Medicines
Integrated Project
1.6. Sustainable Development, Global Change and Ecosystem
1.6.2: Sustainable Surface Transport
6th Framework Programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Effects of medicinal drugs
on actual and simulated
driving
Start date of project: 15.10.2006
Duration: 48 months
Organisation name of lead contractor for this deliverable: UMaas
1
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
6th Framework Programme
Deliverable D 1.2.2.
Effects of medicinal drugs on actual and
simulated driving
Status: Final
Editor/author: Jan Ramaekers, Maastricht University, The Netherlands
Workpagage Leader: Anja Knoche, BASt, Germany
Project Co-ordinator: Horst Schulze, BASt, Germany
Partners
Tim Leufkens, Wendy Bosker. Kim Kuypers, Annemiek Vermeeren, Jan Ramaekers; Maastricht
University, The Netherlands
Markus Schumacher, Anja Knoche; BASt, Germany
Marie Laure Bocca, Pierre Denise, University of Caen, France
Catherine Berthelon: IFSTTAR/INRETS, France
Pierre Angelo Sardi, Gian Marco Sardi, Claudio Signoretti; SIPSiVi, Italy.
Katerina Touliou, CERT/HIT, Greece
Gisela Skopp, University of Heidelberg.
Date: July 7, 2011
2
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
EXECUTIVE SUMMARY ............................................................................................................................. 5
BACKGROUND AND RATIONALE .................................................................................................................... 5
DESIGN, DOSING AND STUDY PROCEDURES ................................................................................................... 8
STANDARD DRIVING PARAMETERS ............................................................................................................. 10
DEFINITION OF CLINICALLY RELEVANT DRUG EFFECTS ............................................................................. 11
STANDARD TOXICOLOGICAL ANALYSES ..................................................................................................... 11
STANDARD STATISTICAL METHODS ............................................................................................................ 11
MAIN RESULTS AND DISCUSSION ................................................................................................................. 12
CONCLUSIONS ............................................................................................................................................ 21
REFERENCES .............................................................................................................................................. 21
CHAPTER 1: RESIDUAL EFFECTS OF ZOPICLONE 7.5 MG ON HIGHWAY DRIVING
PERFORMANCE IN INSOMNIA PATIENTS AND HEALTHY CONTROLS: A PLACEBO
CONTROLLED CROSSOVER STUDY ..................................................................................................... 23
ABSTRACT .................................................................................................................................................. 24
INTRODUCTION .......................................................................................................................................... 25
METHODS ................................................................................................................................................... 25
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS .............................................................................................................................. 30
RESULTS..................................................................................................................................................... 31
DISCUSSION ................................................................................................................................................ 38
REFERENCES .............................................................................................................................................. 41
CHAPTER 2: DRIVING PERFORMANCE OF CHRONIC USERS OF HYPNOTICS AND
UNMEDICATED INSOMNIA PATIENTS ................................................................................................. 44
ABSTRACT .................................................................................................................................................. 45
INTRODUCTION .......................................................................................................................................... 46
METHODS ................................................................................................................................................... 47
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS .............................................................................................................................. 51
RESULTS..................................................................................................................................................... 59
DISCUSSION ................................................................................................................................................ 61
REFERENCES .............................................................................................................................................. 64
CHAPTER 3 : EFFECTS OF BENZODIAZEPINES ON DRIVING PERFORMANCE OF ANXIETY
PATIENTS.................................................................................................................................................... 68
ABSTRACT .................................................................................................................................................. 69
INTRODUCTION .......................................................................................................................................... 70
METHODS ................................................................................................................................................... 72
GROUP ....................................................................................................................................................... 72
STATISTICS ................................................................................................................................................. 78
RESULTS..................................................................................................................................................... 79
DISCUSSION ................................................................................................................................................ 91
REFERENCES .............................................................................................................................................. 94
CHAPTER 4: DAYTIME DRIVING IN TREATED (CPAP) AND UNTREATED SLEEP APNOEA
PATIENTS.................................................................................................................................................... 96
ABSTRACT .................................................................................................................................................. 97
INTRODUCTION .......................................................................................................................................... 98
METHOD .................................................................................................................................................. 100
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS ............................................................................................................................ 104
RESULTS................................................................................................................................................... 104
DISCUSSION .............................................................................................................................................. 112
REFERENCES ............................................................................................................................................ 116
CHAPTER 5 : EFFECTS OF CODOLIPRANE AND ZOLPIDEM, ALONE OR IN COMBINATION,
ON ELDERLY DRIVERS’BEHAVIOR ................................................................................................... 118
ABSTRACT ................................................................................................................................................ 119
INTRODUCTION ........................................................................................................................................ 120
METHOD .................................................................................................................................................. 120
RESULTS................................................................................................................................................... 122
3
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
DISCUSSION .............................................................................................................................................. 128
REFERENCES ............................................................................................................................................ 129
CHAPTER 6 : ACUTE EFFECTS OF 3 DOSES OF ANALGESICS ON SIMULATED DRIVING
PERFORMANCE IN HEALTHY VOLUNTEERS................................................................................... 131
ABSTRACT ................................................................................................................................................ 132
INTRODUCTION ........................................................................................................................................ 133
METHOD .................................................................................................................................................. 134
STATISTICS ............................................................................................................................................... 136
RESULTS................................................................................................................................................... 137
DISCUSSION .............................................................................................................................................. 140
REFERENCES ............................................................................................................................................ 143
CHAPTER 7 : DOSE RELATED EFFECTS OF DRONABINOL ON ACTUAL DRIVING
PERFORMANCE OF OCCASIONAL AND HEAVY CANNABIS USERS ............................................ 147
ABSTRACT ................................................................................................................................................ 148
INTRODUCTION ........................................................................................................................................ 149
METHOD .................................................................................................................................................. 151
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS ............................................................................................................................ 153
RESULTS................................................................................................................................................... 155
DISCUSSION .............................................................................................................................................. 161
REFERENCES ............................................................................................................................................ 164
CHAPTER 8 : EFFECTS OF ANALGETIC MEDICATION ON ACTUAL DRIVING ......................... 166
ABSTRACT ................................................................................................................................................ 167
INTRODUCTION ........................................................................................................................................ 167
METHODS ................................................................................................................................................. 169
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS ............................................................................................................................ 176
RESULTS................................................................................................................................................... 176
DISCUSSION .............................................................................................................................................. 183
REFERENCES ............................................................................................................................................ 185
CHAPTER 9: EFFECTS OF OPIOID ANALGESICS ON DRIVING RELATED SKILLS ................... 188
ABSTRACT ................................................................................................................................................ 189
INTRODUCTION ........................................................................................................................................ 190
METHODS ................................................................................................................................................. 191
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS ............................................................................................................................ 201
RESULTS................................................................................................................................................... 201
DISCUSSION .............................................................................................................................................. 211
REFERENCES ............................................................................................................................................ 214
CHAPTER 10: RISPERIDONE EFFECTS ON REAL DRIVING PERFORMANCE COMPARED WITH
THE EFFECTS OF ALCOHOL ................................................................................................................ 217
ABSTRACT ................................................................................................................................................ 218
INTRODUCTION ........................................................................................................................................ 219
METHOD .................................................................................................................................................. 220
STATISTICAL ANALYSES ........................................................................................................................... 223
DISCUSSION .............................................................................................................................................. 225
REFERENCES ............................................................................................................................................ 226
CHAPTER 11: BLOOD TO SERUM RATIOS OF HYPNOTICS, OPIOID AND NON-OPIOID
ANALGESICS AS WELL AS ANTIPSYCHOTICS AND AMPHETAMINE-LIKE DRUGS AND THEIR
ANALYSIS IN DRIED BLOOD SPOTS.................................................................................................... 227
ABSTRACT ................................................................................................................................................ 228
INTRODUCTION ........................................................................................................................................ 229
METHODS ................................................................................................................................................. 231
RESULTS AND DISCUSSION ........................................................................................................................ 235
CONCLUSION ............................................................................................................................................ 243
REFERENCES ............................................................................................................................................ 249
4
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Executive summary
Background and rationale
Recent statistics reveal that more than 40000 people die on European roads each year, and another
1.7 million are injured. About a quarter of these deaths, some 10000 per year, are estimated to be
caused by drink driving. Although alcohol is by far the most prevalent psychoactive substance
affecting drivers, concerns have been mounting about the role of medicinal drugs to motor vehicle
crashes. In a recent review of drug driving research conducted by The European Monitoring Centre for
Drugs and Drug Addiction (EMCDDA) recently published a literature review of drug driving research
(EMCDDA 2008). In general, drug driving research has followed to methodological approaches (i.e.
experimental and epidemiological) that are mutually supportive. Experimental studies aim to assess
medicinal drug effects after controlled administration in actual driving tests, driving simulator tests or
laboratory measures of skills related to drivers. Epidemiological studies aim to measure accident
involvement of drivers who are receiving medicinal drug treatment. In experimental studies, the
primary outcome measure is (performance) impairment. In epidemiological studies, the primary
outcome measure is crash risk. Experimental research on individual drug effects is more prevalent in
the scientific literature as compared to epidemiological work. Ideally both experimental and
epidemiological research should be conducted on individual drug effect on driver safety. Unfortunately
however, the combination of epidemiological and experimental data on traffic safety is simply lacking
for most medicinal drugs. To date, only 2 drug classes have been consistently shown to increase
driver impairment and crash in both experimental and epidemiological studies respectively. These
drug classes are: benzodiazepines and tricyclic antidepressants. For other medicinal drug classes
only experimental data is available. In general, the most important conclusions from the EMCDDA
review regarding medicinal drugs effects on driving performance were:
•
Benzodiazepines generally have impairing effects, but some types (whether long-, medium- or
short-acting) cause severe impairment, while others are unlikely to have residual effects the
following day.
•
First-generation antihistamines are generally more sedating than second-generation ones,
though there are exceptions in both groups.
•
Tricyclic antidepressants show more impairment than the more recent types, though the
results of experimental tests after consuming second-generation selective serotonin reuptake
inhibitors are not always consistent.
•
5
In every therapeutic class, some substances are associated with little or no impairment. These
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
therapeutic drugs should preferably be prescribed to those wishing to drive.
For many other CNS drugs, information on crash risk or driver impairment is still lacking. For such
drugs, evaluation of their impairing potential primarily depends on the evaluation of its pharmacological
profile. A drug that is known to induce side effects relevant to driving, e.g. drowsiness, sedation, lack
of concentration, is likely to be classified as an impairing drug in categorization systems for medicinal
drugs and driving. Such classification systems thus generally include more potentially impairing
medicinal drugs than those commonly found in experimental and epidemiological literature. From this
point of view it is very interesting that a French research group (Orriols et al 2010) recently published a
registry based study in 72685 drivers on the association between medicinal drug use and crash risk
group for 4 separate risk levels that are distinguished in the French medication categorization system.
These warning/risk levels include: no risk of impairment (level 0); be careful, read leaflet before driving
(level 1); be very careful, to active advise from physician before driving (level 2) and danger, do not
drive (level 3). Level 2 and level 3 medicines included a large number of CNS medicinal drugs such as
opioids, antiepileptics, antipsychotics, antiparkinsonian drugs, antidepressant, anxiolytics and
hypnotics (including benzodiazepines), antihistamines and drugs used in alcohol dependence. The
fraction of road traffic crashes that were attributable to drugs classified in levels 2 and 3 was 3.3%.
Users of level 2 (OR=1.31[1.24-1.4]) and level 3 (OR= 1.25 [1.12-1.4]) drugs were more likely to be
responsible for their crash A within-person case-control analysis furthermore showed that drivers were
more likely to be exposed to level 3 medications on the crash day as compared to a control day
(OR=1.15 [1.05-1.27]).
This study nicely demonstrates that the use of a large range of medicinal drugs is associated
with a significant increase in road crashes. It offers the perfect rationale for conducting more
experimental and epidemiological drug driving research in order to identify medicinal drugs that have
“impairing potential”of individual drugs that significantly increase crash risk and of contributing factors
that may further increase the risk of become involved in traffic accidents, such as concomitant use of
drugs and alcohol, treatment duration or treatment dose. The studies presented in this deliverable
were all designed from to assess medicinal drug effects, covering a range of drug classes, on actual or
simulated driving performance. Individual drugs that were selected for research belonged to one of the
following drug classes.
Hypnotics/ anxiolytics
GABA is a major inhibitory and widely-distributed neurotransmitter in the mammalian CNS. It is
released by a web of short-axon interneurons occupying some 40% of all synapses. Benzodiazepine
(BZD)
ligands
affect
inhibitory
GABA
neurotransmission
by
allosterically
modulating
the
neurotransmitter’
s ability to open chloride channels at the GABAA /BZD receptor complex. The classic
BZD anxiolytics and hypnotics act as agonists and achieve their anxiolytic, anticonvulsant and
sedative effects through potentiation of GABA stimulated chloride flux. Previous reviews of
pharmacodynamic studies with healthy volunteers have generally shown that BZD agonists can cause
severe impairment in tests designed to measure psychomotor and driving performance (Saletu et al
1987; Van Laar 1998; Vermeeren 2004; Verster et al 2004; Woods et al 1992). Among psychomotor
6
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
tasks, measures of CFF, DSST, tracking, and RT were particularly sensitive to the sedative effects of
BZDs. The data generally indicate that BZDs cause a reduction in their users’overall speed of
information processing and motor response. In addition, performance impairment may still be present
to a certain degree the morning after drug ingestion. These longer-lasting side effects are generally
referred to as the ‘
hangover’or ‘
residual’effects of benzodiazepines. The practical relevance of
psychomotor impairment under the influence of BZDs has been amply demonstrated in a long series
of driving studies employing a standardized test (Vermeeren 2004).To date, most experimental driving
studies assessing effects of hypnotics and anxiolytics have been conducted in healthy volunteers. The
present studies were designed to the effects of these drug classes both in healthy volunteers as well
as in insomnia patients, and to evaluate differences in drug effects in both groups.
Opioid and non-opioid analgesics
Analgesics can basically be distinguished in opioid and non-opioid analgesicss. Opioids include
compounds such as morphine, codeine, oxycodone, hydrocodone, tramadol, buprenorphine which all
exert similar influence of the cerebral opioid receptor system. There are three principal classes of
opioid receptors, , ,
(mu, kappa, and delta), although more classes have been reported. Opioids
are very effective anelgesics but are also know to produce a range of unpleasant side effecs such as
sedation, nausea and vomiting. High doses of opioids may produce opioid toxicity (confusion,
respiratory depression and seizures), but tolerance to (high) dose effects also rapidly develops during
chronic use. Non-opioid anelgesics cover a large range of drugs that relief pain through a variety of
pharmacological mechanisms. These include amongst others non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drugs
(NSAIDS, eg. aspirin), paracetamol, cyclooxygenase (COX) inhibitors and CB1 receptors antagonists
(cannabiniods). Studies of opioid effects on human performance generally report no impairment of
psychomotor abilities in opioid dependent or tolerant patients. In contrast, evidence of no impairment
of cognitive function during is inconclusive (Fishbain et al 2003; Strand et al 2011; Zacny 1996; Zacny
et al 1998). Studies with non-opioid anelgesics in general also report a lack of impairment
psychomotor and cognitive abilities after single and repeated doses. However, some non-opioid
analegesics can be considered as potentially impairing but experimental need to be undertaken to
assess the exact nature and magnitude of their effects on human performance. For examples,
cannabinoids such as dronabinol are known to stimulate CB1 receptors in the brain and are expected
to impair driver performance as has been previously demonstrated with cannabis (Ramaekers et al
2004).
Antipsychotics
Antipsychotics all share a common affinity for D2 receptors. Phenothiazines, such as thioridazine and
chlorpromazine were the first D2 receptor antagonists used in the treatment of schizophrenia. Most
produce profound sedation by blocking dopamine neurotransmission required for sustaining arousal
(McClelland et al 1990; Wylie et al 1993). Additional blockade of histaminergic, anticholinergic and adrenergic neurotransmission also contributes to the sedative potential of phenothiazines and results
in a high prevalence of concentration difficulties, fatigue, and daytime sleepiness among. Since their
7
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
introduction in the fifties, more selective and potent dopaminergic drugs such as haloperidol have
largely replaced these drugs. Like any dopaminergic receptor antagonists in empirical studies
employing patients or healthy volunteers, haloperidol also produced severe sedation responsible for
psychomotor impairment (King and Henry 1992; Ramaekers et al 1999). Yet sedation produced by
selective dopaminergic antipsychotics is less profound and less capable of affecting a variety of
mental functions and dependent behaviors, as compared to antipsychotics that block postsynaptic
receptors within other monoamine systems as well. More recently, a new generation of comparable
antipsychotics has been developed that, besides affinity for dopaminergic receptors, possess multiple
mechanisms of action. Clozapine, risperidone, olanzepine and quetiapine are potent antagonists of
the 5HT2A, H1 and
1
receptor, and, in the case of clozapine and olanzapine, the muscarinic
acetylcholine receptor as well. None of these antipsychotics have been extensively investigated in
studies designed for showing their effects on psychomotor and cognitive function. Yet in theory all of
them should produce deficits in performance comparable to those observed for the earlier
phenothiazines.
Design, dosing and study procedures
The following experimental studies were designed to assess the effects of medicinal drugs on actual
or simulated driving performance.
Hypnotics, anxiolytics, sleep disorders:
1)
Residual effects of zopiclone 7.5 mg on highway driving performance in insomnia
patients and healthy controls (Maastricht University, Netherlands)
2)
Actual driving performance of chronic users of hypnotics and unmedicated insomnia
patients (Maastricht University, Netherlands)
3)
Effects of alprazolam on simulated driving performance of anxious patients
(CERTH/ HIT, Greece)
4)
Simulated daytime driving in treated (CPAP) and untreated sleep apnoea patients
(CERTH/HIT, Greece)
Opioid and non-opioid analgesics
5)
Effects of codiliprane (codeine/paracetamol) and zolpidem, alone or in combination,
on elderly drivers’behavior (University of Caen/ INRETS, France)
6)
Acute effects of 3 doses of codiliprane (codeine/paracetamol) on simulated driving
performance of healthy volunteers (University of Caen/ INRETS, France)
7)
Effects of dronabinol on actual driving performance of occasional and heavy
cannabis users (Maastricht University, Netherlands)
8)
Effects of opioid analgesics on actual driving performance of pain patients (BASt,
Germany/ Maastricht University, Netherlands).
8
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Antipsychotics
9)
The effects of risperidone on driving performance of ambulant schizophrenic
patients diagnosed with a psychosis (SIPSiVi, Italy)
What all studies have in common is their use of patient populations or regular users of a drug under
study. The only exception is the dose effect study of codiliprane on simulated driving that was
conducted in healthy volunteers. As such, results from the present studies should provide a high face
validity relative to many previous studies that have been conducted in healthy volunteers, because of
its undisputed relevance and generalization to user populations.
In general, the experimental studies employed either placebo controlled, cross-over within
subjects designs (studies 5 and 6) a between group design (studies 2, 4, 8 and 9) or mixed design
(studies 1, 3 and 7). The studies furthermore proceeded from conventional laboratory testing of
psychomotor skills and cognition to sophisticated driving simulators (i.e. University of Caen, INRETS,
CERTH/HIT), actual driving tests on a closed course (SIPSiVi) and actual on-the-road driving tests
(Maastricht University, BAST) for establishing the driving hazard potential of the respective drugs.
Driving tests were conducted at Tmax, when drug concentrations were maximal or, in case of hypnotic
drug studies in the morning after a nocturnal dose in order to assess residual effects on driving. More
details on study designs, screening, subject characteristics and in- and exclusion criteria can be found
in the separate study reports that are included as separate chapters in the present deliverable. All
studies adhered to the following common set of pre-defined instructions with respect to study
procedures:
•
Number of subjects: the minimum number of subjects was 16. The choice for a subjects’
sample-size was always corroborated by a statistical power analysis.
•
Drug screens: subjects were always tested for drugs in urine prior to administration of drugs
or medicines. Urine was checked for 5 major drugs: i.e. THC, benzodiazepines, opiates,
stimulants and cocaine. Subjects that tested positive for drugs were dismissed (sent home)
and asked to return to the lab at another date and drug negative.
•
Alcohol screens: subjects were always tested for alcohol (by breathalysing) prior to drug or
medicine administration. Subjects that tested positive for alcohol were dismissed (sent home)
and asked to return to the lab at another date and alcohol negative.
•
s license. In case of the stimulant studies
Driving experience: subjects needed to have a driver’
there will be no demand regarding driving experience as the target population is expected to
be very young.
•
Blood alcohol concentration (BAC): all partners employed a standard BAC unit: i.e. mg/mL
•
Training sessions: all subjects received training sessions of actual driving tests, simulator
driving tests and/or laboratory performance tests in order minimize learning effects. Training
was performed in all subjects to achieve a stable performance level prior to study entrance.
•
Subjective measures: all partners included a set of subjective measures on drug effects (e.g.
alertness, mental effort etc).
•
9
Ethics: all partners obtained study approval from their local (and national) ethics review boards
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
and conducted their study according the declaration of Helsinki and Good clinical practice.
Standard driving parameters
All partners adhered to a standard set of driving parameters to increase comparability between
studies. These driving parameters basically covered 3 core levels of driving behaviours:
•
Automated behaviours – Well-learned (over-learned) skills
•
Controlled behaviours – Controlled manoeuvres in traffic
•
Executive, strategic behaviours - Interactive functions with ongoing traffic, planning, risk
taking
All partners agreed on a minimum of 2 driving scenarios to be included in each and every study.
These scenarios represent the behavioural levels above, and constituted the primary driving measures
over all studies.
Road tracking scenario (automated behaviours)
The road tracking scenario was based on the Road Tracking Tests that has been used in the
Netherland in over 100 studies for measuring drug effects on driving (O'Hanlon et al 1982).
Participants are required to drive a 100km course maintaining a constant speed of 95 km/h and a
steady lateral position in traffic lanes. The primary driving measure is the standard deviation of lateral
position or SDLP. SDLP is an index of road tracking error or weaving, swerving and overcorrecting.
SDLP is measured using an electro-optical device mounted on the rear of the vehicle which
continuously records lateral position relative to the traffic lane. An increase in SDLP, measured in
centimeters, indicates driver impairment, as the driver’
s ability to hold the car in a steady lateral
position diminishes.
Car-Following scenario (controlled behaviours)
The Car Following task was developed to measure attention and perception performance, as errors in
these areas often lead to accident causation. In this task participants are required to match the speed
of a lead vehicle and to maintain a constant distance from the vehicle as it executes a series of
deceleration and acceleration manoeuvres. The primary dependant variable is reaction time to lead
vehicle’
s speed decelerations. This test assesses a driver’
s ability to adapt to manoeuvres of other
motorists . (Brookhuis and de Waard 1993; Ramaekers and O'Hanlon 1994).
Risk taking scenario (strategic behaviours)
Risk taking scenarios were only embedded in studies using a driving simulator. Standard parameters
that were used by respective partners were gap acceptance, number of crashes, number of red light
crossings and number of crashes during sudden event scenarios.
In addition, all partners including a number of laboratory tests measuring skills related to driving.
10
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
These test included tracking tasks, attention tasks, reaction tasks and cognitive tasks. Performance
parameters associated with these laboratory tests were considered secondary driving parameters.
Definition of clinically relevant drug effects
All partners employed alcohol effects on driving parameters as a standard reference to quantify
impairment for any other drug. Any drug induced performance change > performance change induced
by BAC 0.5 mg/mL was qualified as a clinical relevant drug effect. Drug effects equal to those
produced by a BAC of 0.5 mg/ml were also considered to define the “threshold”of impairment for an
individual drug. Drug effects were tested for comparability to BAC 0.5 mg/ml effects by means of
equivalence testing (see section statistics). All partners conducted a placebo-controlled alcohol study
in order to calibrate their primary driving parameters for the effects of BAC 0.5 mg/ml.
Standard toxicological analyses
All partners collected whole blood, serum and blood spots for determining concentrations of
dexamphetamine and MDMA during driving tasks. Blood samples were analysed by dr Gisela Skopp
at the University of Heidelberg. Analysis was performed on whole blood, plasma and DBS by
LC/MS/MS following evaluation of the analytical method according to international guidelines. B/p
ratios were derived from in vitro partition experiments (different hematocrit values) and from
corresponding blood and plasma samples (ex vivo). Bland Altman analysis was used to test
agreement of concentrations determined from whole blood and corresponding DBS. Toxicological
analysis served two objectives: 1) determination of drug concentration in whole blood and
corresponding plasma samples to estimate ex vivo blood to plasma (b/p) ratios, and 2) comparison of
drug levels in whole blood and corresponding DBS. A detailed report of toxicological analysis in blood
is given in Chapter 11.
Standard statistical methods
All primary driving parameters were analysed according to the following predefined statistical
procedures.
Superiority testing and equivalence testing
The general statistical analyses consisted of 2 steps: 1) Assessment of overall treatment effect by
means of superiority testing (e.g. ANOVA for within group comparisons). Superiority testing basically
indicates whether drug effects differ from placebo. 2) Equivalence testing of drug effects was based on
difference scores from placebo (within group) relative to the alcohol criterion (i.e. equivalence to a
BAC of 0.5 mg/ml). Basically, equivalence testing assessed whether the alcohol criterion values falls
within the 95% CI for the drug effect. If yes, than the drug effect was considered equivalent to a BAC
of 0.05 mg/ml (and thus clinically relevant for traffic safety). If the 95% CI was below the alcohol
11
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
criterion value than a drug effect was considered not relevant
Concentration effect relations.
Concentration effect relations were conducted for those studies that administered multiple doses of a
dronabinol (Maastricht University) and codiliprane (University of Caen/INRETS).
Data sets were analyzed according to a 2 step procedure. Data collected during different
doses of a drug were converted into difference scores from placebo for analyses of the association
between drug concentration and performance (i.e.: difference score = performance during drug
treatments - performance during placebo treatment). A linear regression analysis was conducted to
establish linear relationships between changes (from placebo) in task performance during drug
treatment and log-transformed drug concentrations in serum. The total number of data points included
in these equations was defined by the number of subjects x maximal number test repetitions x the
number of drug doses.
Second, individual drug concentrations in serum prior to performance assessments in each of
the drug dose conditions was divided over a number mutually exclusive categories covering the full
range of drug concentrations in a particular study. Corresponding change scores of task performance
were then classified either as showing “impairment”or “no impairment”for all individual cases within
each of these categories. Impairment was defined as a positive change score from placebo in case of
SDLP (road tracking) and RT to speed decelerations (Car Following). Binomial tests were applied to
measure whether the proportion of observations showing impairment or no impairment significantly
differed from the hypothesized proportion. It was hypothesized that in case of no effect of a drug on
task performance the proportion of observations showing impairment or no impairment will be equal;
i.e. 50 percent.
Main results and discussion
The present section summarizes the main results from all experimental studies. For full reports of the
studies please see Chapters 1-11.
Studies on hypnotics, anxiolytics and sleep disorder
Two experimental studies were specifically designed to compare the effects of controlled
administration of zopiclone 7.5 mg and alprazolam 0.5mg on driving performance in patients and
healthy controls. Both drugs had previously been shown to produce significant driving impairment in
healthy volunteers (Vermeeren et al, 2004; Leufkens et al, 2007; Verster et al, 2004) but the
generalisation of such results to patient population has been debated.
The present studies demonstrated that both zopiclone and alprazolam produced significant
driving impairment in patients as well as in healthy controls. Nocturnal doses of zopiclone 7.5mg
significantly increased SDLP by 2.6 cm in chronic users of hypnotics, 2.1 cm in infrequent users of
12
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
hypnotics and 3.6 cm in healthy volunteers during morning driving at 10-11 hrs after drug intake. The
increment in SDLP was significantly less in the chronic users as compared with the controls. However,
equivalence testing amply demonstrated that effects of zopiclone on SDLP in all groups were bigger or
comparable to the effect observed after a BAC of 0.5 mg/mL. Laboratory measures of skills related to
driving confirmed findings obtained from the driving tests, and demonstrated impairments of
psychomotor and cognitive functions during zopiclone in all groups.
Alprozalam 0.5 mg increased SDLP by 5.8 cm in medicated anxious patients, 4 cm in
unmedicated anxious patients and 6.8 cm in healthy controls during simulated road tracking
conducted between 1-2 hrs post dosing. These effects largely exceeded effects observed after a
BAC=0.5 mg/ml during road tracking in the same driving simulator which indicates high clinical
relevance. Moreover, alprazolam also increased brake reaction time and increased the time driven a
close proximity to a leading vehicle in a car-following test. In general, these results can be taken to
show that both zopiclone and alprazolam produce significant driving impairment. Results from the
present studies in patients, confirm previous findings of driving studies conducted in healthy
volunteers. This suggests that healthy volunteer studies can serve as valid model to predict hypnotic
and anxiolytic drug effects in patients populations.
Chronic users of hypnotics or anxiolytics did however significantly differ from infrequent users
and healthy controls in one respect. Chronic users did not experience any sedative effects of
zopiclone and alprazolam, whereas infrequent users and healthy users reported feelings of reduced
alertness and sleep. This lack of awareness of (residual) sedative effects of zopiclone and alprazolam
may lead insomnia and anxious patients to belief that car driving is safe during treatment with these
drugs. Objective results for car driving measures however clearly indicate that driving was significantly
impaired after both drugs. These results furthermore stress the major importance of prescribing
physicians to warn their patients about the impairing effects of zopiclone and alprazolam on driving
performance.
Two additional studies were designed to compare general driving performance of insomnia
patients and sleep apnoea patients to that of healthy controls. Insomnia patients had to meet the
inclusion criteria for primary insomnia according to DSM-IV. Inclusion criteria for sleep apnoea patients
OSAS were based an Apnoea-Hypopnoea Index (AHI) equal or larger than 10. Both studies followed
identical designs: driving performance was assessed in treated patients, untreated patients and
controls. In the insomnia study treatment implied the use of hypnotics on a prescription base. In the
sleep apnoea study, treatment implied the use of continuous positive airway pressure (CPAP) as a
mean to prevent cessations and partial obstructions of breathing. Results from the insomnia study
showed that driving performance and driving related psychomotor performance did not differ between
medicated insomniacs, unmedicated insomniacs and normal sleepers (i.e controls). These results
indicate that driving performance of insomniac patients does not differ from that of normal sleepers,
even in insomniacs that had been prescribed hypnotic mediation. The lack of driving impairment in
medicated insomniacs could however be predicted from the type of hypnotics that patients were using.
About 2/3 of the patients received short acting hypnotics or low doses of hypnotics that peviously were
shown not to procedure any residual impairment in driving test ( e.g. zolpidem, temazepam). It is
however more important to note here that therapeutic treatment of insomniacs with hypnotics did not
13
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
improve driving performance of insomniac patients either. This shows, that potential impairing effects
of hypnotics of driving performance will not automatically be compensated by a (presumed) beneficial,
therapeutic of hypnotics on sleep.
Driving performance of sleep apnoea patients receiving CPAP or no treatment was markedly
impaired as compared to controls. SDLP during road tracking was about 10 cm higher in both apnoea
groups relative to the control. In sleep apnoea patients displayed longer break reaction times and
drove at closed proximity to leading vehicles as compared to controls. Driving impairment was
comparable or even worse than that of anxious patients after alprazolam administration (see above)
and indicated severe risk of traffic injury in this patient group. The current finding that sleep apnoea, in
contrast to insomnia, produces severe driving impairment corroborates a large body of previous
research (for review: Fulda and Schultz, 2001). In general, studies in sleep apnoea patients have
consistently shown major driving and cognitive dysfunction in this population. Such deficits are
generally less pronounced or even absent in insomnia patients.
Studies on opioid and non-opioid analgesics
Two studies were conducted to assess the effects of codeine/paracetamol combinations on simulated
driving performance. Combinations of codeine and paracetamol are available on the European market
under the brand name Codiliprane®. Codeine is an alkaloid found in the opium poppy and currently
one of the most widely used opiates in the world. Codeine is a prodrug that is metabolized into the
primary active compounds morphine and codeine-6-glucuronide (C6G). Codeine is, in general, used in
single doses up to 60 mg (and no more than 240 mg in 24 hours). Paracetamol is widely used over the
counter analgesic that is commonly used for the relief of headaches. Codiliprane tablets contain
codeine 20mg and paracetamol 400mg. The “codiliprane” studies were conducted in order 1) to
establish the effects of codeine and paracetamol on simulated driving peformance as a function of
dose and serum concentration; and 2) to asses the effects of codiliprane and zolpidem, an hypnotic
often used in combination with codiliprane in elderly patients. The former study was conducted in
young healthy volunteers according to a double blind, placebo control study design including 3
codeine/paracetamol combinations: i.e. codeine 20mg/ paracetamol 400mg; codeine 40mg/
paracetamol 800mg and codeine 60mg/ paracetamol 1200 mg. The latter study was conducted in
elderly volunteers according to a placebo controlled, cross-over study design assessing simulated
driving after single doses of zolpidem 10mg, codeine 20mg/paracetamol 400mg and their combination.
st
Results from the 1 study did not show any effect of the 3 codeine/paracetamol combinations
on simulated driving parameters or laboratory measures of skills related to driving. Likewise, analyses
of concentration effect relations for morphine, codeine and paracetamol also did not shown any
significant association between drug concentration and the primary driving measures although
correlations between SDLP and morphine/codeine concentration approached significance. The 2 nd
study assess the effects of single doses of codeine 20mg/paracetamol 400mg alone or in combination
with zolpidem 10mg in elderly volunteers however demonstrated driving impairment after single doses
of these when given alone. Codeine/paracetamol increased SDLP in the road tracking test and
increased the number of crashes during the driving simulation. Nocturnal doses of zolpidem also
14
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
increased SDLP to similar degrees (i.e. about 3 cm). The combination of both products did not affect
any of the primary measures of simulated driving. The combination did however produce performance
impairments on secondary measures of simulated driving such as speed and number of line crossings
during road tracking.
In general, data from the coliliprane studies provides conflicting results. The combinations of
codeine/paracetamol produced no effect on driving when administered to young, healhy volunteers,
even when given in high doses. In contrast, a low dose of codeine and paracetamol did produce
driving impairment when administered to elderly volunteers. The present data thus seems to indicate
that the impairing potential of codeine/paracetamol varies with age.
The second analgesic compound that was included to assess its effects on driving is
®
dronabinol. Dronabinol (Marinol ) is a cannabinoid that is used for the treatment of chronic pain,
anorexia in AIDS and other wasting diseases, and as an antiemetic medication in cancer patients
undergoing chemotherapy. The active ingredient dronabinol is synthetic
9
-tetrahydrocannabinol
(THC). THC has been extensively shown to produce driving impairment in experimental studies and
has been associated with increased crash risk in epidemiological studies (Ramaekers et al, 2004). The
effects of dronabinol on driving have never been assessed before but are expected to be very similar
to the effects of smoked cannabis. The present study was designed to assess the effects of single
doses of dronabinol (10 and 20mg) on actual driving performance in 12 occasional and 12 daily
cannabis users according to a double-blind, randomized, 3-way cross-over design. The study was
performed in cannabis users in order to assess the effects of acute and chronic use of THC on driving.
Single dose effects of dronabinol in occasional users represented acute effects of THC, whereas
single dose effects of dronabinol in daily users represented chronic effects of THC use on driving.
Results demonstrated that single doses of dronabinol impaired road tracking performance of
occasional cannabis users during on-the-road driving tests in a dose related manner. Relative to
placebo, SDLP significantly increased by approximately 2.5 and 4 cm respectively, after dronabinol 10
and 20 mg. The upper limits of the 95%CI associated with change SDLP exceed the alcohol criterion
limit indicating that dronabinol effects after both doses on SDLP were comparable to the effects of a
BAC=0.5 mg/ml. The effects of dronabinol on driving performance of heavy cannabis users however
were less pronounced or even absent. Overall, dronabinol did not affect any driving measure as
compared to placebo. Equivalence testing however indicated that the 95%CI associated with change
scores in SDLP after dronabinol 10 and 20mg contained both the alcohol criterion. The 95%CI for the
low dose also included the value zero. This basically indicates large individual variation in change
SDLP after both doses of dronabinol. This suggests that tolerance to the impairing effects of high
doses of dronabinol is not complete and that a high dose of dronabinol may cause impairment on
some regular users of cannabis but not in others. These data are in line with previous research on the
effects of smoked cannabis on driving, showing that THC markedly impairs driving and psychomotor
skills in occasional users and that such effects mitigate in regular cannabis users, due to development
of tolerance.
Finally, a study was conducted to assess driving performance of 26 pain patients who had
been receiving chronic treatments with either transdermal fentanyl, transdermal buprenorphine,
retarded oxycodone (sometimes in combination with naloxone), retarded hydromorphone or retarded
15
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
morfine. They performed a standardized on-the-road “road tracking test”and “car-following test”in and
conducted a number neuropsychological tests, including the Wiener Test Battery. Patient performance
was compared to that of healthy controls who performed the same test both under placebo and
alcohol (BAC=0.5 mg/ml) conditions. Results from the driving test revealed that driving performance of
patients was comparable to that of healthy controls. Neuropsychological tests measuring skills related
to driving revealed that pain patients performed less as compared to healthy control on a number of
tests. However, these differences could also be contributed to differences in age between both
groups. Patients were generally older than their healthy controls. This may be an important bias as
age was shown to correlate positively with driver impairment on these tests. A single dose of alcohol
(i.e BAC=0.5 mg/ml) on skills related to driving did not produce any change in performance as
compared to placebo.
Study on antipsychotics
One partner scheduled a study to assess driver performance of schizophrenic patients with a history of
psychotic episodes receiving risperidone or paliperidone treatment. Their driving performance was
compared to that of a group of healthy controls who were treated with placebo and a single dose of
alcohol. The patient group consisted of 15 ambulant schizophrenic patients that were recruited in
collaboration with the psychiatrist health department of Rome. The inclusion criteria were: patients
treated with risperidone or paliperidone, alone or with mood stabilizing medicines; regular use of fixed
doses for at least three months (3-4 mg/day); diagnosis of schizophrenia, paranoia or bipolar
psychosis. Other inclusion criteria were the possession of a normal driving license, driving a car at
least once a week.
The driving test procedures that were employed in the current study markedly differed from the
general description of standardized driving test procedures given above. The study was conducted at
a closed driving course that posed a number of limitations on driving procedures and driving
environment. Due to the sharp curves in the closed-course, driving tests were conducted at a
maximum speed of only 30 km/h only. The closed course consisted of a single lane with many curves
and few straight sections. Consequently, it was impossible to conduct a road tracking test according to
standardized procedures (e.g. mean driving speed 95 km/h, 1 hour driving of straight primary highway)
in the given circumstances.
As an alternative, the road tracking test was conducted at a driving speed of 30km/h while
measuring the lateral positions relative to the right side lane delineation. The drivers were instructed to
maintain a constant distance of 30 cm from right line during road tracking. SDLP was calculated
separately for straight section as well as curved sections of the driving course. The test was conducted
twice: i.e. with and without feedback on the actual driving speed. Risk taking was measured by means
of a sudden event scenario. Drivers had to respond to a sudden crossing of a “bobby car”in order to
assess their brake reaction time.
Results demonstrated that overall mean lateral position and mean brake reaction time to
sudden events differed between the schizophrenic patient group and controls receiving alcohol or
placebo. The overall effects on lateral position were primarily caused by alcohol relative to placebo.
16
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Direct comparisons between driving performance of patients receiving risperidone and healthy controls
receiving placebo did not reveal any significant differences. However, evaluations of SDLP and
reaction time to sudden events indicated the performance of the patient group was comparable or
worse as compared to healthy controls driving under the influence of a BAC= 0.5 mg/ml. In addition,
driving performance of 11 patients was also evaluated on the standardized Vienna Test Battery. In
total, 9 patients passed this driving evaluation tests and 2 patients failed the test. Together these
results suggest that most driving parameters in this study were not sensitive to the effects of alcohol
and risperidone.
However the standard deviation of lateral position and reaction time to sudden events were
signifanctly increased and comparable or bigger than those observed after a blood alcohol
concentration of 0.5 mg/ml. The present data thus seems to indicate that patients under the influence
of risperidone do demonstrate some impairments that should be considered of clinical relevance. It is
however impossible to determine from the current data whether these impairments in patients were
actually caused by risperidone, the underlying medical disease (psychosis) or both. The general lack
of differences in driving performance between patients and controls (during placebo) seems to indicate
that driving abilities of these groups are comparable. However, the general lack of alcohol effects on
many driving parameters also indicated that these measures may not have been very sensitive to pick
up impairments. The present data therefore do not provide a definitive answers to the question
whether patients on risperidone are fit to drive. The fact that at least two driving parameters
demonstrated relevant impairments in patients however potentially indicates an elevated risk in these
patients. Individual patients using risperidone should we warned accordingly by their prescribing
doctors when discussing their driving capabillities.
17
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Table 1. Summary of treatment and sleep disorder effects on primary and secondary driving parameters as well as subjective measures of arousal or sleep
(ZOP=zopiclone; PLA=placebo; BAS=baseline and ALP=alprazolam)
Study 1:
Study 2:
Study 3:
Study 4:
Residual effects of zopiclone 7.5 mg
Insomnia patients
Alprazolam 0.5 mg in anxious patients
Sleep apnoea
(Maastricht University)
(Maastricht University)
(CERTH/HIT)
patients (CERT/HIT)
Chronic hypnotic
Infrequent hypnotic
users
users
ZOP vs PLA
ZOP vs PLA
Healthy controls
ZOP vs PLA
Medicated insomnia
Medicated
Unmedicated
patients vs unmedicated
anxious patients
anxious patients
Healthy controls
Patients with CPAP
insomnia patients vs
ALP vs BAS
ALP vs BAS
ALP vs BAS
CPAP vs controls
Increased SDLP;
Increased SDLP;
Increased SDLP;
Increased SDLP in
vs patients with no
healthy controls
Road tracking
Increased SDLP;
Increased SDLP;
Increased SDLP;
No difference between
Impairment > BAC
Impairment > BAC
Impairment > BAC
groups
0.5 mg/ml
0.5 mg/ml
0.5 mg/ml
Impairment >
Impairment >
Impairment >
both patients groups
BAC 0.5 mg/ml
BAC 0.5 mg/ml
BAC 0.5 mg/ml
relative to controls
No difference
between patient
groups
Car-Following
Risk Taking
No effect
Not assessed
No effect
Not assessed
No effect
No difference between
Increased brake
Increased brake
Increased brake
Increased brake
groups
reaction time
reaction time
reaction time
reaction time and
and increased
and increased
and increased
increased time driven
time driven at
time driven at
time driven at
at close distance to
close distance to
close distance to
close distance to
leading vehicle in both
leading vehicle
leading vehicle
leading vehicle
patient groups
Not assessed
Not assessed
Not assessed
Not assessed
Not assessed
Not assessed
No effect
No effect
Laboratory measures of
Impairment of
Impairment of
Impairment of
No group differences in
skills related to driving
memory, tracking,
memory, tracking,
memory, tracking,
verbal memory, divided
divided attention,
divided attention,
divided attention,
attention, vigilance and
inhibitory control
inhibitory control
inhibitory control
inhibitory control.
Subjective measures
Increased next day
No effect
Decreased next
No group differences in
(sleepiness/alertness)
alertness
day alertness
sleepiness and alertness
18
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
No effect
Decreased
No difference
reaction time
between groups
Decreased
Decreased
Decreased alertness
alertness
alertness
in patient groups
Table 2. Summary of treatment and pain disorder effects on primary and secondary driving parameters as well as subjective measures of arousal or sleep
(COD= codiliprane; PLA=placebo; ZOL = zolpidem; DRO=dronabinol)
Study 5:
Study 6:
Study 7:
Study 8:
Codiliprane and zolpidem in elderly: alone and in
Codiliprane in healthy
Dronabinol in THC users
Opioid patients
combination
volunteers
(Maastricht University)
(BASt//Maastricht
(University of Caen/ INRETS)
(University of
University)
Caen/INRETS)
COD vs PLA
ZOL vs PLA
COD+ZOL vs
COD vs PLA
Occasional THC users
Heavy THC users
Patients treated with
DRO vs PLA
DRO vs PLA
opioids vs controls
Increased SDLP;
No overall superiority
No difference between
Impairment DRO 10mg >
effect of DRO on SDLP
groups
BAC 0.5 mg/ml
Big individual variation in
Impairment DRO 20mg >
change SDLP after DRO
PLA
Road tracking
Increased SDLP
Increased SDLP
No effect
No effect
BAC 0.8 mg/ml
10 and 20mg : 95% CI
includes zero as well as
alcohol criterion value
Car-Following
No effect
No effect
No effect
Not assessed
Decreased Time to Speed
No effects
Adaption
Risk Taking
Increased
No difference between
groups
No effect
No effect
Not assessed
Not assessed
Not assessed
Not assessed
Not assessed
Not assessed
No effects
No effect on Standard
No effect on Standard
Patient perform worse than
Filed Sobriety Tests
Filed Sobriety Tests
controls on some isolated
number of
crashes
Laboratory measures of
Not assessed
skills related to driving
tests
Subjective measures
Not assessed
Not assessed
Not assessed
(sleepiness/alertness)
19
Small increase of
Increased sedation
sleepiness
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Increased sedation
No difference between
groups
Table 3. Summary of risperidone/paliperidone effects on primary and secondary driving parameters as well
as subjective measures of arousal or sleep
Study 9:
Risperidone/paliperidone
(SIPSiVi)
patients diagnosed with psychosis
and receiving risperidone vs controls
during placebo and alcohol
Road tracking
Increase in SDLP of patients >
increase in SDLP of controls during
alcohol (BAC=0.5 mg/ml). Alcohol
also affected lateral position. The
latter was not affected in patients
Car-Following
Risk Taking
Not assessed
RT to sudden events increased in
patients and controls during alcohol
relative to controls during placebo.
Increase in RT in patients was
comparable/bigger than BAC=0.5
mg/ml
Laboratory measures of
9 out 11 patients passed the Vienna
skills related to driving
driving evaluation test
Subjective measures
Not assessed
(sleepiness/alertness)
Toxicology
Toxicological analyses were conducted in order to 1) determine drug concentrations in whole blood and
corresponding plasma samples to estimate ex vivo blood to plasma (b/p) ratios, and 2) to compare drug
levels in whole blood and corresponding Dry Blood Spots (DBS). The mean obtained b/p ratios were:
hydromorphone: 1.04, morphine: 1.03, fentanyl: 0.87, norfentanyl: 1.19, oxycodone: 1.48, noroxycodone:
1.73, alprazolam: 0.81, zopiclone: 0.89, temazepam: 0.71, risperidone: 0.65, 9-OH-risperidone: 0.73. Ratios
were close to reliably established ratios published in the current literature as far as available. For all analytes
except zopiclone, the mean ratio of the DBS and blood concentrations and their relative standard deviations
indicated that DBS analysis is as reliable as analysis from whole blood. Bland-Altman difference plots for the
several substances supported this thesis. Zopiclone is expected to undergo degradation even in DBS. It is
concluded that dividing the concentrations of the analytes blood by the obtained b/p ratios may give a
reasonably good estimate of the coexisting concentration in plasma. There is sound evidence that the DBS
assay has potential as a precise and inexpensive option for the determination of the investigated analytes in
small blood samples. However, stability of zopiclone in DBS has to be tested and compared to the stability in
20
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
whole blood specimens using a stability indicating method.
Conclusions
•
Zopiclone 7.5mg and alprazolam 0.5mg produced significant driving impairment in patients as well
as in healthy controls.
•
Zolpidem 10mg produced significant driving impairment in elderly subjects.
•
Healthy volunteer studies can serve as valid model to predict hypnotic and anxiolytic drug effects in
patients populations.
•
Chronic users do subjectively not experience any sedative effects of zopiclone and alprazolam,
whereas infrequent users and healthy users reported feelings of reduced alertness and sleep. This
lack of awareness of (residual) sedative effects of zopiclone and alprazolam may lead insomnia and
anxious patients to belief that car driving is safe during treatment with these drugs.
•
Diagnosis of sleep apnoea, but not insomnia, is a strong predictor of driver impairment.
•
Combinations of codeine and paracetamol in general do not produce driving impairment when
assessed in healthy volunteers even at higher doses. However, driving impairment became apparent
after the lowest dose when administered to elderly subjects.
•
Dronabinol impaired driving performance in occasional and heavy users in a dose-dependent way.
Equivalence tests demonstrated that dronabinol induced increments in SDLP were bigger than
impairment associated with BAC of 0.5 mg/mL in occasional and heavy users, although the
magnitude of driving impairment was generally less in heavy users.
•
Ambulant patients receiving risperidone/paliperidone is comparable to that of healthy controls.
Patients that are stabilized on risperidone/paliperidone treatment should be allowed to operate a
vehicle.
•
Patients using risperidone drove with a lateral position that was comparable to that observed in
controls. However the standard deviation of lateral position and reaction time to sudden events were
significantly increased in patients and comparable or bigger than those observed in controls with
blood alcohol concentration of 0.5 mg/ml. The present data thus seems to indicate that patients
under the influence of risperidone do demonstrate impairments that should be considered of clinical
relevance
•
There is sound evidence that the DBS assay has potential as a precise and inexpensive option for
the determination of the investigated analytes in small blood samples. However, stability of zopiclone
in DBS has to be tested and compared to the stability in whole blood specimens using a stability
indicating method.
References
Brookhuis KA, de Waard D (1993): The use of psychophysiology to assess driver status. Ergonomics 36:1099-1110.
EMCDDA (2008): Drug use, impaired driving and traffic accidents: European Monitoring Centre for Drugs and Drug
Addiction, Lisbon.
21
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Fishbain DA, Cutler RB, Rosomoff HL, Rosomoff RS (2003): Are opioid-dependent/tolerant patients impaired in
driving-related skills? A structured evidence-based review. J Pain Symptom Manage 25:559-577.
King DJ, Henry G (1992): The effect of neuroleptics on cognitive and psychomotor function. A preliminary study in
healthy volunteers. Br J Psychiatry 160:647-653.
McClelland GR, Cooper SM, Pilgrim AJ (1990): A comparison of the central nervous system effects of haloperidol,
chlorpromazine and sulpiride in normal volunteers. Br J Clin Pharmacol 30:795-803.
O'Hanlon JF, Haak TW, Blaauw GJ, Riemersma JB (1982): Diazepam impairs lateral position control in highway
driving. Science 217:79-81.
Orriols L, Delorme B, Gadegbeku B, Tricotel A, Contrand B, Laumon B, et al (2010): Prescription medicines and the
risk of road traffic crashes: a French registry-based study. PLoS Med 7:e1000366.
Ramaekers JG, Berghaus G, van Laar M, Drummer OH (2004): Dose related risk of motor vehicle crashes after
cannabis use. Drug Alcohol Depend 73:109-119.
Ramaekers JG, Louwerens JW, Muntjewerff ND, Milius H, de Bie A, Rosenzweig P, et al (1999): Psychomotor,
Cognitive, extrapyramidal, and affective functions of healthy volunteers during treatment with an atypical
(amisulpride) and a classic (haloperidol) antipsychotic. J Clin Psychopharmacol 19:209-221.
Ramaekers JG, O'Hanlon JF (1994): Acrivastine, terfenadine and diphenhydramine effects on driving performance as a
function of dose and time after dosing. Eur J Clin Pharmacol 47:261-266.
Saletu B, Grunberger J, Linzmayer L, Anderer P (1987): Comparative placebo-controlled pharmacodynamic studies
with zotepine and clozapine utilizing pharmaco-EEG and psychometry. Pharmacopsychiatry 20:12-27.
Strand M, Fjeld B, Arnestad M, Morland J (2011): Psychomotor relevant performance after administration of opioids,
narcoanalgesics and hallucinogens: Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, DRUID Deliverable 1.1.2.c.
Van Laar MVE (1998): Driving and benzodiazepine use: evidence that they don't mix. CNS drugs:383-396.
Vermeeren A (2004): Residual effects of hypnotics: epidemiology and clinical implications. CNS Drugs 18:297-328.
Verster JC, Veldhuijzen DS, Volkerts ER (2004): Residual effects of sleep medication on driving ability. Sleep Med
Rev 8:309-325.
Woods JH, Katz JL, Winger G (1992): Benzodiazepines: use, abuse, and consequences. Pharmacol Rev 44:151-347.
Wylie KR, Thompson DJ, Wildgust HJ (1993): Effects of depot neuroleptics on driving performance in chronic
schizophrenic patients. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 56:910-913.
Zacny JP (1996): Should people taking opioids for medical reasons be allowed to work and drive? Addiction 91:15811584.
Zacny JP, Hill JL, Black ML, Sadeghi P (1998): Comparing the subjective, psychomotor and physiological effects of
intravenous pentazocine and morphine in normal volunteers. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 286:1197-1207.
22
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Chapter 1: Residual effects of zopiclone 7.5 mg on
highway driving performance in insomnia patients
and healthy controls: a placebo controlled
crossover study
T.R.M. Leufkens1, J.G. Ramaekers1, A.W. de Weerd2, W.J. Riedel 1 & A. Vermeeren1,*
1
Department of Neuropsychology and Psychopharmacology, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience,
Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
2
Department of Clinical Neurophysiology and Sleep Centre SEIN, Zwolle, The Netherlands
Date: 7/7/2011
*
Corresponding author:
Dr. A. Vermeeren
Department of Neuropsychology & Psychopharmacology
Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience
Maastricht University
PO Box 616
6200 MD Maastricht, The Netherlands
Tel: +31 43 388 1952
Fax: +31 43 388 4560
E-mail: [email protected]
23
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Abstract
Residual effects of hypnotics on driving performance have been mainly determined in studies using a
standardized driving test with healthy good sleepers. Responses to effects may differ, however, between
insomniacs and healthy volunteers due to the underlying sleep disorder. In addition, a majority of insomniacs
uses hypnotics chronically resulting in the development of tolerance to impairing effects. Impaired driving
performance in healthy volunteers may then be an overestimation of the actual effects in insomniacs.
The present study aims to compare the residual effects of zopiclone 7.5 mg on driving performance
of 16 middle-aged insomniacs chronically using hypnotics (chronic users), 16 middle-aged insomniacs not or
infrequently using hypnotics (infrequent users) and 16 healthy, age matched, good sleepers (controls).
The study was conducted according to a 3x2 double-blind, placebo controlled crossover design, with
three groups and two treatment conditions. Treatments were single oral doses of zopiclone 7.5 mg and
placebo administered at bedtime (23:30 hours). Between 10 and 11 hours after administration subjects
performed a standardized highway driving test.
Results indicated that zopiclone 7.5 mg significantly impaired driving performance in both insomnia
groups and healthy controls. The magnitude of impairment was significantly less in the chronic users as
compared with the controls. Effects found in the infrequent users were in line with previous studies,
suggesting that these studies are able to validly predict the residual effects of hypnotics in insomnia patients
who do not or infrequently use hypnotics.
Keywords: zopiclone, hypnotics, residual effects, insomnia, on-the-road driving
24
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Introduction
Residual daytime sedation is one of the main problems associated with hypnotic drug use. Experimental
studies have demonstrated that the sedative actions of hypnotics impair psychomotor and cognitive
functioning the morning after evening administration (Vermeeren, 2004). The related reduced alertness and
slowed reactions are a particular problem for individuals who have to drive a car the morning following an
evening dose. Epidemiological studies have shown that use of benzodiazepines, as well as zopiclone, is
associated with an increased risk of car accidents (Hemmelgarn et al., 1997; Barbone et al., 1998; Neutel,
1998; Glass et al., 2005).
The severity and duration of residual effects on actual driving performance of hypnotics have been
determined in experimental studies using a standardized driving test (Vermeeren, 2004). Most of those
studies have been conducted in healthy volunteers rather than in the target population, i.e. patients suffering
from insomnia. Responses to the residual effects of hypnotics, however, may differ between insomnia
patients and healthy good sleepers due to the underlying sleep disorder. In insomnia patients, hypnotics are
expected to improve sleep and, as a consequence, they are expected to improve daytime performance as
well. This improvement is supposed to attenuate or even compensate for the impairing effects of hypnotics.
In addition, the majority of insomnia patients use hypnotics for prolonged periods (Curran et al., 2003), which
may result in the development of tolerance to the impairing effects. Impaired driving performance found in
healthy medication naïve volunteers may then be an overestimation of the actual effects in insomnia
patients.
To date, there is a lack of experimental studies that assess driving performance following hypnotic
administration in insomnia patients. Moreover, the residual effects of hypnotics on driving have not yet been
directly compared between insomnia patients and healthy good sleepers. Recently, an experimental study
explored driving performance between pharmacologically treated and untreated insomnia patients and
healthy, good sleepers (Leufkens et al., in prep.). Results showed that performance was not significantly
different between insomnia patients and healthy controls. In addition, there were no significant differences
between insomnia patients who chronically used hypnotics and patients who used hypnotics infrequently. A
limitation of that study was, however, that the chronic users group used a variety of hypnotic drugs most of
which were not expected to produce residual sedation at all. In addition, variability in dose and half-life may
have added to the absence of any performance impairment. In order to determine residual effects of
hypnotics on driving performance in insomnia patients, studies need to be conducted with hypnotics that
have been shown to produce residual impairment in healthy volunteer studies, such as zopiclone
(Vermeeren et al., 1998; Vermeeren et al., 2002; Leufkens et al., 2009; Leufkens and Vermeeren, 2009).
Therefore, the present study aims to compare the residual effects of the frequently prescribed
hypnotic zopiclone 7.5 mg on driving performance of 16 insomnia patients who chronically use hypnotics, 16
insomnia patients who do not or infrequently use hypnotics and 16 healthy, age matched, good sleepers.
Methods
Subjects
All subjects in the present study participated in a previous study by Leufkens et al. (Leufkens et al., in prep.).
They were asked upon completion of the former study to continue their participation in the present study. In
25
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
the previous study, insomnia patients, in the age range of 52 to 73 years, were initially recruited through a
network of local general practitioners in the region of Maastricht, The Netherlands (Regionaal Netwerk
Huisartsen, RNH). Possible candidates were selected from a computerized database of the Center for Data
and Information Management of Maastricht University (MEMIC) (Metsemakers et al., 1992). This recruitment
procedure was subsequently backed up by advertisement in local newspapers. Healthy controls were
recruited by advertisements in local newspapers.
Three groups of 16 subjects, ranging from 52 to 71 years of age, participated in the present study.
Groups were 16 individuals with insomnia who chronically used hypnotics (‘
chronic users’
; 7 female and 9
male), 16 individuals with insomnia who did not or infrequently used hypnotics (‘
infrequent users’; 8 female
and 8 male) and 16 self-defined good sleepers, matched for age and driving experience (‘
controls’; 7 female
and 9 male). Their mean (±SD) ages were 62.6 (4.5) for the chronic users, 62.3 (6.2) for the infrequent users
and 62.9 (4.3) for the controls.
Insomnia patients had to meet the inclusion criteria for primary insomnia according to DSM-IV
(Association, 1994): (i) subjective complaints of insomnia, defined as difficulties initiating sleep (sleep latency
>30 min) and/or maintaining sleep (awakenings >30 min); (ii) duration of more than 1 month; (iii) the sleep
disturbance causes clinically significant distress or impairment; (iv) insomnia does not occur exclusively
during the course of a mental disorder and (v) insomnia is not due to another medical or sleep disorder or
effects of medication or drug abuse.
Sleep complaints were measured using Dutch versions of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI)
(Buysse et al., 1989), the Sleep Wake Experience List (SWEL) (Van Diest et al., 1989) and the general
version of the Groningen Subjective Quality of Sleep questionnaire (GSQS-gen) (Mulder-Hajonides van der
Meulen, 1981). Additionally, a daily journal and the specific version of the Groningen Subjective Quality of
Sleep questionnaire (GSQS-spec) (Mulder-Hajonides van der Meulen, 1981) were completed upon arising
each morning for two weeks providing subjective estimates of sleep quality.
Insomnia patients were assigned to the ‘
chronic users’group when they used a benzodiazepine,
zopiclone or zolpidem as sleeping medication for at least four nights per week during the previous three
months or more. The average (±SD) nightly use of hypnotics in the chronic users group was 6.6 (1.0) nights
per week. The average (±SD) duration of their hypnotic use was 7.1 (5.0) years. Hypnotics used were
zopiclone (1 patient: 7.5 mg; 2 patients: 3.75 mg), temazepam (1 patient: 20 mg; 2 patients: 10 mg),
midazolam (3 patients: 7.5 mg), oxazepam (3 patients: 10 mg; 20 mg; 50 mg), lormetazepam (1 patients: 0.5
mg; 1 patient: 2 mg), nitrazepam (1 patients: 5 mg) and flurazepam (1 patients: 15 mg). Patients not using
hypnotics or using hypnotics less than or equal to three days per week were assigned to the ‘infrequent
users’group. Ten infrequent users reported ingesting hypnotics at an average (±SD) nightly use of 1.1 (0.8)
night per week. Their average (±SD) duration of use was 7.8 (8.1) years. Hypnotics used were zopiclone (3
patients: 7.5 mg), temazepam (1 patient: 20 mg; 4 patients: 10 mg), nitrazepam (1 patient: 5 mg) and
lorazepam (1 patient: 10 mg) The other six insomnia patients did not use hypnotics. Self-defined good
sleepers did not meet any of the criteria for insomnia and did not use any hypnotics.
All participants had to meet the following inclusion criteria: possession of a valid driving license for at
least three years; average driving experience of at least 3000 km per year over the last three years; mentally
and physically fit to drive; good health based on a pre-study physical examination, medical history, vital
signs, electrocardiogram, blood biochemistry, haematology, serology and urinalysis; body mass index (BMI)
26
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
2
between 19 and 30 kg/m .
Exclusion criteria were history of drug or alcohol abuse; presence of a significant medical,
neurological, psychiatric disorder, or sleep disorder other than insomnia; chronic use of medication that
affects driving performance, except hypnotics; drinking more than 6 cups of coffee per day; drinking more
than 21 alcohol containing beverages per week; smoking more than 10 cigarettes per day.
Participants were screened for major psychopathology by use of the Symptom Checklist 90 Revised
(SCL-90-R) (Derogatis, 1983), the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) (Beck et al., 1961), the State-Trait
Anxiety Inventory (STAI) (Spielberger et al., 1983) and the Multidimensional Fatigue Inventory (MFI) (Smets
et al., 1995).
During participation use of caffeine was prohibited from 8 hours prior to arrival on test days, until
discharge the next morning. Alcohol intake was not allowed from 24 hours prior to each dosing until
discharge. Smoking was prohibited from 1 hour prior to bedtime until discharge.
In order to minimize withdrawal symptoms during the placebo night, patients assigned to the chronic
users group were instructed to discontinue their hypnotic intake three nights before each treatment period.
Chronic users who expected difficulties during the three hypnotic-free nights were provided escape
medication, consisting of zolpidem at a maximum of 1 dose of 10 mg per night, to be used only in case of
intolerable withdrawal effects. Zolpidem 10 mg was selected to limit variability in hypnotic drugs used and
because it is known to be free from residual effects when taken at bedtime before 8 hours of sleep
(Vermeeren, 2004).
The study was conducted in accordance with the code of ethics on human experimentation
established by the World Medical Association’
s Declaration of Helsinki (1964) and amended in Edinburgh
(2000). The protocol was approved by the medical ethics committee of Maastricht University and University
Hospital of Maastricht. Subjects were explained the aims, methods, and potential hazards of the study and
they signed a written informed consent prior to any study-related assessments.
Design and treatments
The study was conducted according to a 3x2 double-blind, placebo controlled crossover design, with three
groups (16 insomnia patients chronically using hypnotics, ‘
chronic users’
; 16 insomnia patients not or
infrequently using hypnotics, ‘infrequent users’; and 16 self-defined good sleepers, matched for age and
driving experience, ‘
controls’
) and two treatment conditions. Treatments were single oral doses of zopiclone
7.5 mg and placebo administered in identical looking capsules and ingested immediately before retiring to
bed at 23:30 hours. Treatments orders were balanced within groups (placebo – zopiclone or vice versa).
Washout periods between treatments were at least one week.
Assessments
Sleep
Sleep during treatment nights was evaluated objectively by polysomnography using montage including
electroencephalogram, electrooculogram and electromyogram. Sleep stages were visually assessed by
qualified technicians according to standardized criteria (Iber et al., 2007). Sleep continuity parameters
derived after analysis are sleep onset latency (in min); wake after sleep onset (in min); total sleep time (in
min); sleep efficiency (in %); and number of awakenings. Sleep architecture parameters are percentages in
27
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Stage 1, 2, Slow Wave and REM of the total sleep time.
Upon arising subjects completed the specific version of the Groningen Sleep Quality Scale (GSQSspec) (Mulder-Hajonides van der Meulen, 1981). In addition, subjects estimated sleep onset latency (in min),
total sleep time (in min), time awake before rising (in min), and number of awakenings.
Driving performance
Driving performance was assessed using two on-the-road driving tests, a highway driving test and a car
following test. The Highway Driving Test (O'Hanlon, 1984) measures road tracking performance that is
mainly determined by the delay lag between sensory information, execution of motor reaction and the
vehicle’
s dynamic response. In this test, subjects operate a specially instrumented vehicle over a 100 km (61
mi) primary highway circuit, accompanied by a licensed driving instructor having access to dual controls. The
subjects’task is to maintain a constant speed of 95 km/h (58 mi/h) and a steady lateral position between the
delineated boundaries of the slower traffic lane. The vehicle speed and lateral position are continuously
recorded. These signals are edited off line to remove data recorded during overtaking maneuvers or
disturbances caused by roadway or traffic situations. The remaining data are then used to calculate means
and standard deviations of lateral position and speed. Standard deviation of lateral position (SDLP in
centimeters) is the primary outcome variable. SDLP is a measure of road tracking error or ‘
weaving’. The test
duration is approximately 1 hour.
The Car-Following Test measures changes in controlled information processing such as selective
attention, stimulus interpretation and decision making, and speed of an adaptive motor response to events
which are common in driving (Brookhuis and De Waard, 1994; Ramaekers and O'Hanlon, 1994). In the test
two vehicles travel in tandem over a 2-lane, undivided, secondary highway at 70 km/h (44 mi/h). An
investigator drives the leading car and the subject, in the second car, is instructed to follow at a distance
between 25 and 35 meter. Subjects are further instructed to constantly attend the leading car since it may
slow down or speed up at unpredictable times. They are required to follow the leading car’
s speed
movements, i.e. maintain the initial headway by matching the velocity of the car to the other’
s. During the
test, the speed of the leading car is automatically controlled by a modified ‘cruise control’system. At the
beginning it is set to maintain a constant speed of 70 km/h and, by activating a microprocessor the
investigator can start sinusoidal speed changes reaching amplitude of -10 km/h and returning to the starting
level within 50 sec. The maneuver is repeated 6 times. The leading car’
s speed and signals indicating the
beginning of the maneuver are transmitted via telemetry to be recorded in the following vehicle together with
the following vehicle’s speed. Phase-delay converted to a measure of the subject’s average reaction time to
the movement of the leading vehicle (RT, in s) is taken as the primary dependent variable in this test.
Headway is continuously recorded by means of an optical distance sensor and serves as a control variable.
Test duration is approximately 25 minutes.
Cognitive and psychomotor performance
Cognitive and psychomotor performance was assessed by use of a battery of laboratory tests for word
learning, digit span, critical tracking, divided attention, psychomotor vigilance and inhibitory control. Tests
were previously proven to be sensitive to daytime sleepiness or sedation due to use of hypnotics
(Vermeeren et al., 1995; Vermeeren et al., 1998; Vermeeren et al., 2002; Verster et al., 2002; Vermeeren,
28
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
2004; Leufkens et al., 2009; Leufkens and Vermeeren, 2009) or insomnia (Fulda and Schulz, 2001).
The Word Learning Test (Rey, 1964) is a verbal memory test for the assessment of immediate recall,
delayed recall and recognition performance. Fifteen monosyllabic nouns are presented and at the end of the
sequence the subject is asked to recall as many words as possible. This procedure is repeated five times
and after a delay of at least 30 minutes the subject is again required to recall as many words as possible. At
this trial the nouns are not presented. Finally, a sequence of 30 monosyllabic nouns is presented, containing
15 nouns from the original set and 15 new nouns in random order. The subject has to indicate whether a
noun originates from the old set or it is from a new set of nouns.
The Psychomotor Vigilance Task (Dinges and Powell, 1985) is based on a simple visual RT test.
Subjects are required to respond to a visual stimulus presented at variable interval (2000 to 10000 msec) by
pressing either the right or the left button with the dominant hand. The visual stimulus is a counter turning on
and incrementing from 0 to 60 sec at 1-msec intervals. In response to the subject’
s button press, the counter
display stops incrementing, allowing the subject 1 sec to read the RT before the counter restarts. If a
response has not been made in 60 sec, the clock resets and the counter restarts.
The Critical Tracking Test measures the ability to control an unstable signal in a tracking task. The
signal deviates horizontally from a midpoint and the subject has to compensate this signal deviation by
moving a joystick in opposite direction. The test includes five trials of which the lowest and the highest score
are discarded (Jex et al., 1966).
The Divided Attention Task measures the ability to divide attention between two simultaneously
performed tasks (Moskowitz, 1973). The first task is to perform the CTT at a constant level of difficulty set at
50% of his or her maximum capacity. In the other task the subject has to monitor 24 single digits that are
presented in the four corners of the screen. The digits change asynchronously at 5-second intervals. The
subjects are instructed to remove their foot from a pedal as rapidly as possible whenever the digit ‘
2’
appears. This signal occurs twice at every location, in random order, at intervals of 5 to 25 sec. Task
duration is fixed at 12 minutes. The main performance parameters are average tracking error and speed of
target detection in the visual search task.
In the Stop Signal Task the concept of inhibitory control is defined as the ability to stop a pending
thought or action and to begin another (Logan et al., 1984). The paradigm consists of two concurrent tasks,
i.e. a go task (primary task) and a stop task (secondary task). The go signals (primary task stimuli) are two
letters (‘
X’or ‘
O’) presented one at a time in the center of a computer screen. Subjects are required to
respond to each letter as quickly as possible by pressing one of two response buttons. Occasionally, a stop
signal (secondary task stimulus) occurs during the test. The stop signal consists of an auditory cue, i.e. a
1000 Hz tone, that is presented for 100 ms. The interval at which the stop signal is presented is dependent
from the subject’s own successful and unsuccessful inhibitions. By continuously monitoring the subject’s
response the stop signal reaction time is calculated during the task. Subjects are required to withhold any
response in case a stop signal is presented.
Subjective evaluation
Subjective evaluations of driving quality, sedation and mood were assessed using a series of visual
analogue scales (100 mm). Subjects rated the degree of effort they had to put in driving performance using
the Rating Scale Mental Effort (Zijlstra, 1993). The scale is a visual analogue scale (150 mm) with additional
29
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
verbal labels. In addition, subjects rated the extent of influence of the drug on their driving performance, prior
to and upon completion of the Highway Driving Test, using a 100 mm visual analogue scale.
The driving instructors rated each subject’s driving quality and apparent sedation at the conclusion of
the Highway Driving Test, using two 100 mm visual analogue scales.
The subjects were instructed to rate their subjective feelings of alertness and sleepiness before the
start of cognitive testing using a 16-item mood scale which provides three factor analytically defined
summary scores for ‘
alertness’, ‘
contentedness’, and ‘calmness’(Bond and Lader, 1974) and the Karolinska
Sleepiness Scale with scores ranging from 1 (extremely alert) to 9 (very sleepy, fighting sleep) (Akerstedt
and Gillberg, 1990).
Blood samples
Blood samples were taken at 9:30 hours after ingestion of zopiclone 7.5 mg to determine serum
concentrations of hypnotics before driving. Samples were centrifuged after a clotting period, and serum was
frozen at -20°C until analyses for pharmacokinetic assessments.
Procedure
Subjects were individually trained to perform the laboratory tests during two sessions of approximately 1.5
hours in a previous study (Leufkens et al., in prep.). In that study, they underwent two nights of sleep
evaluation. Subjects were therefore sufficiently familiarized with the testing facilities and procedures.
Treatment periods started in the evening of Day 1, when the subjects arrived at the site at
approximately 20:00 hours, and lasted until Day 2, when they were transported home after the driving test, at
approximately 11:45 hours. On arrival at the sleeping facility in each treatment period, subjects’eligibility was
verified. They were questioned about adverse events and use of medication since their last visit. Hereafter,
electrodes for polysomnographic recording were attached.
Subjects ingested their medication and retired to bed at 23:30 hours. They were awakened at 07:30
hours and served a light standardized breakfast. At 08:00 hours (i.e. 8.5 hours post dose) they filled out the
subjective rating scales for sleep, mood, and daytime sleepiness, and started the laboratory tests. At
approximately 9:00 hours a blood sample was taken. Subjects were subsequently transported to the start of
the highway driving test. Before driving they rated the anticipated effect of the drug on their driving
performance, and performed the highway driving test between 09:30 and 10:30 hours (i.e. 10:00 – 11:00
hours post-dose). Upon completion subjects were asked to rate the mental effort it took to perform the
driving test and to evaluate the influence of the drug on their driving performance. Next, subjects performed
the car following test, after which they returned to the testing facilities for removal of the electrodes.
Statistical analysis
Sample size was based on a power calculation for detecting a clinically relevant effect of 2.4 cm in the
primary measure of this study, the SDLP. This change corresponds to the effects of alcohol on SDLP, while
blood alcohol concentrations (BACs) are 0.5 g/L as measured in a previous study (Louwerens et al., 1987).
Given a test-retest reliability of SDLP of at least r=0.70, a group of 16 subjects should permit detection of a
mean change in SDLP of 2.0 cm, with a power of at least 90% and an
risk of 0.05.
Overall effects were analyzed using a mixed model analysis of variance with Group as between
30
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
subject factor with three levels (‘
chronic users’
, ‘
infrequent users’, ‘
controls’) and Treatment as within
subjects factor with two levels (zopiclone, placebo). Significant (p<0.05) main effects or interactions were
further analyzed using three univariate comparisons between groups for each treatment, and paired t-tests
between placebo and zopiclone within each group.
Secondly, change scores in SDLP from placebo were calculated and used for equivalence testing
using an alcohol criterion of 2.4 cm while blood alcohol concentrations are 0.5 g/L.
All statistical analyses were done by using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS)
statistical program (version 15.0 for Windows; SPSS, Chicago, IL).
Results
Driving performance
Out of 96 driving tests, one was terminated before scheduled completion because the driving instructor
judged that it would be unsafe to continue. The subject was a female insomnia patient from the infrequent
users group who had been administered zopiclone. Her Standard Deviation of Lateral Position (SDLP) score
was calculated from the data collected until termination of the ride.
Figure 1 presents mean ± SE SDLP values recorded after placebo and zopiclone 7.5 mg for each group
separately.
placebo
23
Standard Deviation of Lateral Position (SDLP in cm)
22
21
*
*
zopiclone
*
+3.6
cm
+1.6
cm
+2.1
cm
20
19
18
17
16
15
Medicated
chronic
users
Unmedicated
infrequent
users
Control
healthy
controls
Figure 1. Mean (±SE) SDLP for each group separately (* = significant drug effect; p<.05)
Analysis showed a highly significant overall Treatment effect on SDLP (F1,45=33.86, p<0.001). Zopiclone
significantly impaired driving in all groups. Compared to placebo the increase in SDLP was +1.6 cm
(p=0.010) in the chronic users group, +2.1 cm (p=0.020) in the infrequent users group and +3.6 cm
(p<0.001) in the healthy control group. T-tests for independent samples showed that the mean increase in
31
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
SDLP from placebo to zopiclone was significantly lower in the chronic users group than the control group
(p=0.045). There was no difference between the infrequent users and controls and between the two
insomnia groups.
Tests of equivalence showed that zopiclone impaired driving performance in all three groups. The
Mean change (95% CI) from placebo in Standard Deviation of Lateral Position (SDLP; in cm)
95% confidence intervals crossed the alcohol criterion of 2.4 cm and none crossed the null-level (figure 2).
4,8
3,6
2,4
1,2
0
chronic users
infrequent users
healthy controls
Figure 2. Mean change (95% CI) from placebo in Standard Deviation of Lateral Position for each group separately
Standard Deviation of Speed (table 1) showed a significant main effect of Treatment (F1,45=12.24,
p=0.001) and a Treatment by Group interaction (F2,45=3.42, p=0.041).
Overall, zopiclone significantly impaired subjects’control over speed variability. Paired t-tests showed that
zopiclone significantly increased SDSP in the control group (p<0.004) and the chronic users group
(p=0.009), but not in the infrequent users group. There were no significant overall Group effects on SDSP.
There were no overall effects on any of the Car Following Test parameters.
Subjective evaluations of driving performance
Mean ± SE scores of the subjective evaluations of driving performance are presented in table 1. The driving
instructors did not judge the subjects’driving quality and appearance of being sedated to be significantly
different between zopiclone and placebo in all groups.
Overall, subjects’ratings of anticipated and experienced driving quality were lower after zopiclone as
compared to placebo (anticipated: F1,44=6.14, p=0.017; experienced: F1,44=5.79, p=0.020). Paired t-tests
showed that these differences reached significance within the control group, but not in the patient groups.
Healthy controls expected driving quality to be worse after zopiclone administration (p=0.006) and they
confirmed this expectation after the driving test (p=0.013). Changes
32
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Table 1. Mean (±SE) scores of driving tests and subjective evaluations for each condition and group separately
Group
Variable
Treatment
Statistics (p-values)
Chronic
Infrequent
users
users
Controls
DrugxGroup
Drug
Group
Highway Driving Test
SDLP (cm)
SDSP (km/h)
placebo
19.7 (1.0)
18.2 (0.9)
17.8 (0.6)
zopiclone
a
a
a
21.3 (1.2)
20.3 (1.0)
21.4 (1.0)
placebo
2.2 (0.1)
2.3 (0.2)
2.1 (0.1)
zopiclone
a
2.4 (0.1)
a
placebo
4.3 (0.5)
4.7 (0.4)
4.2 (0.6)
zopiclone
4.5 (0.3)
3.8 (0.5)
4.4 (0.6)
placebo
1.2 (0.05)
1.1 (0.03)
1.2 (0.06)
zopiclone
1.2 (0.06)
1.2 (0.07)
1.1 (0.03)
2.5 (0.2)
2.5 (0.4)
NS
<.001
NS
.041
.001
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
Car Following Test
Reaction Time (sec)
Headway
Subjective Evaluations by Driving Instructors
Driving Quality
Apparent Sedation
placebo
58.4 (5.8)
58.8 (4.6)
59.2 (5.0)
zopiclone
58.6 (4.7)
61.1 (4.5)
55.8 (3.8)
placebo
18.9 (5.4)
15.1 (5.0)
18.4 (6.0)
zopiclone
23.4 (4.3)
21.8 (3.2)
21.8 (4.4)
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
Subjective Evaluations by Participants
Anticipated Driving Quality
Experienced Driving Quality
Mental Effort
Karolinska Sleepiness Scale
placebo
Calmness
a
86.2 (3.5)
a
67.6 (6.2)
71.7 (5.9)
placebo
67.9 (4.9)
65.0 (4.5)
78.3 (4.3)
a
66.4 (5.5)
zopiclone
60.8 (5.4)
62.4 (6.1)
placebo
31.4 (5.3)
35.4 (5.4)
24.8 (4.7)
zopiclone
45.1 (8.9)
a
a
placebo
b
4.4 (0.5)
3.5 (0.3)
b
4.6 (0.4)
3.7 (0.3)
b
placebo
5.1 (0.4)
5.1 (0.4)
58.8 (4.5)
53.3 (7.9)
66.8 (5.8)
45.1 (6.8)
64.9 (4.1)
73.9 (3.4)
a
65.8 (2.8)
a
placebo
68.3 (3.8)
72.0 (3.4)
76.5 (4.2)
zopiclone
75.3 (3.5)
73.7 (3.7)
71.3 (4.5)
placebo
72.6 (3.9)
75.9 (3.9)
75.2 (3.7)
zopiclone
71.2 (3.5)
74.1 (4.0)
76.0 (3.6)
zopiclone
Contentedness
73.6 (4.8)
zopiclone
zopiclone
Alertness
71.7 (5.6)
69.2 (3.9)
66.2 (4.7)
NS
.017
NS
NS
.020
NS
NS
<.001
NS
NS
NS
.009
.013
NS
NS
.048
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
b
= significant Drug effect (p<.05); = significantly different from control group (p<.05); NS = no significant effect
33
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Table 2. Mean (±SE) scores of cognitive performance tests for each condition and group separately
Group
Variable
Statistics (p-values)
Chronic
Infrequent
users
users
39.5 (1.9)
zopiclone
35.4 (2.9)
placebo
Treatment
Controls
DrugxGroup
Drug
Group
43.6 (2.2)
46.2 (1.9)
NS
.003
NS
a
41.6 (2.5)
5.3 (0.7)
6.8 (0.9)
7.3 (0.8)
NS
.024
NS
zopiclone
4.8 (0.8)
5.8 (0.8)
5.4 (0.7)
placebo
25.4 (1.1)
25.0 (1.3)
25.5 (1.3)
NS
.014
NS
a
NS
.006
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
.007
NS
NS
<.001
NS
NS
.034
NS
NS
.016
NS
NS
.001
NS
.034
<.001
.016
Word Learning Test
Immediate Recall Score
placebo
Delayed Recall
Recognition Score
Recognition Reaction Time (msec)
39.1 (2.7)
zopiclone
24.4 (0.8)
23.4 (1.4)
23.5 (1.5)
placebo
848 (32)
785 (36)
851 (33)
zopiclone
889 (39)
863 (40)
893 (34)
placebo
290 (13)
290 (15)
281 (8)
zopiclone
288 (12)
303 (16)
297 (11)
placebo
3.1 (1.0)
1.7 (0.4)
2.1 (0.6)
zopiclone
2.6 (0.8)
3.1 (1.0)
2.6 (0.6)
placebo
3.1 (0.2)
2.9 (0.2)
2.8 (0.2)
zopiclone
2.9 (0.2)
2.7 (0.2)
2.7 (0.1)
18.4 (1.1)
19.8 (1.3)
a
21.4 (1.1)
2052 (87)
1971 (75)
1941 (81)
zopiclone
2038 (65)
a
2140 (93)
placebo
241 (3)
246 (1)
247 (1)
zopiclone
240 (3)
245 (1)
245 (2)
placebo
437 (19)
432 (19)
438 (13)
zopiclone
442 (18)
a
a
placebo
181 (7)
184 (8)
202 (10)
zopiclone
b
a
a
Psychomotor Vigilance Task
Average Reaction Time (msec)
Lapses (>500 msec)
Critical Tracking Task
Average Lambda (rad/sec)
Divided Attention Task
Average Error (mm)
placebo
zopiclone
Reaction Time (msec)
placebo
16.5 (1.5)
a
18.9 (1.2)
20.5 (1.3)
2086 (71)
Stop Signal Task
Hits
Go Reaction Time (msec)
Stop Reaction Time (msec)
a
184 (9)
452 (20)
197 (8)
459 (15)
229 (9)
b
= significant Drug effect (p<.05); = significantly different from control group (p<.05); NS = no significant effect
34
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Table 3. Mean (±SE) scores of subjective and objective sleep quality
Group
Variable
Treatment
Chronic users
Statistics (p-values)
Infrequent
users
Controls
DrugxGroup
Drug
Group
.022
<.001
<.001
.049
.001
<.001
NS
<.001
.027
.012
<.001
<.001
NS
.031
.008
NS
<.001
.029
NS
.004
NS
NS
<.001
.002
NS
<.001
.002
NS
.007
NS
NS
.009
NS
NS
.001
NS
.027
<.001
NS
Subjective
Groningen Sleep Quality Scale
Sleep Onset Time (min)
b
8.9 (0.9)
b
4.2 (1.0)
3.3 (0.6)
a
2.4 (0.5)
b, c
53 (11)
36 (8)
a
27 (4)
24 (4)
placebo
3.5 (0.6)
3.8 (0.6)
b
2.0 (0.4)
zopiclone
a, b
1.4 (0.4)
a
a
291 (23)
b
385 (13)
403 (12)
a
a
placebo
10.8 (0.8)
a, b
zopiclone
5.3 (1.0)
placebo
114 (22)
zopiclone
Awakenings (#)
Total Sleep Time (min)
44 (11)
1.9 (0.4)
b
placebo
241 (21)
zopiclone
355 (18)
a, b, c
0.7 (0.2)
419 (8)
Polysomnographic parameters
Sleep Onset Time (min)
42.8 (7.6)
c
21.2 (4.3)
31.3 (4.8)
c
16.6 (2.6)
22.5 (2.6)
placebo
94.1 (11.4)
73.5 (9.2)
73.6 (13.2)
zopiclone
b, c
a
a
placebo
zopiclone
Wake After Sleep Onset (min)
Awakenings (#)
Total Sleep Time (min)
Sleep Efficiency (%)
Stage 1 Sleep (% of Total Sleep Time)
Stage 2 Sleep (% of Total Sleep Time)
Stage SWS Sleep (% of Total Sleep Time)
Stage REM Sleep (% of Total Sleep Time)
a
78.2 (12.6)
placebo
8.7 (1.2)
48.4 (5.2)
25.9 (4.5)
35.9 (4.5)
7.6 (0.8)
7.3 (1.1)
a
zopiclone
7.5 (1.0)
5.9 (0.8)
placebo
343 (15)
386 (11)
381 (13)
zopiclone
a, b, c
a
a
372 (14)
414 (6)
4.4 (0.8)
423 (5)
placebo
71.5 (3.1)
80.4 (2.4)
79.6 (2.7)
zopiclone
a, b, c
a
a
77.3 (2.9)
placebo
86.9 (1.0)
88.6 (0.7)
7.9 (0.9)
6.7 (0.8)
7.2 (1.1)
zopiclone
7.5 (1.0)
4.8 (0.6)
a
a
placebo
55.9 (2.1)
51.5 (1.6)
53.2 (2.3)
zopiclone
59.9 (2.2)
54.1 (2.0)
55.9 (2.2)
placebo
15.1 (2.3)
20.6 (1.9)
20.7 (2.2)
zopiclone
18.5 (2.4)
a
21.9 (2.2)
placebo
20.9 (1.2)
21.1 (1.3)
18.9 (1.0)
zopiclone
a
a
17.0 (1.0)
14.1 (0.8)
24.4 (2.1)
16.8 (1.3)
5.1 (0.8)
b
= significant Drug effect (p<.05); = significantly different from control group (p<.05); c = significantly different from
infrequent users group (p<.05); NS = no significant effect
in the patient groups were in the same direction, but smaller. T-tests for independent samples revealed that
both insomnia groups rated their driving quality in the placebo condition significantly lower than the control
group (chronic users: p=0.040; infrequent users: p=0.045).
Overall, subjects’perceived mental effort to perform the driving test was increased after zopiclone as
compared to placebo (F1,45=16.15, p<0.001). Paired t-tests showed that this difference reached significance
35
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
within the control group (p=0.009) and the infrequent users group (p=0.008), but not in the chronic users
group. Differences between groups were not significant.
Subjective evaluations of sleepiness and feelings
Mean ± SE scores of the subjective evaluations of sleepiness and feelings are presented in table 1.
Subjects’ratings of sleepiness as measured by the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale were significantly different
between groups (F2,44=5.28, p=0.009), but not between treatments. The chronic users group felt more sleepy
than the healthy controls after placebo (0.003) and after zopiclone (p=0.023) administration.
Subjective feelings of alertness and contentedness as measured by Bond and Lader’
s mood scale
showed a significant Treatment by Group interaction (F2,45=4.81, p=0.013 and F2,45=3.25, p=0.048,
respectively). In the placebo condition, the insomnia groups felt significantly less alert than the control group.
This difference was significant for the chronic users group (p=0.011). Use of zopiclone increased next day
alertness in the chronic users (p=0.029), whereas it impaired alertness in the healthy controls (p=0.040). For
feelings of contentedness, further analyses did not reveal differences between Treatments and Groups.
There were no overall main effects for subjective feelings of calmness.
Cognitive and psychomotor assessment
Table 2 summarizes the mean ± SE scores of the cognitive performance tests.
Overall, zopiclone impaired all parameters of the Word Learning Test, i.e. immediate recall (F1,45=9.78,
p=0.003), delayed recall (F1,45=5.49, p=0.024), recognition score (F 1,45=6.48, p=0.014), and recognition
reaction time (F1,45=8.50, p=0.006). Paired t-tests revealed that these effects did not reach significance in
each group separately, except for the effect on immediate recall in the infrequent users group (p=0.039), and
the effects on the recognition score in the control group (p=0.045). T-tests for independent samples revealed
that the chronic users scored significantly worse than the healthy controls on the immediate recall score in
the placebo condition (p=0.020). Other significant group differences were not found.
Performance in the Psychomotor Vigilance Task did not show significant overall differences between
Treatments and Groups.
Overall, zopiclone affected psychomotor performance in the Critical Tracking Task (F 1,44=8.15,
p=0.007). Analysis for groups separately did not reveal significant Treatment effects, however.
In the Divided Attention Task there was an overall significant impairment by zopiclone in both the
tracking subtask (F1,44=16.49, p<0.001) and the detection subtask (F 1,44=4.81, p=0.034). Tracking, as
reflected by the average error, was significantly worse after zopiclone administration in both insomnia groups
(chronic users: p=0.024; infrequent users: p=0.008), but not in the control group. Detection, as reflected by
reaction time, was significantly impaired following zopiclone in the infrequent users group only (p=0.048).
A significant overall Treatment effect was found on the number of hits (F 1,43=6.31, p=0.016) and the
go reaction time (F1,4312.63, p<0.001) in the Stop Signal Task. For the number of hits, paired t-tests revealed
that this effect did not reach significance in each group separately, however. Paired t-tests did show that the
go reaction time was significantly slower after zopiclone administration in the infrequent users (p=0.026) and
the controls (p=0.033). Stop reaction time showed a significant main effect of Treatment (F 1,43=15.84,
p=0.001), a significant overall Group difference (F2,43=4.58, p=0.016) and a Treatment by Group interaction
36
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
(F2,43=3.67, p=0.034).
Overall, zopiclone significantly slowed subjects’response to a stop signal. Paired t-tests showed that
zopiclone significantly increased stop reaction time in the infrequent users group (p=0.046) and the control
group (p=0.007), but not in the chronic users group.
Overall analysis for Group differences revealed a significant effect following zopiclone administration
(F2,44=5.27, p=0.009), but not following placebo administration. Post-hoc analysis showed that the chronic
users had significantly less problems with responding to a stop signal after zopiclone than the controls had
(p=0.002).
Sleep quality
Subjective evaluation
Table 3 summarizes the mean ± SE scores of both the subjective and objective sleep parameters for each
group separately after administration of placebo and zopiclone.
Significant overall main effects of Treatment and Group were found for all parameters (number of
complaints: F1,45=49.34, p<0.001; sleep onset time: F1,45=14.05, p=0.001; number of awakenings:
F1,45=33.56, p<0.001; total sleep time: F1,44=52.29, p<0.001). In addition, significant interactions showed that
the effect of zopiclone differed between groups in number of complaints (F2,45=4.18, p=0.022), sleep onset
time (F2,45=3.24, p=0.049) and total sleep time (F 2,44=4.90, p=0.012). Paired t-tests of Treatment effect for
each group separately showed that in both insomnia groups number of sleep complaints was significantly
reduced (both groups: p<0.001) and total sleep time was significantly increased (chronic users: p<0.001;
infrequent users: p=0.001) after zopiclone administration. Number of awakenings was significantly reduced
after zopiclone in all groups (chronic users: p=0.022; infrequent users: p<0.001; controls: p=0.008). Sleep
onset time was significantly diminished after zopiclone in the chronic users group only (p=0.014).
Significant differences between chronic users and controls were found in number of complaints
(p<0.001), sleep onset time (p=0.006) and total sleep time (p<0.001) after placebo. Following zopiclone
administration chronic users reported significantly more sleep complaints (p=0.007), shorter total sleep time
(p=0.001) and more awakenings (p=0.015) than controls.
Comparisons between the infrequent users and the controls revealed that there were significant
differences in number of complaints (p=0.001), total sleep time (p=0.001) and number of awakenings
(p=0.027) after administration of placebo. Differences between the groups disappeared after zopiclone
administration.
Comparisons between the insomnia groups showed that the chronic users had a significantly longer
sleep onset time (p=0.006) and a significantly shorter total sleep time (p=0.001) than the infrequent users in
the placebo condition. There were no differences between the insomnia groups after zopiclone
administration.
Polysomnographic parameters
Significant overall main Treatment effects were found in all polysomnographic parameters for sleep
continuity (sleep onset time: F1,42=4.97, p=0.031; wake after sleep onset: F1,41=18.71, p<0.001; number of
awakenings: F1,42=9.36, p=0.004; total sleep time: F1,42=23.30, p<0.001; sleep efficiency: F1,41=23.26,
p<0.001).
37
Paired t-tests showed that following zopiclone administration all groups slept significantly
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
longer (chronic users: p=0.033; infrequent users: p=0.014; controls: p=0.006) and improved their sleep
efficiency (chronic users: p=0.034; infrequent users: p=0.012; controls: p=0.009). Furthermore after
zopiclone, the infrequent users and controls were significantly less time awake after sleep onset, p=0.016
and p=0.017, respectively. Lastly, the controls had significantly fewer awakenings after zopiclone
administration as compared to placebo (p=0.027).
Except in the number of awakenings, significant overall Group effects were found in all objective
evaluations (sleep onset time: F2,42=5.47, p=0.008; wake after sleep onset: F2,41=3.86, p=0.029; total sleep
time: F2,42=7.11, p=0.002; sleep efficiency: F 2,41=7.61, p=0.002). Following zopiclone administration and
compared to both the controls and infrequent users, the chronic users had a significantly longer wake after
sleep onset time (p=0.001 and p=0.033, respectively), significantly less total sleep time (p=0.001 and
p=0.004, respectively) and significantly worse sleep efficiency (p<0.001 and p=0.001, respectively). Sleep
onset time appeared to be significantly longer for the chronic users as compared to the infrequent users after
both placebo (p=0.029) and zopiclone administration (p=0.010). No differences between infrequent users
and controls were found on any of the parameters for sleep continuity after placebo or zopiclone
administration.
Overall main effects of Treatment were also found in all parameters for sleep architecture
(percentage of Stage 1 Sleep: F1,42=8.13, p=0.007; percentage of Stage 2 Sleep: F 1,42=7.40, p=0.009;
percentage of Slow Wave Sleep: F1,42=11.57, p=0.001; percentage of REM Sleep: F1,42=37.10, p<0.001). In
the infrequent users, Stage 1 Sleep and REM Sleep were significantly reduced after zopiclone administration
(p=0.028 and p=0.001, respectively) and Slow Wave Sleep was significantly increased (p=0.001). Zopiclone
also significantly decreased the percentage of Stage 1 Sleep in the controls (p=0.042) and REM Sleep in the
chronic users (p<0.001).
Overall main Group differences were not found on any of the parameters. A significant Treatment by
Group interaction was shown in the percentage of REM sleep. Yet, further analysis did not reveal any group
differences.
Serum concentrations
Mean (±SE) serum concentrations for zopiclone were 9.9 (1.0) ng/mL in the chronic users group, 11.3 (1.2)
ng/mL in the infrequent users group and 10.7 (0.7) ng/mL in the controls group. Overall Group analysis
revealed no significant differences between the three groups.
Correlations between change in SDLP from placebo to zopiclone and serum concentrations were
0.06 (n.s.) for the chronic users, 0.48 (n.s.) for the infrequent users and 0.54 (p<0.05) for the controls.
Discussion
Results of the present study show that a single oral dose of zopiclone 7.5 mg significantly impairs driving
performance in insomnia patients who chronically use hypnotics, in insomnia patients who not or infrequently
use hypnotics and in healthy, good sleepers at 10 to 11 hours after bedtime administration. The impairing
effect of zopiclone on driving, as reflected by the rise in SDLP compared with placebo, was significantly
38
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
different between the chronic users and the healthy controls. As a result, interpretation of the severity of the
effects differs between groups. The effect found in the chronic users group (+1.6 cm) is on average of lesser
magnitude than that produced by alcohol in a previous study while subjects drove with blood alcohol
concentrations (BAC) of 0.5 mg/mL (+2.4 cm) (Louwerens et al., 1987), which is the legal limit for driving a
car in most countries. In contrast, the increase of 3.6 cm in SDLP from placebo after zopiclone 7.5 mg
administration in the healthy control group is above this effect of alcohol. Zopiclone produced an effect of
+2.1 cm in the infrequent users group, which was slightly less than that of alcohol while BACs are 0.5
mg/mL. However, there was no statistical difference in effect of zopiclone between the infrequent users and
the controls.
The significantly decreased magnitude of effect of zopiclone 7.5 mg on driving performance in the
chronic users as compared to the healthy controls suggests that residual effects are attenuated by chronic
use of hypnotics. This implicates that results from studies conducted with healthy volunteers appear to give
an overestimation of the actual effects in insomnia patients chronically using hypnotics. It should be
mentioned, however, that SDLP scores following zopiclone administration were still significantly increased in
the chronic users group, indicating that residual effects do not completely disappear. In addition, the effect of
zopiclone in the chronic users group may even be slightly larger than found in the present study. Inspection
of the SDLP scores after placebo administration showed that, although not reaching statistical significance,
scores in the chronic users group were elevated compared with the infrequent users and healthy controls.
The results suggest that withdrawal symptoms were still present in the chronic users despite
discontinuation of own hypnotic intake three days before each treatment period. The patients may therefore
have experienced discomfort from the hypnotic free night which may have affected their driving performance.
As a consequence, the elevated SDLP scores in the placebo condition may have reduced the effect of
zopiclone on driving performance in this group. Yet, according to polysomnographic analyses, sleep in the
chronic users appeared not to be significantly affected by possible withdrawal effects during the placebo
night. Besides a significant difference of 20 minutes in sleep onset as compared to the infrequent users, the
sleep profile of the chronic users was comparable with that of the other two groups.
The only indication of reduced ability to perform in the chronic users may be the decreased feelings
of alertness and increased feelings of sleepiness as compared to the controls following placebo
administration. Even more, the chronic users reported feeling significantly more alert after the zopiclone
night, whereas the controls felt the exact opposite. This may suggest that the chronic users experienced
more discomfort after the placebo night than after the zopiclone night and may have influenced their driving
performance. After all, this was their fourth hypnotic-free night in succession possibly causing physical
problems (Pétursson, 1994).
The magnitude of residual effects of zopiclone 7.5 mg found in the infrequent users group is in line
with previous studies conducted in healthy younger drivers (Vermeeren et al., 1998; Vermeeren et al., 2002;
Leufkens et al., 2009). In those studies, the mean increase in SDLP from placebo after zopiclone
administration ranged from +2.5 cm to +4.9 cm. This suggests that the residual effects of hypnotics found in
healthy volunteers can validly predict the effects in older patients suffering from insomnia.
The impairing effects on driving after zopiclone appeared not to be noticed by the insomnia patients.
Prior to the start of the driving test, they did not expect to drive differently after administration of zopiclone
than after placebo. In addition, after completion of the test they did not rate their driving quality differently
39
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
between zopiclone and placebo. In contrast, the healthy controls anticipated their driving quality to be
significantly worse after zopiclone. Their expectations were confirmed by their performance as they
evaluated their driving quality after the test significantly lower after zopiclone administration as compared
with placebo.
Whereas the residual adverse effects of zopiclone remained undetected by the insomnia patients
according to their subjective evaluations, opposite results were found for the evaluations of its therapeutic
effects. Both insomnia groups reported significantly improved subjective sleep quality on most parameters
after zopiclone administration as compared with placebo. The healthy controls, however, appeared to benefit
considerably less from the sleep inducing properties of zopiclone. According the objective measures of
sleep, there were virtually no differences in sleep quality between the groups, however. Still, the chronic
users group felt significantly more alert the morning after an evening dose of zopiclone than after placebo.
The infrequent users did not report a difference in alertness, whereas the healthy controls reported feeling
significantly less alert after zopiclone than after placebo.
The lack of awareness of residual sedative effects of zopiclone 7.5 mg may cause insomnia patients
to belief that car driving is safe the morning after evening administration. Even more, these beliefs may be
strengthened by the experienced improvement of subjective sleep quality. These results stress, however, the
importance of general physicians to warn their patients about the impairing effects of zopiclone 7.5 mg on
driving performance.
The impairing effects of zopiclone on driving performance could not be completely corroborated by
the results of the cognitive performance tests. Although there were overall significant differences between
placebo and zopiclone on a majority of the parameters, in about only half of the cases impairing effects of
zopiclone were found in one or two specific groups. For instance, zopiclone only impaired immediate recall in
the infrequent users group and recognition performance in the healthy controls. Evident impairing residual
effects of zopiclone 7.5 mg on verbal learning have been found recently, however, in both healthy older and
younger subjects (Leufkens and Vermeeren, 2009l; Leufkens et al., 2009). Average scores of the three
groups in the present study following placebo administration appeared to be slightly lower already than the
average scores of healthy older subjects in the previous study (Leufkens and Vermeeren, 2009). This may
suggest that scores were close to a minimum, showing possible floor effects.
Performance on the psychomotor vigilance task was not different between placebo and zopiclone
administration. Although there is ample evidence that the PVT is highly sensitive to the effects of sleep
deprivation (Lim and Dinges, 2008), there seem to be almost no studies assessing effects of sedating drugs
on this test. To our knowledge, the sedative residual effects of zopiclone 7.5 mg have not yet been
investigated with use of this task. There is a recent study, however, showing significant effects of blood
alcohol concentrations of 0.03 mg/mL on PVT performance in 18 healthy men (Howard et al., 2007). This
suggests that the task should have been sufficiently sensitive to detect the residual effects zopiclone, which
were comparable in magnitude to those of blood alcohol concentrations of 0.05 mg/mL as measured by
SDLP. The failure to find an effect on the PVT therefore indicates that performance on this test is less
sensitive to residual effects of GABAergic hypnotic drugs than acute effects of alcohol.
To summarize, results of the present study indicate that driving performance is moderately impaired
in insomnia patients after evening administration of zopiclone 7.5 mg at least until 11 hours after intake.
Chronic use of hypnotics seems to attenuate the severity of effects of zopiclone 7.5 mg. Nevertheless, this
40
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
reduction does not result in an absence of impairing effects in insomnia patients chronically using hypnotics.
The magnitude of effects found in the infrequent users group was in line with previous studies investigating
residual effects of zopiclone 7.5 mg in healthy, younger volunteers. This suggests that these studies are able
to validly predict the residual effects of hypnotics in insomnia patients who do not use hypnotics.
Acknowledgements
This study is financially supported by the integrated project Driving Under the Influence of Drug, Alcohol and
th
Medicine (DRUID), which is part of the European Union’
s 6 Framework Programme. The content reflects
only the authors’view. The European Commission is not liable for any use that may be made of the
information contained therein.
The authors would like to express their gratitude to Gwenda Engels, Nicky van Gennip, Jolien
Gooijers, Liene Ketelslegers, Jasmijn Kromhout, Loes van Langen, Anita van Oers, Elmy Theuniszen,
Natalie Valle Guzman, Floor van de Water and Tim Weysen for the assistance in data collection; Renilde
van den Bossche for the polysomnographic analyses; Cees van Leeuwen for the medical supervision; Henk
Brauers, Willy Jeurissen, Jo Gorissen and Hans Sleebe for ensuring the safety of the subjects during driving;
and Irma Brauers for the logistic work.
References
Akerstedt T, Gillberg M (1990) Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. Int J Neurosci 52:
29-37.
Association A P (1994) DSM-IV: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. American Psychiatric
Press, Washington DC.
Barbone F, McMahon A D, Davey P G, Morris A D, Reid I C, McDevitt D G, MacDonald T M (1998)
Association of road-traffic accidents with benzodiazepine use. Lancet 352: 1331-1336.
Beck A T, Ward C H, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J (1961) An inventory for measuring depression.
Archives of General Psychiatry 4: 561-571.
Bond A, Lader M (1974) The use of analogue scales in rating subjective feelings. Br J Med Psychol 47: 211218.
Brookhuis K A, De Waard D (1994) Measuring driving performance by car-following in traffic. Ergonomics 37:
427-434.
Buysse D J, Reynolds C F, 3rd, Monk T H, Berman S R, Kupfer D J (1989) The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality
Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Research 28: 193-213.
Curran H V, Collins R, Fletcher S, Kee S C, Woods B, Iliffe S (2003) Older adults and withdrawal from
benzodiazepine hypnotics in general practice: effects on cognitive function, sleep, mood and quality
of life. Psychological Medicine 33: 1223-1237.
Derogatis L R (1983) The SCL-90-R: Administration, Scoring and Procedures Manual II. Baltimore Clinical
Psychometric Research, Baltimore.
Dinges D F, Powell J W (1985) Microcomputer analyses of performance on a portable, simple visual RT task
during sustained operations. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comp 17: 652-655.
41
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Fulda S, Schulz H (2001) Cognitive dysfunction in sleep disorders. Sleep Medicine Review 5: 423-445.
Glass J, Lanctot K L, Herrmann N, Sproule B A, Busto U E (2005) Sedative hypnotics in older people with
insomnia: meta-analysis of risks and benefits. Br Med J 331: 1169-1175.
Hemmelgarn B, Suissa S, Huang A, Boivin J F, Pinard G (1997) Benzodiazepine use and the risk of motor
vehicle crash in the elderly. JAMA 278: 27-31.
Howard M E, Jackson M L, Kennedy G A, Swann P, Barnes M, Pierce R J (2007) The interactive effects of
extended wakefulness and low-dose alcohol on simulated driving and vigilance. Sleep 30: 13341340.
Iber C, Ancoli-Israel S, Chesson A, Quan S F (2007) The AASM manual for the scoring of sleep ans
associated events: rules, terminology and technical specifications. Westchester, Ill, American
Academy of Sleep Medicine.
Jex H R, McDonnell J D, Phatak A V (1966) A "critical" tracking task for man-machine research related to the
operator's effective delay time. I. Theory and experiments with a first-order divergent controlled
element. NASA CR-616. NASA Contract Rep NASA CR.
Leufkens T R M, Lund J S, Vermeeren A (2009) Highway driving performance and cognitive functioning the
morning after bedtime and middle-of-the-night use of gaboxadol, zopiclone and zolpidem. Journal of
Sleep Research 18: 387-396.
Leufkens T R M, Ramaekers J G, De Weerd A W, Riedel W J, Vermeeren A (in prep.) On-the-road driving
performance and driving related skills in untreated insomnia patients and chronic users of hypnotics.
Leufkens T R M, Vermeeren A (2009) Highway driving in elderly the morning after bedtime use of hypnotics:
a comparison between temazepam 20 mg, zopiclone 7.5 mg and placebo. J Clin Psychopharmacol
29: 432-438.
Lim J, Dinges D F (2008) Sleep deprivation and vigilant attention. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1129: 305-322.
Logan G D, Cowan W B, Davis K A (1984) On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time
responses: a model and a method. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 10: 276-291.
Louwerens J W, Gloerich A B M, Vries d, G., Brookhuis K A, O'Hanlon J F (1987) The relationship between
drivers' blood alcohol concentration (BAC) and actual driving performance during high speed travel.
In Noordzij, P C & Roszbach, R (eds), International Congres on Alcohol, Drugs and Traffic Safety,
T86. Exerpta Medica, Amsterdam.
Metsemakers J F M, Höppener P, Knottnerus J A, Kocken R J J, Limonard C B G (1992) Computerized
health information in the Netherlands: a registration network of family practices. British Journal of
General Practice 42: 102-106.
Moskowitz
H (1973)
Proceedings:
Psychological
tests and
drugs.
Pharmakopsychiatrie,
Neuro
Psychopharmakologie 6: 114-126.
Mulder-Hajonides van der Meulen W R E H (1981) Measurement of subjective sleep quality. Elsevier,
Amsterdam.
Neutel I (1998) Benzodiazepine-related traffic accidents in young and elderly drivers. Human
Psychopharmacology 13: S115-S123.
O'Hanlon J F (1984) Driving performance under the influence of drugs: rationale for, and application of, a
new test. Br J Clin Pharmacol 18: 121s-129s.
Pétursson H (1994) The benzodiazepine withdrawal syndrome. Addiction 89: 1455-1459.
42
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Ramaekers J G, O'Hanlon J F (1994) Acrivastine, terfenadine and diphenhydramine effects on driving
performance as a function of dose and time after dosing. Eur J Clin Pharmacol 47: 261-266.
Rey A (1964) L'examen clinique en psychologie. Presses Universitaires de France, Paris.
Smets E M, Garssen B, Bonke B, De Haes J C (1995) The Multidimensional Fatigue Inventory (MFI)
psychometric qualities of an instrument to assess fatigue. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 39:
315-325.
Spielberger C D, Gorsuch R L, Lushene R E, Vagg P R, Jacobs A G (1983) Manual for the State-Trait
Anxiety Inventory (Form Y). Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc., Palo Alto.
Van Diest R, Milius H, Markusse R, Snel J (1989) The Sleep-Wake Experience List. Tijdschrift voor Sociale
Gezondheidszorg 67: 343-347.
Vermeeren A (2004) Residual effects of hypnotics: epidemiology and clinical implications. CNS Drugs 18:
297-328.
Vermeeren A, Danjou P E, O'Hanlon J F (1998) Residual effects of evening and middle-of-the-night
administration of zaleplon 10 and 20 mg on memory and actual driving performance. Human
Psychopharmacology 13: S98-S107.
Vermeeren A, O'Hanlon J F, Declerck A C, Kho L (1995) Acute effects of zolpidem and flunitrazepam on
sleep, memory and driving performance, compared to those of partial sleep deprivation and placebo.
Acta Ther 21: 47-64.
Vermeeren A, Riedel W J, van Boxtel M P, Darwish M, Paty I, Patat A (2002) Differential residual effects of
zaleplon and zopiclone on actual driving: a comparison with a low dose of alcohol. Sleep 25: 224231.
Verster J C, Volkerts E R, Schreuder A H, Eijken E J, van Heuckelum J H, Veldhuijzen D S, Verbaten M N,
Paty I, Darwish M, Danjou P, Patat A (2002) Residual effects of middle-of-the-night administration of
zaleplon and zolpidem on driving ability, memory functions, and psychomotor performance. J Clin
Psychopharmacol 22: 576-583.
Zijlstra F R H (1993) Efficiency in work behavior. A design approach for modern tools. Delft, Delft University
of Technology.
43
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Chapter 2: Driving performance of chronic users
of hypnotics and unmedicated insomnia patients
1
1
2
1
1,*
T.R.M. Leufkens , J.G. Ramaekers , A.W. de Weerd , W.J. Riedel & A. Vermeeren
1
Department of Neuropsychology and Psychopharmacology, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience,
Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
2
Department of Clinical Neurophysiology and Sleep Centre SEIN, Zwolle, The Netherlands
Date: 07.07.2011
*
Corresponding author:
Dr. A. Vermeeren
Department of Neuropsychology & Psychopharmacology
Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience
Maastricht University
PO Box 616
6200 MD Maastricht, The Netherlands
Tel: +31 43 388 1952
Fax: +31 43 388 4560
E-mail: [email protected]
44
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Abstract
Objectives: Impairing effects of hypnotics on next day driving performance may be compensated by
improved sleep in insomnia. In addition, chronic use of hypnotics may lead to tolerance and reduce these
residual effects. The present study aims to compare driving performance of patients complaining of insomnia
and chronic users of hypnotics with that of healthy age-matched controls.
Design: A 3x2 parallel group design with three groups and two within subject conditions
Setting: University research institute.
Participants: Twenty-two elderly insomnia patients chronically using hypnotics, 20 unmedicated elderly
insomniacs and 21 healthy, age-matched controls
Interventions: Insomniacs chronically using hypnotics ingested their own prescribed hypnotics.
Unmedicated insomniacs and healthy controls did not ingest hypnotics.
Measurements and results: A standardized highway driving test was performed between 10 and 11 hours
after bedtime. In the evening and morning before the driving test, cognitive and psychomotor performance
was assessed using laboratory tests.
Results showed that driving performance and driving related psychomotor performance did not differ
between unmedicated insomniacs, chronic users of hypnotics and normal sleepers. In chronic users, working
memory, as measured by Digit Span backward, was significantly worse compared to controls. No group
differences were found in verbal memory, divided attention, psychomotor vigilance and inhibitory control.
Conclusions: Insomnia patients appear to be able to successfully perform a one hour driving task that
requires prolonged attentional demands. Chronic use of hypnotics does not seem to change driving
performance.
Keywords: on-the-road driving, insomnia, hypnotics, cognition
45
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Introduction
Approximately one-third of the general adult population suffers from insomnia, reporting difficulties in
initiating sleep or in maintaining sleep, and feelings of nonrestorative sleep.
1, 2
Although non-pharmacological
strategies, such as cognitive behavioral therapy, are increasingly being implemented in the treatment of
3
insomnia, pharmacotherapy is still the most frequently used treatment for insomnia. The primary choice of
sleep-enhancing medication is sedative hypnotics, such as benzodiazepines and the newer benzodiazepine
receptor agonists (e.g. zopiclone, zolpidem and zaleplon).4
Ideally, a hypnotic should improve sleep and be free from residual sedative effects after arising. It is
known, however, that a number of hypnotics that are currently prescribed can produce next-day residual
sedation, depending on type of hypnotic, dose, time after administration and frequency of dosing.5 This may
lead to the impairment of a wide range of cognitive abilities and can have serious consequences for daily
activities, such as driving a car. In order to select the safest alternative among the available hypnotics,
patients and prescribing physicians should be informed about the possible impairing effects of hypnotics.
To date, information of the residual effects on driving performance is mainly derived from
experimental studies conducted with young, healthy, hypnotic naïve volunteers after a single night of
treatment. Investigating the residual effects in this population leaves two important issues unanswered,
however. First, responses to the adverse effects of hypnotics may be different in patients than in healthy
volunteers because of the underlying disorder. Untreated insomniacs report reduced performance in daily life
routines as a consequence of their disturbed sleep.6, 7 When treated pharmacologically, it can be expected
that the sleep improving effects of a hypnotic improve daytime performance in insomniacs. The net effects of
hypnotics on daytime performance may therefore be less in patients than in healthy volunteers. However,
despite complaints of impaired daytime functioning among insomnia patients, there is still little objective
evidence supporting this. Reviews of the literature show that most experimental studies examining cognitive
and psychomotor performance have not been able to objectively demonstrate impaired daytime functioning
in untreated insomnia patients.8, 9 Only minor impairment was found in tasks measuring attention span and
vigilance.9 The absence of objective impairment could be explained by the use of short and relatively simple
tests, in which patients are able to maintain adequate performance by temporarily increasing their efforts.
11
7, 10,
Longer tasks and especially tasks that are closer to real-life activities could be more sensitive to the effects
of insomnia.10
The second issue that has not yet been clarified in experimental designs using single doses in
healthy young volunteers is whether residual effects of hypnotics on driving are still present in insomniacs
who chronically use hypnotics. Although it is recommended not to use hypnotics for periods longer than four
weeks, a majority of insomnia patients are treated for prolonged periods, which may result in tolerance and
reduced effects on performance. 12,
13
There is only one published study that compared performance of
chronic users of hypnotics to that of untreated insomnia patients and self defined good sleepers.
10
Results of
a neuropsychological test battery showed no significant differences between these groups, thus suggesting
that patients may have become tolerant to the impairing effects of hypnotics on cognition. Nonetheless,
epidemiological studies have shown that chronic use of hypnotics is still related to an increased risk of
becoming involved in traffic and occupational accidents.14-18
In summary, it is unclear whether insomnia has significant impairing consequences on daily life
46
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
routines, such as driving a car, which can be attenuated by the use of hypnotics. Furthermore, it remains to
be investigated whether the residual effects on driving performance are absent in insomniacs chronically
using hypnotics.
The present study aimed to determine whether driving performance of chronic users of hypnotics
and unmedicated insomnia patients differs from normal sleepers of the same age. Therefore, we compared
sleep and performance of two groups of insomnia patients to that of a group of age-matched normal
sleepers. One group of patients used hypnotics occasionally and was unmedicated before testing. The other
group consisted of chronic users of hypnotics who used their own prescribed hypnotics the night before
testing. Driving and cognitive performance were assessed by two on-the-road tests (i.e. a standardized
highway driving test and a car-following test) and battery of computerized tests. Sleep was evaluated by
polysomnography and subjective rating scales.
Methods
Subjects
A total of 42 insomnia patients and 21 healthy controls, in the age range of 52 to 73 years, were recruited
through a network of local general practitioners in the region of Maastricht, The Netherlands (Regionaal
Netwerk Huisartsen, RNH)
19
and by advertisement in local newspapers. Insomnia patients had to meet the
20
inclusion criteria for primary insomnia according to DSM-IV : (i) subjective complaints of insomnia, defined
as difficulties initiating sleep (sleep latency >30 min) and/or maintaining sleep (awakenings >30 min); (ii)
duration of more than 1 month; (iii) the sleep disturbance causes clinically significant distress or impairment;
(iv) insomnia does not occur exclusively during the course of a mental disorder and (v) insomnia is not due to
another medical or sleep disorder or effects of medication or drug abuse. Healthy controls were self-defined
good sleepers.
Sleep complaints of patients and healthy controls were measured as a part of the screening
procedure using Dutch versions of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI),21 the Sleep Wake Experience
List (SWEL)22 and the general version of the Groningen Sleep Quality Scale (GSQS-gen) 23. The GSQS
provides a score between 0 and 14 representing a number of sleep complaints. Additionally, a daily journal
and the specific version of the Groningen Sleep Quality Scale (GSQS-spec)
23
were completed upon arising
each morning for two weeks providing subjective estimates of sleep quality.
Insomnia patients were assigned to one of two groups depending on the frequency and duration of
their use of hypnotic drugs (benzodiazepine, zopiclone or zolpidem). Patients were assigned to a ‘
chronic
users’group when they used a hypnotic for at least four nights per week and longer than three months
(n=22). Patients not using hypnotics or using hypnotics less than three days per week were assigned to the
‘
unmedicated insomniacs’group (n=20) as they did not ingest hypnotics during the test periods.
The hypnotics used in the chronic users group were temazepam (n=4), zopiclone (n=4), midazolam
(n=4), oxazepam (n=3), zolpidem (n=2), lormetazepam (n=2), clonazepam (n=1), flurazepam (n=1) and
nitrazepam (n=1). Average (±SD) duration of hypnotic use was 7.7 (±6.8) years, with a mean (±SD)
frequency of use of 6.4 (±1.2) nights a week (table 1).
{Insert Table 1 about here}
47
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
In the unmedicated insomniacs group, 7 patients reported no history of hypnotic use. The remaining 13
patients used hypnotics on average (±SD) 1.0 (±0.7) night a week, since 7.8 (±7.9) years. The hypnotics
used were temazepam (n=6), zopiclone (n=4), loprazolam (n=1), nitrazepam (n=1) and lorazepam (n=1).
All participants had to meet the following inclusion criteria: possession of a valid driving license for at
least three years; average driving experience of at least 3000 km per year over the last three years; mentally
and physically fit to drive; good health based on a pre-study physical examination, medical history, vital
signs, electrocardiogram, blood biochemistry, haematology, serology and urinalysis; body mass index (BMI)
2
between 19 and 30 kg/m .
Exclusion criteria were history of drug or alcohol abuse; presence of a significant medical,
neurological, psychiatric disorder, or sleep disorder other than insomnia; chronic use of medication that
affects driving performance, except hypnotics; drinking more than 6 cups of coffee per day; drinking more
than 21 alcohol containing beverages per week; smoking more than 10 cigarettes per day.
Participants were screened for major psychopathology by use of the Symptom Checklist 90 Revised
(SCL-90-R),24 the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI),25 the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI),26 and the
27
Multidimensional Fatigue Inventory (MFI).
During participation use of caffeine was prohibited from 8 hours prior to arrival on test days, until
discharge the next morning. Alcohol intake was not allowed from 24 hours prior to each dosing until
discharge. Smoking was prohibited from 1 hour prior to bedtime until discharge.
The study was conducted in accordance with the code of ethics on human experimentation
established by the World Medical Association’
s Declaration of Helsinki (1964) and amended in Edinburgh
(2000). The protocol was approved by the medical ethics committee of Maastricht University and University
Hospital of Maastricht. Subjects were explained the aims, methods, and potential hazards of the study and
they signed a written informed consent prior to any study-related assessments.
Assessments
Sleep
On nights before testing sleep quality and duration was measured by polysomnography using montage
including electroencephalogram, electrooculogram and electromyogram. Sleep stages were visually
assessed by qualified technicians according to standardized criteria.28 Parameters derived after analysis are
sleep onset latency (in min); wake after sleep onset (in min); total sleep time (in min); sleep efficiency (in %);
and number of awakenings.
Upon arising subjects completed the specific version of the Groningen Sleep Quality Scale (GSQS23
spec).
In addition, subjects estimated sleep onset latency (in min), total sleep time (in min), time awake
before rising (in min), and number of awakenings.
Driving performance
Driving performance was assessed using two standardized driving tests developed to measure different
aspects of driving performance. The primary test is the Highway Driving Test 29 which measures road tracking
performance. Performance in this test is mainly determined by the delay lag between sensory processing,
execution of motor reaction and the vehicle’s dynamic response. In this test, subjects operate a specially
48
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
instrumented vehicle over a 100 km (61 mi) primary highway circuit, accompanied by a licensed driving
instructor having access to dual controls. The subjects’task is to maintain a constant speed of 95 km/h (58
mi/h) and a steady lateral position between the delineated boundaries of the slower traffic lane. The vehicle
speed and lateral position are continuously recorded. These signals are edited off line to remove data
recorded during overtaking maneuvers or disturbances caused by roadway or traffic situations. The
remaining data are then used to calculate means and standard deviations of lateral position and speed.
Standard deviation of lateral position (SDLP in centimeters) is the primary outcome variable. SDLP is a
measure of road tracking error or ‘
weaving’
. The test duration is approximately 1 hour.
The Car-Following Test
30, 31
measures changes in controlled information processing such as selective
attention, stimulus interpretation and decision making, and speed of an adaptive motor response to events
which are common in driving. In the test two vehicles travel in tandem over a 2-lane, undivided, secondary
highway at 70 km/h (44 mi/h). An investigator drives the leading car and the subject, in the second car, is
instructed to follow at a distance between 25 and 35 meter. Subjects are further instructed to constantly
attend the leading car since it may slow down or speed up at unpredictable times. They are required to follow
the leading car’
s speed movements, i.e. maintain the initial headway by matching the velocity of the car to
the other’
s. During the test, the speed of the leading car is automatically controlled by a modified ‘
cruise
control’system. At the beginning it is set to maintain a constant speed of 70 km/h and, by activating a
microprocessor the investigator can start sinusoidal speed changes reaching amplitude of -10 km/h and
returning to the starting level within 50 sec. The maneuver is repeated 6 times. The leading car’
s speed and
signals indicating the beginning of the maneuver are transmitted via telemetry to be recorded in the following
vehicle together with the following vehicle’
s speed. Phase-delay converted to a measure of the subject’s
average reaction time to the movement of the leading vehicle (RT, in s) is taken as the primary dependent
variable in this test. Headway is continuously recorded by means of an optical distance sensor and serves as
a control variable. Test duration is approximately 25 minutes.
Cognitive and psychomotor performance
Cognitive and psychomotor performance was assessed by tests for word learning, digit span, tracking,
divided attention, sustained attention and inhibitory control. Tests were selected based on their sensitivity to
residual sedating effects of hypnotics or sleep disturbances, and their relation to driving performance.
38
The Word Learning Test
5, 9, 32-37
is a verbal memory test for the assessment of immediate recall, delayed
recall and recognition performance. Fifteen monosyllabic nouns are presented and at the end of the
sequence the subject is asked to recall as many words as possible. This procedure is repeated five times
and after a delay of at least 30 minutes the subject is again required to recall as many words as possible. At
this trial the nouns are not presented. Finally, a sequence of 30 monosyllabic nouns is presented, containing
15 nouns from the original set and 15 new nouns in random order. The subject has to indicate whether a
noun originates from the old set or from the new set of nouns.
The Digit Span Forward and Backward is a subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised
(WAIS-R).39 In this test, subjects are asked to repeat orally presented digits with increasing sequence length,
either in forward or reverse order. There are two trials at each series length, and the test continues until both
trials of a series length are failed. One point is awarded for each correct trial.
The Critical Tracking Test
49
40
measures the ability to control an unstable signal in a tracking task. The
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
signal deviates horizontally from a midpoint and the subject has to compensate this signal deviation by
moving a joystick in opposite direction. The test includes five trials of which the lowest and the highest score
are discarded.
The Divided Attention Task
41
measures the ability to divide attention between two simultaneously
performed tasks. The first task is to perform the CTT at a constant level of difficulty set at 50% of his or her
maximum capacity. In the other task the subject has to monitor 24 single digits that are presented in the four
corners of the screen. The digits change asynchronously at 5-second intervals. The subjects are instructed
to remove their foot from a pedal as rapidly as possible whenever the digit ‘
2’appears. This signal occurs
twice at every location, in random order, at intervals of 5 to 25 sec. Task duration is fixed at 12 minutes. The
main performance parameters are average tracking error and speed of target detection in the visual search
task.
In the Stop Signal Task42 the concept of inhibitory control is defined as the ability to stop a pending
thought or action and to begin another. The paradigm consists of two concurrent tasks, i.e. a go task
(primary task) and a stop task (secondary task). The go signals (primary task stimuli) are two letters (‘
X’or
‘
O’) presented one at a time in the center of a computer screen. Subjects are required to respond to each
letter as quickly as possible by pressing on of two response buttons. Occasionally, a stop signal (secondary
task stimulus) occurs during the test. The stop signal consists of an auditory cue, i.e. a 1000 Hz tone, that is
presented for 100 ms. The interval at which the stop signal is presented is dependent from the subject’
s own
successful and unsuccessful inhibitions. By continuously monitoring the subject’s response the stop signal
reaction time is calculated during the task. Subjects are required to withhold any response in case a stop
signal is presented.
The Psychomotor Vigilance Task43 is based on a simple visual RT test. Subjects are required to
respond to a visual stimulus presented at variable interval (2000 to 10000 msec) by pressing either the right
or the left button with the dominant hand. The visual stimulus is a counter turning on and incrementing from 0
to 60 sec at 1-msec intervals. In response to the subject’
s button press, the counter display stops
incrementing, allowing the subject 1 sec to read the RT before the counter restarts. If a response has not
been made in 60 sec, the clock resets and the counter restarts.
Subjective evaluation
Subjective evaluations of mood, sedation and driving quality were assessed using a series of visual
analogue scales (100 mm). The subjects were instructed to rate their subjective feelings of alertness and
sleepiness before the start of cognitive testing using a 16-item mood scale44 which provides three factor
analytically defined summary scores for ‘alertness’
, ‘
contentedness’, and ‘calmness’and the Karolinska
Sleepiness Scale with scores ranging from 1 (extremely alert) to 9 (very sleepy, fighting sleep).45
At the conclusion of the Highway Driving test subjects rated the degree of mental effort they had to
put in driving performance with the Rating Scale Mental Effort.
46
This is a visual analogue scale (150 mm)
with additional verbal labels. Finally, the driving instructors rated each subject’s driving quality and apparent
sedation, using two 100 mm visual analogue scales.
Blood samples
Blood samples were taken from chronic users at 9:30 hours after ingestion of the drug to determine serum
50
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
concentrations of hypnotics before driving. Samples were centrifuged after a clotting period, and serum was
frozen at -20°C until analyses for pharmacokinetic assessments.
Procedure
Subjects were individually trained to perform the laboratory tests during two sessions of approximately 1.5
hours within 10 days before their first night. After training the subjects underwent two nights of sleep
evaluation. The first night was a habituation and practice condition to familiarize subjects with the sleeping
facilities and polysomnographic and test procedures. The second night was considered as the actual test
condition.
A test condition started in the evening of Day 1, when the subjects arrived at the site at
approximately 19:00 hours, and lasted until Day 2, when they were discharged at approximately 11:45 hours.
On arrival at the sleeping facility, subjects rated their subjective feelings and subjective sleepiness. From
19:30 hours until 20:30 hours they performed the first session of laboratory tests, comprising the Word
Learning Test immediate and first delayed recall, the Critical Tracking Task, the Divided Attention Task, the
Psychomotor Vigilance Task, the Stop Signal Task, and the Digit Span forward and backward. Hereafter,
electrodes for polysomnographic recording were attached.
Subjects retired to bed at 23:30 hours. Immediately preceding retiring, subjects in the chronic users
group ingested their own prescribed hypnotic, whereas subjects in the unmedicated insomniacs group and
controls did not ingest medication. Subjects were awakened at 07:30 hours and after arising a light
standardized breakfast was served. At 08:00 hours subjects evaluated sleep quality and duration, and
feelings of daytime sleepiness and alertness. Subsequently, they started the second session of laboratory
tests, comprising the Word Learning Test second delayed recall and recognition, the Critical Tracking Task,
the Divided Attention Task, the Psychomotor Vigilance Task, and the Digit Span forward and backward. At
9:00 hours blood samples were taken from the chronic users to determine serum concentrations of hypnotics
before driving. Thereafter subjects were transported to the Highway Driving Test which they performed
between 09:30 and 10:30 hours. Upon completion subjects rated the mental effort it took to perform this
driving test, and subsequently they conducted the Car-Following Test. Upon completion of this test subjects
returned to the testing facilities for removal of the electrodes and were discharged.
Statistical analysis
The primary parameter of the study was the Standard Deviation of Lateral Position (SDLP, in cm). Driving
and sleep related parameters were compared between the three groups, using a one-way analysis of
variance with Group as between subject factor with three levels (‘
chronic users’, ‘
unmedicated insomniacs’
,
‘
controls’
). Parameters which were assessed in evening and morning session were analyzed using 2x3
Repeated Measures analysis with Time of Day as within subject factor with two levels (evening, morning)
and Group as between subject factor. Significant (p<0.05) main effects or interactions, were further analyzed
using three univariate comparisons between groups for each time of day separately and paired t-tests
between sessions for each group separately. If the model assumptions were violated, a suitable
transformation or nonparametric method was chosen for analysis.
All statistical analyses were done by using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS)
statistical program (version 15.0 for Windows; SPSS, Chicago, IL).
51
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Table 1. Overview of hypnotics and doses used by chronic users, listed in increasing order of expected residual effects.
Indicated are age and gender of the user, duration and frequency of hypnotic use
Hypnotic
Dose (mg)
Gender
Age
Duration of use
(years)
Nights per week
zolpidem
10
female
58
3
4
zolpidem
10
male
59
10
7
midazolam
7.5
male
68
1.5
7
midazolam
7.5
male
64
5
7
midazolam
7.5
male
64
4
7
midazolam
7.5
female
57
3
7
lormetazepam
0.5
male
55
7
6
temazepam
10
female
63
30
4
temazepam
10
male
65
4
4
temazepam
10
female
69
15
7
temazepam
20
female
56
1.5
7
zopiclone
3.75
female
58
3
7
zopiclone
3.75
female
63
15
7
zopiclone
3.75
female
60
2
7
5
female
68
12
7
zopiclone
7.5
male
61
13
7
oxazepam
10
male
68
1.5
7
oxazepam
20
male
62
9
7
oxazepam
50
male
63
1
7
nitrazepam
lormetazepam
2
male
63
10
4
flurazepam
15
female
56
11
7
clonazepam
0.5
female
67
7
7
52
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Table 2. Means (±SD) of pre-study group characteristics
Variable
Chronic users
Unmedicated
insomniacs
(n=20)
(n=22)
Controls
F
†
(n=21)
Sociodemographics
Gender (male:female)
11:11
10:10
13:8
Age (years)
62.1 (4.4)
60.8 (5.9)
61.7 (5.0)
.40
Education (years)
12.3 (3.3)
10.9 (2.7)
13.2 (3.3)
2.67
Average Annual Mileage (1000 km)
8.5 (6.3)
11.9 (11.5)
9.7 (4.2)
.98
Driving License (years)
38.0 (8.3)
39.0 (8.4)
40.6 (5.9)
.64
Sleep
a
Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index
12.6 (3.5)
Groningen Sleep Quality Scale - general
9.4 (3.0)
Sleep Wake Experience List
a
2.4 (1.6)
100.20
a
1.0 (1.5)
126.08
0
16.73
12.6 (2.6)
a
11.1 (1.7)
***
***
‡
Sleep Initiation Problems
Sleep Maintenance Problems
a
a
12
10
a
***
a
13
0
23.26
a
a
14
***
*
Early Morning Awakenings
15
5
0
7.61
Difficulty Waking Up
2
2
0
2.12
Tiredness Upon Waking Up
3
2
0
2.86
Daytime Sleepiness
5
3
1
2.80
Psychological
Symptom Checklist 90-R
Sleeping Problems
Depression
a
a
9.6 (3.1)
10.0 (2.5)
a
52.57
27.1 (10.7)
18.0 (3.0)
7.62
a
13.9 (4.8)
10.8 (1.3)
5.28
7.3 (0.8)
2.42
26.7 (9.7)
Anxiety
15.8 (7.3)
Phobic anxiety
8.9 (4.5)
Psychoneuroticism
***
3.3 (0.7)
a
7.4 (1.0)
a
141.9 (37.1)
137.9 (39.6)
a
***
**
***
103.0 (13.1)
9.37
a
14.2 (1.8)
6.80
a
a
19.1 (5.9)
a
**
Somatization
20.5 (7.9)
Cognitive Insufficiency
16.1 (6.3)
15.8 (8.1)
11.0 (1.8)
4.78
Interpersonal Sensitivity
25.8 (6.5)
24.8 (8.4)
21.7 (5.6)
2.11
Hostility
Beck Depression Inventory
7.4 (1.6)
7.9 (2.8)
a
6.8 (1.3)
a
8.6 (5.0)
*
8.6 (6.6)
1.41
***
2.8 (3.0)
9.38
a
28.1 (7.5)
10.03
a
27.6 (5.8)
9.30
6.8 (2.5)
16.36
State Trait Anxiety Inventory
a
41.6 (11.0)
a
41.9 (13.1)
State Anxiety
35.3 (10.1)
Trait Anxiety
38.4 (10.9)
***
***
Multidimensional Fatigue Inventory
General Fatigue
Physical Fatigue
Mental Fatigue
Reduced Motivation
Reduced Activity
a
11.8 (3.7)
a
10.9 (3.5)
a
11.5 (3.9)
a
a
11.9 (3.7)
6.6 (2.3)
12.23
a
7.0 (2.4)
11.24
10.6 (3.5)
10.8 (3.5)
a
9.8 (3.6)
9.6 (4.2)
a
10.1 (4.2)
10.0 (3.2)
† = degrees of freedom (df) are 2,62, except for State Trait Anxiety Inventory – State: 2,61 and Trait: 2,56;
***
***
***
**
6.7 (2.5)
5.29
7.4 (2.6)
4.03
‡
*
= values for each variable are the
frequencies in each group. Statistical analysis was conducted with the Kruskal-Wallis test, depicted are the Chi-square values;
p<0.01;
***
a
*
= p<0.05;
=
= p<0.001; a = significant difference with control group (p<0.05; post-hoc analysis with Bonferroni correction); b = significant difference
between frequent and in frequent users (p<0.05; post-hoc analysis with Bonferroni correction)
53
**
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Table 3. Sleep diary
Variable
Chronic users
(n = 22)
a
Unmedicated
insomniacs
(n = 20)
a
Controls
F
†
p
(n = 21)
Groningen Sleep Quality Scale
6.0 (2.6)
6.8 (2.0)
1.9 (1.1)
32.76
<.001
Time in Bed (min)
521.0 (61.1)
498.9 (52.7)
495.1 (36.8)
1.53
.226
Sleep Onset Time (min)
43.0 (36.9)
a
Awakenings (#)
0.75 (0.69)
b
Early Morning Awakening (min)
33.1 (42.6)
18.3 (4.1)
5.44
.007
a
0.46 (0.44)
8.50
.001
43.6 (29.8)
1.38 (0.93)
a
50.0 (41.5)
Total Sleep Time (min)
410.9 (73.1)
Sleep Efficiency (%)
79.3 (13.3)
†
a
b
a,b
a
348.5 (71.2)
a
69.2 (11.8)
17.8 (13.8)
4.07
.022
440.3 (36.8)
10.63
<.001
89.2 (6.7)
16.09
<.001
= degrees of freedom (df) for Groningen Sleep Quality Scale: 2,61, for Time in Bed: 2,60, for Sleep Onset Time: 2,59, for Number of Awakenings:
2,58, for Early Morning Awakening: 2,59, for Total Sleep Time: 2,59, and for Sleep Efficiency: 2,59;
(p<0.05; post-hoc analysis with Bonferroni correction);
b
a
= significant difference with control group
= significant difference between frequent and in frequent users (p<0.05; post-hoc analysis
with Bonferroni correction)
54
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Table 4. Mean (±SD) of objective and subjective sleep parameters
Variable
Chronic users
(n = 22)
Unmedicated
insomniacs
(n = 20)
Controls
F
†
p
(n = 21)
Subjective
Groningen Sleep Quality Scale
6.7 (3.9)
a
a
7.8 (3.8)
3.9 (3.5)
6.24
.003
Sleep Onset Time (min)
47.6 (97.9)
67.9 (73.0)
33.0 (36.8)
1.19
.321
Awakenings (#)
2.1 (1.6)
3.0 (2.5)
1.8 (1.5)
1.97
.149
Early Morning Awakening (min)
49.1 (57.5)
67.4 (62.0)
26.4 (34.7)
3.23
.046
Total Sleep Time (min)
351 (101)
302 (94)
377 (111)
2.92
.062
26.4 (9.4)
19.4 (13.4)
19.2 (14.6)
2.20
.120
a
Polysomnographic parameters
Continuity
Sleep Onset Time (min)
Wake After Sleep Onset (min)
76.5 (65.0)
73.1 (39.4)
55.2 (35.7)
1.16
.321
Awakenings (#)
7.9 (4.1)
10.2 (4.3)
7.5 (4.5)
2.37
.102
Total Sleep Time (min)
383 (35)
389 (46)
408 (40)
2.15
.125
Sleep Efficiency (%)
79.6 (8.8)
80.8 (9.5)
84.6 (8.1)
1.82
.170
Architecture
†
Stage 1 Sleep (% of Total Sleep Time)
6.6 (2.9)
6.9 (2.4)
6.1 (3.0)
0.50
.612
Stage 2 Sleep (% of Total Sleep Time)
56.6 (6.1)
54.5 (6.5)
56.0 (9.0)
0.44
.644
Stage SWS Sleep (% of Total Sleep Time)
18.4 (8.5)
18.5 (5.9)
16.5 (5.5)
0.58
.566
Stage REM Sleep (% of Total Sleep Time)
18.4 (4.3)
20.1 (5.8)
21.4 (6.0)
1.61
.209
= degrees of freedom (df) for all parameters: 2,61;
a
= significant difference with control group (p<0.05; post-hoc analysis with Bonferroni
b
correction); = significant difference between frequent and in frequent users (p<0.05; post-hoc analysis with Bonferroni correction)
55
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Table 5. Overview of hypnotics and doses used by chronic users, listed in increasing order of expected residual effects.
Indicated are recommended therapeutic dose, drug’s half-life, expected residual effects, each users’Standard Deviation
of Lateral Position (SDLP) scores, associated drug serum concentrations age and referral concentrations
Hypnotic
Dos
e
(mg)
Recommend
ed
therapeutic
dose for
adults (mg)
(for elderly)
t1/2 (hours)
zolpidem
10
10 (5)
zolpidem
10
10 (5)
a
Residual
effects
8-12 hrs
post
c
dose
SDLP
(in
cm)
Serum
concentratio
n (ng/mL) at
9.5 hrs post
dose
1.9 ± 0.2
unlikely
12.5
10.2
5.8 (11 hrs; plasma; 20 mg)
1.9 ± 0.2
unlikely
15.6
53.1
5.8 (11 hrs; plasma; 20 mg)
Concentration at 8-11 hrs
d
post- dose (ng/mL)
midazolam
7.5
7.5 (7.5)
1.9 ± 0.6
unlikely
13.2
9.5
1.5 (8 hrs; plasma; 7.5 mg)
midazolam
7.5
7.5 (7.5)
1.9 ± 0.6
unlikely
16.8
8.1
1.5 (8 hrs; plasma; 7.5 mg)
midazolam
7.5
7.5 (7.5)
1.9 ± 0.6
unlikely
16.3
52.3
1.5 (8 hrs; plasma; 7.5 mg)
midazolam
7.5
7.5 (7.5)
1.9 ± 0.6
unlikely
19.9
ND
1.5 (8 hrs; plasma; 7.5 mg)
lormetazepa
m
0.5
1 (0.5)
10 ± 2.5
unlikely
20.4
2.6
4.3 (11 hrs; serum; 1 mg)
temazepam
10
20 (10)
11
unlikely
14.7
191.7
160 (12 hrs; serum; 20 mg)
temazepam
10
20 (10)
11
unlikely
27.6
85.4
160 (12 hrs; serum; 20 mg)
temazepam
10
20 (10)
11
unlikely
24.0
198.7
160 (12 hrs; serum; 20 mg)
20
20 (10)
11
unlikely
13.3
137.6
160 (12 hrs; serum; 20 mg)
zopiclone
temazepam
3.75
7.5 (3.75)
5
unlikely
12.3
4.6
6.2 (8 hrs; plasma; 3.75 mg)
zopiclone
3.75
7.5 (3.75)
5
unlikely
12.8
6.5
6.2 (8 hrs; plasma; 3.75 mg)
zopiclone
3.75
7.5 (3.75)
5
unlikely
18.9
9.3
6.2 (8 hrs; plasma; 3.75 mg)
5
5 (5)
26 ± 3
minor
21.6
58.0
34 (8 hrs; serum; 5 mg)
7.5
7.5 (3.75)
5
moderate
17.0
7.8
14.1 (8 hrs; plasma; 7.5 mg)
nitrazepam
zopiclone
oxazepam
10
20 (10)
8 ± 2.4
NA
16.5
37.6
231 (12.15 hrs; serum; 50 mg)
oxazepam
20
20 (10)
8 ± 2.4
NA
15.4
122.9
231 (12.15 hrs; serum; 50 mg)
oxazepam
50
20 (10)
8 ± 2.4
moderate
25.2
360.1
231 (12.15 hrs; serum; 50 mg)
lormetazepa
m
2
1 (0.5)
10 ± 2.5
moderate
16.0
7.3
7.1 (11 hrs; serum; 2 mg)
flurazepam
15
30 (15)
1-2 (74 ±
b
24)
moderate
14.1
0.0
0.0 (>2 hrs; whole blood; 30 mg)
clonazepam
0.5
1 (1)
19-60
NA
18.5
ND
a
Source Vermeeren (2004)5, and Riss et al. (2008)59;
prescribed dose ;
d
b
half-life of active metabolite between brackets;
5.5 (8 hrs; plasma; 1 mg)
c
Source Vermeeren (2004)5 following
Source for zolpidem Bensimon et al. (1990)60 , for midazolam Bornemann et al. (1985)61, for lormetazepam Brookhuis et al.
(1990)62, for temazepam Tedeschi et al. (1985)63, for zopiclone Billiard et al. (1987)64, for nitrazepam Lahtinen et al. (1978)65, for oxazepam Volkerts
et al. (1992)66, for flurazepam Kaplan et al. (1973)67, and for clonazepam Wildin et al. (1990)68; ND = blood sample not drawn; NA = information not
available
56
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Table 6. Mean (±SD) of driving performance parameters
Variable
Chronic users
(n = 22)
Unmedicated
insomniacs
(n = 20)
Controls
(n = 21)
†
F
p
Highway Driving Test
Standard Deviation of Lateral Position (cm)
17.4 (4.3)
17.7 (2.9)
16.8 (2.7)
0.38
.688
Standard Deviation of Speed (km/h)
2.11 (0.5)
2.34 (0.7)
2.18 (0.7)
0.76
.471
Car Following Test
Reaction time (sec)
3.55 (1.57)
3.32 (1.40)
3.06 (1.01)
0.67
.518
Headway
1.14 (0.13)
1.21 (0.25)
1.15 (0.12)
0.94
.397
67.0 (11.2)
67.9 (9.7)
65.0 (13.9)
0.34
.713
Subjective Evaluation of Driving Test
Subjective Driving Quality (mm)
†
Apparent Sedation (mm)
11.9 (11.4)
12.2 (14.9)
9.1 (10.2)
0.42
.657
Mental Effort (mm)
30.3 (22.6)
35.7 (24.9)
20.3 (14.1)
2.77
.071
= degrees of freedom (df) for Highway Driving Test: 2,61, for Car Following test: 2,59, and for Subjective Evaluations: 2,62
57
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Table 7. Mean (±SD) of psychomotor and cognitive performance parameters and analysis of main effect of Group
Variable
Chronic
users
(n = 22)
Unmedicated
insomniacs
(n = 20)
Controls
(n = 21)
F
†,a
p
Critical Tracking Task
Average lambda (in rad/sec) evening
3.34 (0.72)
3.23 (0.60)
3.03 (0.49)
Average lambda (in rad/sec) morning
3.29 (0.70)
3.21 (0.72)
3.02 (0.49)
14.1 (5.2)
14.4 (4.4)
17.4 (5.2)
0.06
.945
0.75
.478
2.56
.086
Divided Attention Task
Tracking subtask: Average Error (in mm) evening
b
Tracking subtask: Average Error (in mm) morning
15.5 (5.9)
14.9 (4.2)
18.7 (5.2)
Detection subtask: Reaction Time (in msec) evening
1924 (328)
1974 (299)
1973 (335)
Detection subtask: Reaction Time (in msec) morning
2030 (344)
1898 (288)
1920 (338)
Go Reaction Tim e (in msec)
423 (63)
427 (77)
422 (50)
0.03
.972
Stop Signal Reaction Time (in msec)
179 (35)
178 (31)
181 (35)
0.05
.953
269 (39)
257 (32)
264 (24)
1.33
.273
0.50
.610
0.73
.486
Stop Signal Task
Psychomotor Vigilance Task
Average Reaction Time (in msec) evening
b
Average Reaction Time (in msec) morning
264 (39)
254 (34)
251 (22)
Median Reaction Time (in msec) evening
254 (34)
244 (31)
247 (23)
Median Reaction Time (in msec) morning
252 (35)
242 (30)
241 (21)
Lapses (>500 msec) evening
1.2 (2.0)
1.3 (1.1)
1.0 (1.3)
Lapses (>500 msec) morning
1.3 (2.1)
0.7 (1.0)
0.7 (1.2)
Word Learning Task
Immediate Total Recall Score
46.3 (12.3)
46.8 (9.4)
49.4 (9.0)
0.53
.590
Delayed Recall Score evening
7.8 (3.5)
8.4 (2.9)
8.9 (3.4)
0.14
.870
b
b
b
Delayed Recall Score morning
6.5 (2.5)
7.0 (3.4)
7.3 (3.2)
Recognition Score
24.8 (4.4)
24.6 (3.0)
25.7 (3.4)
0.49
.615
Recognition Reaction Time (in msec)
924 (189)
892 (192)
883 (141)
0.33
.722
1.43
.248
0.04
.965
1.05
.356
0.56
.576
0.62
.542
0.26
.770
Digit Span
Forward Score evening
3.6 (1.3)
3.8 (1.1)
3.9 (1.2)
Forward Score morning
3.5 (0.9)
4.0 (1.1)
4.4 (1.1)
Backward Score evening
3.5 (1.3)
3.9 (1.4)
4.3 (1.1)
3.8 (1.4)
4.2 (1.1)
73.8 (13.2)
81.4 (13.4)
c
Backward Score morning
3.3 (1.0)
Subjective Evaluations of Feelings
c
Alertness evening
†
65.9 (16.7)
Alertness morning
64.3 (18.6)
69.4 (14.0)
74.4 (14.6)
Contentedness evening
72.5 (21.2)
78.3 (13.0)
82.7 (12.6)
Contentedness morning
74.8 (19.0)
76.3 (12.1)
81.1 (13.6)
Calmness evening
69.8 (22.5)
74.9 (14.0)
82.4 (13.9)
Calmness morning
72.7 (17.3)
73.0 (13.9)
79.8 (14.4)
Karolinska Sleepiness Scale evening
3.9 (1.7)
3.9 (1.7)
3.0 (0.8)
Karolinska Sleepiness Scale morning
4.6 (1.5)
4.3 (1.7)
3.8 (1.3)
b
b
= degrees of freedom (df) for all parameters: 2,60, except for Psychomotor Vigilance Task: 2,59 and for Stop Signal Task: 2,62; a = test for Session
by Group interaction, except for the Stop Signal Task which was only assessed in the evening session;
b
= significant Session effect p<0.05;
significant difference from control group p<0.05
58
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
c
=
Results
Pre-study group characteristics
Table 2 illustrates the descriptive variables of sociodemographic, sleep and psychological data. One-way
ANOVA showed that there were no differences between the groups in years of age, years of education,
average annual mileage and years of possession of a driving license.
Evaluation of sleep at home differed significantly between groups. Sleep quality was poorer in both
insomnia groups as compared to controls, as indicated by significantly higher scores on the PSQI, GSQSgeneral and the sleep subscale of the SCL90-R (p<0.001). There were no differences in mean PSQI, GSQS
and SCL90-R sleep scores between the insomnia groups. Sleep complaints were most frequently classified
as sleep initiation and sleep maintenance problems, using the SWEL, with similar prevalences in both
groups of insomniacs. Early morning awakening was more frequent in chronic users compared to
unmedicated insomniacs, but the difference was not significant. None of the controls reported problems with
sleep onset, sleep maintenance or early awakening.
The two-week sleep diary showed that complaints of disturbed sleep as measured by the GSQSspecific were significantly increased in both insomnia groups as compared to controls (table 3).
Subjective estimates of sleep times, averaged over two weeks, showed that sleep was worst in the
unmedicated insomniacs. Compared with the chronic users and the healthy, good sleepers, the unmedicated
insomniacs reported significantly more nocturnal awakenings (p<0.001), shorter total sleep time (p<0.001),
and reduced sleep efficiency (p<0.001). Compared to controls they also reported significantly longer sleep
onset times (p<0.018) and earlier awakening in the morning (p <0.018). Chronic users also reported
significantly longer sleep onset times than controls (p<0.018), and reduced sleep efficiency (p<0.017). The
difference from controls in total sleep time and early morning awakenings did not reach significance.
Both insomnia groups scored significantly higher than controls on rating scales of depression (BDI:
p<0.001, SCL90-R: p<0.004), without significant differences between the chronic users and unmedicated
insomniacs. Anxiety was also increased in insomniacs as compared to controls, as shown by significantly
higher scores on both subscales of the STAI in both groups (STAI state: chronic users p=0.044 and
unmedicated insomniacs p<0.001; STAI trait: chronic users p=0.005 and unmedicated insomniacs p<0.001).
A significant difference on the SCL90-R anxiety scale was found between the unmedicated insomniacs and
controls (p=0.006), but not between the chronic users and controls. There were no significant differences in
anxiety ratings between the insomnia groups.
In addition to sleeping problems, depression and anxiety, the SCL90-R showed that insomnia
patients scored higher than controls on the subscales for somatization (chronic users p=0.002 and
unmedicated insomniacs p=0.027), cognitive insufficiency (chronic users p=0.021 and unmedicated
insomniacs p=0.041), and psychoneuroticism (chronic users p=0.001 and unmedicated insomniacs
p=0.003). The Fatigue Inventory showed that both insomnia groups reported suffering significantly more
from general fatigue (both p<0.001), physical fatigue (both p<0.001) and mental fatigue (chronic users
p<0.001 and unmedicated insomniacs p=0.002) when compared to the control group. Both groups reported
significant reductions in motivation (chronic users p=0.015 and unmedicated insomniacs p=0.027), and the
chronic users also in activity (p=0.040). No differences between the insomnia groups were found on any of
59
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
these scales.
Sleep before driving
Polysomnography showed no differences between the three groups on any of the sleep parameters
recorded during sleep in the laboratory the night before driving (table 4).
In contrast, subjective evaluations of sleep that night differed significantly between groups as measured by
number of complaints in the GSQS-specific (p<0.003) and estimates of early morning awakening (p<0.046).
Both insomnia groups reported significantly more sleep complaints than the control group (chronic users:
p<0.043; unmedicated insomniacs: p<0.004), and the unmedicated insomniacs reported to awake
significantly earlier than the controls (p<0.042). There were no significant group differences in subjective
estimates of sleep onset time, number of awakenings and total sleep time.
Driving, cognitive and psychomotor performance
Table 5 shows individual serum concentrations of hypnotics used by chronic users and their corresponding
SDLP scores. Analysis showed that driving performance as measured by the highway driving test and the
car-following test did not reveal significant differences between the three groups (table 6).
29
Standard Deviation of Lateral Position (SDLP, in cm)
27
25
23
21
19
17
15
13
11
Chronic
Unmedicated
Controls
users
insomniacs
Figure 1. Mean (±SD) and individual SDLP scores for each group separately. Open circles ( ) indicate SDLP scores of
subjects using no hypnotic or a hypnotic that produces no or minor residual effects; closed circles ( ) indicate SDLP
scores of subjects using hypnotics likely to produce moderate or severe residual effects
The primary performance parameter, SDLP in the highway driving test, was on average normal and similar
for unmedicated insomniacs (17.7 ±2.9 cm), chronic users (17.4 ±4.3 cm) and controls (16.8 ±2.7 cm) (figure
1)
Performance in the critical tracking test, divided attention task and the stop signal task showed no significant
60
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
differences between the groups. There was, however, a significant overall group difference in the Digit Span
Backward (F2,60=3.93, p<0.025). Pairwise comparison revealed significantly lower scores in the chronic users
as compared to the controls (p<0.020). There were no significant differences between the unmedicated
insomniacs and the controls, and between the insomnia groups (table 7).
Comparisons between performance in the evening and morning sessions showed significant Time of
Day differences in the reaction times in the PVT and delayed recall in the word learning test. The latter was
as expected because the interval between learning and delayed recall increased from evening to morning
sessions. Average reaction time in the PVT was significantly faster in the morning as compared to the
evening in controls (F1,59=8.57, p<0.005), but not in the insomnia groups.
Performance in the critical tracking test, divided attention task and the stop signal task showed no
significant differences between times of day (evening vs. morning).
Subjective rating scales
Analysis of the subjective evaluation of mood revealed an overall significant group difference in feelings of
alertness (F2,60=4.44, p<0.016). Post-hoc comparisons showed that the chronic users felt significantly less
alert in the morning than the controls (p<0.013). In addition, the controls felt more alert in the evening when
compared to the morning (p<0.004).
Scores on the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale were not different between the three groups. There was,
however, a significant between the evening and morning evaluation (F 1,60=7.25, p<0.009). The control group
reported significantly more feelings of sleepiness in the morning as compared to the evening (p<0.006).
Discussion
The present experimental study is the first directly comparing driving performance between insomnia
patients who chronically use hypnotics, insomnia patients who do not or infrequently use hypnotics and
healthy, good sleepers. Results show that driving, as measured by a standardized highway driving test and a
car-following test, is not impaired in insomniacs, irrespective of the use of hypnotics. In addition, the present
study shows that driving related psychomotor and cognitive performance is virtually similar between
insomnia patients and healthy, good sleepers. Only working memory, as measured by the Digit Span
backward, appeared to be significantly worse in the chronic users compared to the healthy controls. No
group differences were found in verbal memory, divided attention, psychomotor function and inhibitory
control.
Results of the study corroborate previous findings by Vignola et al. showing an absence of
neuropsychological deficits in both chronic users of hypnotics and unmedicated insomniacs.10 These
investigators compared sleep and performance between 20 chronic users of benzodiazepine hypnotics, 20
unmedicated insomniacs and 20 good sleepers, using a battery of neuropsychological tests,
polysomnography and subjective rating scales. Aside from lower scores in the Digit Span forward test, the
study revealed no significant differences in performance between treated insomniacs, untreated insomniacs
and healthy, good sleepers. Vignola et al. suggested that the absence of cognitive impairment in their study
may have been due to methodological limitations. The tests used were of short duration and demanded low
effort (e.g. digit symbol substitution test and purdue pegboard test). Consequently, insomnia patients may
61
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
have been able to exert enough effort for a short period to complete the tests successfully. In the present
study, subjects performed a 1-hour standardized highway driving test and a 25-minutes car following test.
The prolonged attentional demands of these tasks were expected to reveal possible performance deficits in
insomnia patients, which were not found with tasks of short duration. Nevertheless, there were no indications
of deterioration in driving performance in insomnia patients, irrespective of use of hypnotics.
These results can be interpreted in two ways. First, it can be argued that daytime performance was
unaffected because sleep in the insomnia groups was not significantly different from controls as shown by
polysomnographic data. This may be due to sleeping in a different environment, as home-based
polysomnographic recordings have shown that insomnia patients’sleep appears more disturbed when they
sleep at home than when they sleep at the laboratory.
47
Indeed, in the present study, subjective sleep
evaluations showed that sleep quality at the laboratory improved in both insomnia groups compared to sleep
at home. In addition, subjective sleep quality for the healthy controls was worse during laboratory sleep than
at home. These changes in sleep quality may have diminished differences between the groups. On the other
hand, the failure to find objective sleep disturbances in the insomnia groups was expected. It is known that
discrepancies between subjective and objective sleep parameters are characteristic in a majority of primary
insomnia patients.e.g.
10, 48, 49
Assuming that patients’sleep is somehow disturbed, it is suggested that the
current standards of sleep analysis may not be adequate for distinguishing insomnia from healthy,
undisturbed sleep.50 A possible solution may be found in spectral analysis of the sleep microstructure,
dissociating characteristic electroencephalographic activities.
A second explanation for the absence of impairment in driving performance may be that the driving
tests were not very demanding and patients were therefore able to complete the tasks normally by investing
a little more effort. Driving is a well practiced and highly automated skill 51 and may not require such high
demands in particularly experienced drivers. All participants in the present study had ample driving
experience and may not have had any difficulties in performing the test.
Judging from the low scores on the mental effort scale this assumption seems to be confirmed.
Scores on the scale can range from 0 to 150. The average scores in the present study were 30.3 for the
chronic users, 35.7 for the unmedicated insomnia patients and 20.3 for the healthy controls, which
corresponds to low or little effort. In a study comparing cognitive performance between patients with
seasonal allergic rhinitis and healthy controls, subjects evaluated the mental effort they had to put in a 45minutes Mackworth clock vigilance test considerably higher.52 Mental effort scores were around 90 for both
groups, indicating substantially higher demands of that test as compared with the highway driving test. Still,
the insomniacs, in particular the chronic users group, evaluated the degree of mental effort they had to put in
the driving test tentatively higher than the healthy controls. Although not reaching statistical significance, this
supports the idea that the insomnia patients compensated possible performance difficulties by increasing
their effort. Establishing performance impairment in insomnia may require more demanding tasks.53
In addition to the findings that driving is not affected in insomnia patients, results of the present study
provided no evidence that driving is impaired in patients chronically using hypnotics. One explanation may
be related to tolerance. The absence of impairment, combined with the still present subjective sleep
complaints suggests the development of, at least partial, tolerance to both therapeutic and residual effects of
hypnotics. With respect to the residual effects, the results are partly supported by epidemiological data,17
showing that prolonged use of hypnotics is associated with a lowered risk of becoming involved in a car
62
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
accident when compared with initial use of hypnotics. Nevertheless, the risk of injurious traffic accidents after
chronic use of hypnotics remained twice as high in long-term hypnotic users in comparison to healthy,
unmedicated drivers.
An alternative explanation for the absence of residual effects on driving in the present study may be
related to the wide variety of hypnotic drugs and doses used by the chronic users. Most importantly, the
majority of hypnotics used are considered unlikely to produce residual effects.
5
Consequently, the
differences in degree of residual effects may explain the relatively high variability of performance in this
group as compared to controls. Averaging may have masked any detectable impairment which is only
associated with hypnotics belonging to category II or III, i.e. drugs and doses judged likely to produce
moderate or severe impairment.54, 55 This was confirmed by post hoc inspection of the average SDLP scores
from the category I users and the category II users, showing that performance of the former group was better
than that of the latter group. Mean (±SD) SDLP scores were 16.8 (4.7) cm and 18.3 (3.5) cm respectively.
Future research in patients chronically using the same hypnotic is needed to shed more light on this issue.
The present study aimed, however, to evaluate driving performance in a representative, non-selective study
sample of insomnia patients chronically using hypnotics.
In contrast to driving, chronic use of hypnotics did seem to impair memory performance. Chronic
users performed significantly worse on the Digit Span Backward as compared to the controls. This was
supported by other parameters in memory tests showing a tentative non-significant pattern of cognitive
decline in the chronic users. On average, performance in the Word Learning Test and the Digit Span
Forward was worse in chronic users than in unmedicated insomniacs and controls. This does not seem to be
the result from general sedation following administration of a hypnotic as no impairment was found in the
psychomotor tests. Rather, the pattern suggests a general cognitive deterioration due to long-term use,
confirming previous findings as reviewed by Barker et al.56
Finally, it may be questioned whether the study had sufficient power to detect differences in driving
performance between insomnia patients and controls. We do not believe this is the case. First of all, the
mean difference in SDLP between unmedicated patients and controls was small (i.e. less than 1 cm), and
not considered clinically relevant. The minimum clinically relevant difference in SDLP is 2.4 cm which
corresponds to the effect of alcohol when blood alcohol concentrations are at the legal limit for driving in
most countries. Secondly, previous studies assessing driving performance in other patients groups, using the
same standardized driving test, have shown that the method is sufficiently sensitive to detect significant
impairment in small samples of patients with chronic nonmalignant pain57 and depressed patients receiving
58
long-term antidepressant treatment.
It seems therefore that the effects of insomnia are relatively small and
less debilitating than the effects of pain and depression.
To conclude, results of the present study indicate that driving performance is not impaired in patients
suffering from insomnia, irrespective of use of hypnotics. Chronic users of hypnotics perform worse than
healthy controls on the Digit Span backward. However, other driving related psychomotor and cognitive
performance appears not to be affected in medicated and unmedicated insomnia patients. Therefore, studies
investigating residual effects of hypnotics on driving performance in healthy volunteers are not expected to
yield different results than in insomnia patients.
Acknowledgements
63
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
This study is financially supported by the integrated project Driving Under the Influence of Drug, Alcohol and
th
Medicine (DRUID), which is part of the European Union’
s 6 Framework Programme. The article reflects
only the authors’ view. The European Community is not liable for any use that may be made of the
information contained therein. The authors like to express their gratitude to Gwenda Engels, Nicky van
Gennip, Jolien Gooijers, Liene Ketelslegers, Jasmijn Kromhout, Loes van Langen, Anita van Oers, Elmy
Theuniszen, Natalie Valle Guzman, Floor van de Water and Tim Weysen for the assistance in data
collection; Renilde van den Bossche for the polysomnographic analyses; Cees van Leeuwen for the medical
supervision; Henk Brauers, Willy Jeurissen and Jo Gorissen for ensuring the safety of the subjects during
driving; and Irma Brauers for the logistic work.
References
1.
Ohayon MM. Epidemiology of insomnia: what we know and what we still need to learn. Sleep Med
Rev. 2002;6:97-111.
2.
Morin CM, Leblanc M, Daley M, et al. Epidemiology of insomnia: Prevalence, self-help treatments,
consultations, and determinants of help-seeking behaviors. Sleep Med. 2006;7:123-30.
3.
Morin AK, Jarvis CI, Lynch AM. Therapeutic options for sleep-maintenance and sleep-onset
insomnia. Pharmacother. 2007;27:89-110.
4.
Verster JC, Volkerts ER, Olivier B, et al. Zolpidem and traffic safety - the importance of treatment
compliance. Curr Drug Safe. 2007;2:220-6.
5.
Vermeeren A. Residual effects of hypnotics: epidemiology and clinical implications. CNS Drugs.
2004;18:297-328.
6.
Chambers MJ, Keller B. Alert insomniacs: Are they really sleep deprived? Clin Psychol Rev.
1993;13:649-66.
7.
Varkevisser M, Kerkhof GA. Chronic insomnia and performance in a 24-h constant routine study. J
Sleep Res. 2005;14:49-59.
8.
Riedel BW, Lichstein KL. Insomnia and daytime functioning. Sleep Med Rev. 2000;4:277-98.
9.
Fulda S, Schulz H. Cognitive dysfunction in sleep disorders. Sleep Med Review. 2001;5:423-45.
10.
Vignola A, Lamoureux C, Bastien CH, et al. Effects of chronic insomnia and use of benzodiazepines
on daytime performance in older adults. J Gerontol. 2000;55:P54-62.
11.
Drummond SPA, Smith MT, Orff HJ, et al. Functional imaging of the sleeping brain: review of
findings and implications for the study of insomnia. Sleep Med Rev. 2004;8:227-42.
12.
Ashton H. The diagnosis and management of benzodiazepine dependence. Curr Opin Psychiat.
2005;18:249-55.
13.
Paterniti S, Dufouil C, Alperovitch A. Long-term benzodiazepine use and cognitive decline in the
elderly: the Epidemiology of Vascular Aging Study. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 2002;22:285-93.
14.
Ray WA, Fought RL, Decker MD. Psychoactive drugs and the risk of injurious motor vehicle crashes
in elderly drivers. Am J Epidemiol. 1992;136:873-83.
15.
Hemmelgarn B, Suissa S, Huang A, et al. Benzodiazepine use and the risk of motor vehicle crash in
the elderly. JAMA. 1997;278:27-31.
64
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
16.
Barbone F, McMahon AD, Davey PG, et al. Association of road-traffic accidents with benzodiazepine
use. Lancet. 1998;352:1331-6.
17.
Neutel
I.
Benzodiazepine-related
traffic
accidents in
young
and
elderly
drivers.
Hum
Psychopharmacol. 1998;13:S115-S23.
18.
Dubois S, Bedard M, Weaver B. The impact of benzodiazepines on safe driving. Traff Inj Prev.
2008;9:404-13.
19.
Metsemakers JFM, Höppener P, Knottnerus JA, et al. Computerized health information in the
Netherlands: a registration network of family practices. Br J Gen Pract. 1992;42:102-6.
20.
American Psychiatric Association. DSM-IV: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4
ed. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Press; 1994.
21.
Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, 3rd, Monk TH, et al. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument
for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiat Res. 1989;28:193-213.
22.
Van Diest R, Milius H, Markusse R, et al. The Sleep-Wake Experience List. Tijdschr Soc
Gezondheidsz. 1989;67:343-7.
23.
Mulder-Hajonides van der Meulen WREH. Measurement of subjective sleep quality. Amsterdam:
Elsevier; 1981.
24.
Derogatis LR. The SCL-90-R: Administration, Scoring and Procedures Manual II. Baltimore:
Baltimore Clinical Psychometric Research; 1983.
25.
Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, et al. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiat.
1961;4:561-71.
26.
Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene RE, et al. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Form
Y). Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc.; 1983.
27.
Smets EM, Garssen B, Bonke B, et al. The Multidimensional Fatigue Inventory (MFI) psychometric
qualities of an instrument to assess fatigue. J Psychosom Res. 1995;39:315-25.
28.
Iber C, Ancoli-Israel S, Chesson A, et al. The AASM manual for the scoring of sleep ans associated
events: rules, terminology and technical specifications. Westchester, Ill 2007.
29.
O'Hanlon JF. Driving performance under the influence of drugs: rationale for, and application of, a
new test. Br J Clin Pharmacol. 1984;18:121s-9s.
30.
Brookhuis KA, De Waard D, Mulder LJ. Measuring driving performance by car-following in traffic.
Ergonomics. 1994;37:427-34.
31.
Ramaekers JG, O'Hanlon JF. Acrivastine, terfenadine and diphenhydramine effects on driving
performance as a function of dose and time after dosing. Eur J Clin Pharmacol. 1994;47:261-6.
32.
Vermeeren A, O'Hanlon JF, Declerck AC, et al. Acute effects of zolpidem and flunitrazepam on
sleep, memory and driving performance, compared to those of partial sleep deprivation and placebo.
Acta Ther. 1995;21:47-64.
33.
Vermeeren A, Danjou PE, O'Hanlon JF. Residual effects of evening and middle-of-the-night
administration of zaleplon 10 and 20 mg on memory and actual driving performance. Hum
Psychopharmacol. 1998;13:S98-S107.
34.
Vermeeren A, Riedel WJ, van Boxtel MP, et al. Differential residual effects of zaleplon and zopiclone
on actual driving: a comparison with a low dose of alcohol. Sleep. 2002;25:224-31.
35.
65
Verster JC, Volkerts ER, Schreuder AH, et al. Residual effects of middle-of-the-night administration
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
of zaleplon and zolpidem on driving ability, memory functions, and psychomotor performance. J Clin
Psychopharmacol. 2002;22:576-83.
36.
Leufkens TRM, Lund JS, Vermeeren A. Highway driving performance and cognitive functioning the
morning after bedtime and middle-of-the-night use of gaboxadol, zopiclone and zolpidem. J Sleep
Res. 2009;18:387-96.
37.
Leufkens TRM, Vermeeren A. Highway driving in elderly the morning after bedtime use of hypnotics:
a comparison between temazepam 20 mg, zopiclone 7.5 mg and placebo. J Clin Psychopharmacol.
2009;29:432-8.
38.
Rey A. L'examen clinique en psychologie. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France; 1964.
39.
Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale - Revised manual. New York: Psychological
Corporation; 1981.
40.
Jex HR, McDonnell JD, Phatak AV. A "critical" tracking task for man-machine research related to the
operator's effective delay time. I. Theory and experiments with a first-order divergent controlled
element. NASA CR-616 1966.
41.
Moskowitz
H.
Proceedings:
Psychological
tests
and
drugs.
Pharmakopsychiat
Neuro
Psychopharmakol. 1973;6:114-26.
42.
Logan GD, Cowan WB, Davis KA. On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time
responses: a model and a method. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. Apr 1984;10:276-91.
43.
Dinges DF, Powell JW. Microcomputer analyses of performance on a portable, simple visual RT task
during sustained operations. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comp. 1985;17:652-5.
44.
Bond A, Lader M. The use of analogue scales in rating subjective feelings. Br J Med Psychol.
1974;47:211-8.
45.
Akerstedt T, Gillberg M. Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. Int J Neurosci.
1990;52:29-37.
46.
Zijlstra FRH. Efficiency in work behavior. A design approach for modern tools. [PhD thesis]. Delft,
Delft University of Technology; 1993.
47.
Edinger JD, Fins AI, Sullivan RJ, Jr., et al. Do our methods lead to insomniacs' madness?: Daytime
testing after laboratory and home-based polysomnographic studies. Sleep. 1997;20:1127-34.
48.
Carskadon MA, Dement WC, Mitler MM, et al. Sel-reports versus sleep laboratory findings in 122
drug-free subjects with complaints of chronic insomnia. Am J Psychiat. 1976;133:1382-8.
49.
Orff HJ, Drummond SPA, Nowakowski S, et al. Discrepancy between subjective symptomatology
and objectiv neuropsychological performance in insomnia. Sleep. 2007;30:1205-11.
50.
Bastien CH, LeBlanc M, Carrier J, et al. Sleep EEG power spectra, insomnia, and chronic use of
benzodiazepines. Sleep. 2003;26:313-7.
51.
Brouwer WH. Attention and driving: a cognitive neuropsychological approach. In: Leclercq M,
Zimmermann P, eds. Applied Neuropsychology of Attention. Hove, UK: Routledge; 2002:230 - 54.
52.
Hartgerink-Lutgens I, Vermeeren A, Vuurman E, et al. Disturbed cognitive functions after nasal
provocation in patients with seasonal allergic rhinitis. Clin Exp Allergy. 2009;39:500-8.
53.
Altena E, Van der Werf YD, Strijers RLM, et al. Sleep loss affects vigilance: Effects of chronic
insomnia and sleep therapy. J Sleep Res. 2008;17:335-43.
54.
66
Wolschrijn H, De Gier JJ, De Smet PAGM. Drugs and driving: a new categorization system for drugs
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
affecting psychomotor performance. Maastricht: Institute for drugs, safety and behaviour, University
of Limburg; 1991. IVGV 91.
55.
De Gier JJ, Alvarez FJ, Mercier-Guyon C, et al. Prescribing and dispensing guidelines for medicinal
drugs affecting driving performance. In: Verster JC, Pandi-Perumal SR, Ramaekers JG, et al., eds.
Drugs, Driving and Traffic Safety. Basel, Switzerland: Birkhäuser Verlag; 2009:121-34.
56.
Barker MJ, Greenwood KM, Jackson M, et al. Cognitive effects of long-term benzodiazepine use: a
meta-analysis. CNS Drugs. 2004;18:37-48.
57.
Veldhuijzen DS, Van Wijck AJM, Wille F, et al. Effect of chronic nonmalignant pain on highway
driving performance. Pain. 2006;122:28-35.
58.
Wingen M, Ramaekers JG, Schmitt JA. Driving impairment in depressed patients receiving long-term
antidepressant treatment. Psychopharmacol. 2006;188:84-91.
59.
Riss J, Cloyd J, Gates J, et al. Benzodiazepines in epilepsy: pharmacology and pharmacokinetics.
Acta Neurol Scand. 2008;118:69-86.
60.
Bensimon G, Foret J, Warot D, et al. Daytime wakefulness following a bedtime oral dose of zolpidem
20 mg, flunitrazepam 2 mg and placebo. Br J Clin Pharmacol. 1990;30:463-9.
61.
Bornemann LD, Min BH, Crews T, et al. Dose dependent pharmacokinetics of midazolam. Eur J Clin
Pharmacol. 1985;29:91-5.
62.
Brookhuis KA, Volkerts ER, O'Hanlon JF. Repeated dose effects of lormetazepam and flurazepam
upon driving performance. Eur J Clin Pharmacol. 1990;39:83-7.
63.
Tedeschi G, Griffiths AN, Smith AT, et al. The effect of repeated doses of temazepam and
nitrazepam on human psychomotor performance. Br J Clin Pharmacol. 1985;20:361-7.
64.
Billiard M, Besset A, de Lustrac C, et al. Dose-response effects of zopiclone on night sleep and on
nighttime and daytime functioning. Sleep. 1987;10:27-34.
65.
Lahtinen U, Lahtinen A, Pekkola P. The effect of nitrazepam on manual skill, grip strength, and
reaction time with special reference to subjective evaluation of effects on sleep. Acta Pharmacol
Toxicol. 1978;42:130-4.
66.
Volkerts ER, Van Laar MW, Van Willigenburg APPP, et al. A comparative study of on-the-road and
simulated driving performances after nocturnal treatment with lormetazepam 1 mg and oxazepam 50
mg. Hum Psychopharmacol. 1992;7:297-309.
67.
Kaplan S, deSilva JAF, Jack ML, et al. Blood level profile in man following chronic oral administration
of flurazepam hydrochloride. J Pharm Sc. 1973;62:1932-5.
68.
Wildin JD, Pleuvry BJ, Mawer GE, et al. Respiratory and sedative effects of clobazam and
clonazepam in volunteers. Br J Clin Pharmacol. 1990;29:169-77.
67
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Chapter 3 : Effects of benzodiazepines on driving
performance of anxiety patients
Katerina Touliou, CERT/HIT, Greece
68
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Abstract
Alprazolam is a widely prescribed anxiolytic for the treatment of anxiety, panic disorder, and depression.
Reported adverse effects after alprazolam intake include sleepiness, sedation, drowsiness, and reduced
alertness. Studies based on laboratory tests have found that alprazolam affects memory, attention and
tracking by decreasing performance in most cases (Seppala et al., 1986; de Gier et al., 1986).
In this study, the alprazolam effect (0.5 mg) was investigated in three groups: a) treated anxiety patients, b)
untreated anxiety patients, and c) control group.
51 participants matched for age, gender and driving experience completed two driving tasks; a lane tracking
and a car following scenario in a simulated environment.
Driving variables, cognitive measures, blood
samples and subjective assessments were gathered in a baseline and an oral administration of alprazolam
(0.5 mg) condition.
Alprazolam administration impaired weaving control (SDLP) in lane tracking scenario for all groups.
It
appears that moderate concentrations of alprazolam in blood serum (ng/mL) are associated with impairment
in road tracking for the control group. For the untreated anxiety patients only two concentration levels were
detected and no improvement in lateral position keeping was observed. Impairment (i.e. less weaving) was
found in the treated anxiety group for the serum concentration groups of 5-10 and >15 ng/mL. Delayed
brake reaction times (sec) were observed in treated and untreated anxiety patients in the car following
scenario. Healthy participants showed riskier behaviour after alprazolam administration compared to treated
and untreated anxiety patients (p<.001) who showed increased percentage of time spent with Time-toCollision (TTC) values between 2 to 4 seconds. Alertness in attentional performance tests was significantly
decreased only in healthy participants (p=.015). Only untreated patients and healthy participants reported
decreased vigilance.
This study clearly showed an alprazolam effect (0.5mg) in driving performance in treated, untreated anxiety
patients and healthy participants. Future studies might focus on dose-related relationships between the
same groups.
Additionally, further comparisons with other medicinal drugs would be desirable to be
investigating in the future reflecting the real medicinal drug users and, consequently, the real driving
performance.
Conclusively, the main findings of this study are in agreement with current research that alprazolam has a
detrimental effect on driving behaviour. Thus, people under alprazolam medication should be informed by
general practitioners about the impairing effects of alprazolam administration to their everyday activities and
driving.
69
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Introduction
Alprazolam (Generic Xanax®) is a benzodiazepine derivative mainly prescribed for the treatment of
generalised anxiety, panic disorder, and depression. Alprazolam is the most often prescribed psychoactive
substance (Verster et al., 2002). The usual clinical dosages of alprazolam, administrated in divided doses,
range from 0.5 mg to 4 mg/day for the treatment of anxiety disorder and from 6 to 10 mg/day for the
treatment of panic disorder. Anxiety is often accompanied by depression; alprazolam is being prescribed for
these cases as it has antidepressant properties (Petty et al., 1995).
Reported adverse effects after
alprazolam intake include sleepiness, sedation, drowsiness, and reduced alertness.
Alprazolam is a widely prescribed medicine mainly prescribed to outpatients and it is the most used
benzodiazepine for recreational purposes. It affects all aspects of everyday living activities including driving
and operating machinery. Many studies have investigated its effect in cognitive and driving performance.
Relevant studies have focussed on cognitive impairments that both directly and indirectly investigate the
effect of alprazolam and other benzodiazepines on driving performance in close tracks or driving simulators.
Studies based on laboratory tests have found that alprazolam affects memory, attention and tracking by
decreasing performance in most cases (Seppala et al., 1986; de Gier et al., 1986).
However, not all
benzodiazepines deteriorate cognitive performance (Busto et al., 2000). It is questionable whether the
disparities spring from variations in properties and effects of different benzodiazepines or because the tests
do highlight diverse aspects of cognitive functioning with the application of various tests as different
measures may lead to different results.
Moreover, studies have been conducted in order to investigate if the clinical suggestion of tolerance after a
few days of administration holds true. Current literature is in line with this proposition; it has been found that
chronic use does not deteriorate performance.
Impairment may be limited to the early stages of
benzodiazepine intake, with the general clinical suggestion that tolerance develops within a few days of
benzodiazepine use (Busto et al., 2000). Benzodiazepines’effect on driving performance (e.g. steering,
reaction time, lane keeping) has been supported by many studies performed either on road or in simulated
environment (Mortimor, 1986; Smiley, 1985). On the other hand, epidemiological and laboratory studies
provide inconclusive findings regarding risk assessment and potentially impairing effects of benzodiazepines
on driving behaviour (EMCDDA, 1999; Longo et al., 2001; deGier, 1986).
On the whole, a handful of studies have investigated the effect of alprazolam in cognitive and psychomotor
skills which are a related to driving and to actual driving measures.
Snyder and colleagues (2005) conducted a study in order to investigate the degree and nature of cognitive
impairment by the administration of two doses of alprazolam (0.5 and 1 mg).
Several recordings of
psychomotor function, working memory, visual attention, planning, and learning were obtained. Significant
findings indicated that attention speed was affected by lower alprazolam doses, and higher dose affected
psychomotor functions. The authors suggest the combination of these two led to the mediocre deterioration
findings in memory and learning which are related to executive functions. Thus, lower doses affect simple
choices and higher doses affect more complex functions.
The participants in this study were healthy
participants and not alprazolam users. Therefore, these findings might be important for developing baseline
assessments for further comparisons aiming at dose-related relationships in relevant patient groups.
Current research suggests that alprazolam impairs cognitive performance after acute dosages (e.g.
70
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Ellinwood et al., 1995; Kroboth et al., 1998; Vermeeren et al., 1995) in skills related to driving such as
tracking, reaction and alertness. As noted above, the application of diverse measure types in order to
investigate the effect of different types of benzodiazepines on driving performance it makes it even harder
and complex to compare the results from different studies.
The investigation of potential effects of
alprazolam on driving with the application of cognitive and laboratory tests might allow erroneous transfer of
inferences as related validity is questionable. Volkerts and colleagues (1992) advocated that there is low
sensitivity in the inferences and findings derived by laboratory and cognitive tests when compared to real
traffic and on the road tests.
Verster and colleagues (2002) investigated the acute effects of alprazolam (1mg) on driving performance
during real traffic in conjunction with laboratory tests related to driving skills. Participants had to complete a
standardised driving task on a highway one hour after alprazolam administration. A cognitive tests’battery
was administered two and a half hours after the driving task. Statistically significant differences were found
between the alprazolam and placebo groups for Standard Deviation of Lateral Position (SDLP) and Standard
Deviation of Speed (SDS) accompanied by impairment in laboratory tests. Vermeeren and colleagues
(2009) reviewed related literature on anxiolytics’effect on driving. Their discussion on over-the-road driving
points out that the mean increase of SDLP in this study was comparable to BAC=1.5mg/ml (Louwerens et
al., 1987). Subjective scales showed, also, impairment in driving quality, decrease in alertness, lower mental
activation, and increased mental effort.
The comparability level of anxiolytics consumption to alcohol
concentrations that are far above the legal limit in most countries (i.e. some countries have “zero tolerance”
towards alcohol consumption) brings to attention the increased risk of patients to be involved in road
accidents. The authors concluded that warnings should be given to users who are driving a car.
Likewise, Leufkens and colleagues (2007) compared the effects of two types of release (extended release
(ER) and immediate release (IR)) of alprazolam in both real driving settings and laboratory tests. The driving
condition was, also, a highway driving task four hours after oral administration. Laboratory tests comprised
cognitive and psychomotor tests that were administered one hour, 2 and a half hours, and five hours post
alprazolam administration.
Impairment was found for both formulations; however almost double was
observed for the immediate release (XR) between four and five hours after administration. The findings were
supported by the laboratory tests, which, also, showed impairment in memory functioning. Similar to the
Verster and colleagues (2002) findings, the authors emphasised the increased risk of alprazolam users to be
involved in traffic accidents.
Verster and colleagues (2005) carried out a literature review on 14 placebo controlled and double blind
studies which investigated the effects of anxiolytic drugs on on-the road tests. Standard Deviation of Lateral
Position (SDLP) was the main driving parameter.
They concluded that, among other types of
benzodiazepines, a single dose of alprazolam might impair driving performance (i.e. increased weaving was
recorded). The authors suggested patients treated with alprazolam should be cautioned when driving a car
and that it might not be safe to drive while under alprazolam therapy.
In general, relevant studies have shown detrimental impairment due to alprazolam administration on driving
performance, controlled laboratory settings and subjective scales.
71
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Objectives
In the present study the major objective was to investigate the effect of alprazolam in treated and untreated
anxiety patients compared to a healthy control group after oral alprazolam administration (0.5 mg) (acute
phase) in a simulated environment. Primary variables were the vehicle variables (car simulator: driving
performance measures).
The secondary objective was to compare multiple cognitive and subjective measures, collected for each
participant, in order to establish the whole range of driving impairment. It was hypothesised that alprazolam
will affect driving performance of all groups. In addition, it was of interest to compare potentially additive
effects (treated patients) to acute effects (untreated and healthy participants).
Methods
This section describes the participants’demographics and the sample selection process.
Participants
In total, 51 participants were recruited in the experiment. The following table presents age and gender
distribution for each group.
Table 1: Group characteristics
Group
Age
Treated
Gender
(Mean±SD)
(M/F)
42.4±13.9
8/7
36.9±8.9
9/9
35.4±8.8
8/10
(Group A; N=15)
Untreated
(Group B; N=18)
Control
(Group C; N=18)
All participants were screened prior participation and were medically examined by two collaborating doctors.
Correct medical diagnosis was ensured by the collaborating doctors. In groups A and B (patients), the
participants were diagnosed with anxiety and, specifically, they should have a Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale
score equal or greater than 20 (HAM-A: mild to moderate severity) (Hamilton, 1959). Patients in Group A
were systematically using alprazolam for at least 2 months before the testing day. Patients in Group B did
not receive any kind of treatment for at least two months before the testing day. Participants in Group C had
no medical history of anxiety or alcohol abuse and were free of medication.
Participants in Groups A, B and C were matched for age, gender and driving experience. Participants were
72
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
experienced drivers (at least 3 years since they obtained their driving license) and were currently active
drivers. Volunteers received reimbursement for their participation.
In case of alcohol or drug screening tests were positive, participants were excluded from the study (with the
exception of Group A, where participants were positive for benzodiazepines). Participants with other medical
conditions (psychiatric, neurological conditions and/or cognitive impairments) were excluded from the study.
Participants were asked to refrain from smoking prior testing and not to have any meal during the last four
hours before the experiment.
Before each phase (Tables 2 and 3), participants were medically examined.
The collaborating doctor
present was responsible for the acquisition and administration of alprazolam to the participants.
The
collaborating doctor closely monitored the participant during the experiment. If the collaborating doctor
judged the participant was unable to perform any of the required tasks, the task was interrupted and the
experiment stopped.
Gathered data (subjective scales, vehicle logfiles, etc.) were anonymised and kept in safe place.
The
following steps were taken in order to ensure anonymity, confidentiality and protection of personal data.
Only the chief administrator had access to raw data.
Upon detailed experimental briefing, participants
signed a written consent. Participants were assured that they were able to withdraw at any point without any
consequences.
If the participants were diagnosed with a medical problem during medical examination this was made known
to them. The study was reviewed and approved by the Bioethics Committee of CERTH.
1
Study design
The present study utilises a within repeated- and between-participants’design for the comparison of patient
and control experimental groups in question. The independent variable is the alprazolam administration with
two levels: (baseline and alprazolam 0.5 mg intake). Dependent variables are: a) the driving performance
(simulated environment), b) attentional performance (winTAP), and c) subjective assessments. The aim was
to investigate the acute effect of alprazolam administration (0.5mg) and the possibility of additive effects
(treated) in two driving tasks.
Procedure
Participants were asked to arrive at the premises at the same time for each phase of the experiment (14:0017:00).
At arrival they completed an introductory (background) questionnaire and written consent was
obtained after detailed briefing.
It was made sure that participants understood the objectives and the
experimental procedure before they consented to participate Both alcohol screening (with breathanalyser)
and urine drug screening were performed and if found negative, participants had a familiarisation drive in
order to get used to the driving simulator.
Participants had to complete the following tasks at the driving simulator: a) perform a lane tracking scenario
for about 20 minutes in a highway environment maintaining a constant speed of 90 km/h and b) perform a
car following scenario for about 20 minutes in a highway environment maintaining a safe distance from the
1
CERTH: Hellenic institute of Transport (HIT) is an institute of the non-profit
organisation Centre for Research and Technology, Hellas .
73
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
lead vehicle that was moving with a steady speed of 90 km/h. Four instances of abrupt breaking (leading
vehicle) occurred randomly.
Participants received instructions for each driving scenario.
For the lane
tracking scenario participants were instructed to maintain a constant speed of 90 km/h and steady lateral
position. For the car following scenario they were instructed to maintain a safety distance from the lead
vehicle.
Scenarios were counter-balanced between the two phases and among participants. Following the simulator
driving scenarios, participants completed the neuropsychological tests (winTAP, Zimmerman and Fimm,
1993). Participants had breaks between the driving scenarios and if required they could get more breaks.
Participants were constantly monitored for simulator sickness symptoms. They were, also, screened during
familiarisation driving phase by completing a simulator sickness questionnaire. Participants were asked not
to have coffee or any alcoholic beverages 24 hours prior testing took place. Subjective questionnaires were
filled in before and after each scenario and before and after the neuropsychological tests rating their
sleepiness, mental effort, and driving quality. Detailed accounts of the driving scenarios, subjective scales,
and the neuropsychological battery are given in the respective sections.
The experimental study was carried out in three phases. The respective timelines are shown in tables 2 and
3. Baseline and treatment phases were counterbalanced in order to control for order effects and potentially
related confounders. Blood collection lasted approximately 10-15 minutes and usually participants wanted to
relax and take a small break before the driving tasks. Participants started the driving tasks almost 15
minutes after blood collection and about an hour after alprazolam intake (Table 3). The time difference
between administration and driving is adequate for testing procedure and similar to relevant studies (e.g.
Leufkens et al., 2007).
Table 2: Phases I(familiarisation)-II(baseline assessment) timelines
Time
Task
-30 min
Breathanalysing
Urine screening
0
Scenario 1 (lane tracking) (KSS pre-post)
or
Scenario 2 (car following) (KSS pre-post)
The order of scenarios is counterbalanced
+30 min
Scenario 1 (lane tracking) (KSS pre-post)
or
Scenario 2 (car following) (KSS pre-post)
The order of scenarios is counterbalanced
+60 min
Attentional performance (WinTAP) (KSS
+90min
Subjective assessments (RSME, DQS)
pre-post)
Grey parts of the table were parts of the experiments performed in phase II only to avoid overfamiliarisation.
Phases II and III were separated by a wash out period of 7 days.
74
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Table 3: Phase III timeline (experimental
measures)
Time
Task
-75 min
Medical check
Breathanalysing
Urine screening
-60 min
Subjects are administered 0.5 mg of alprazolam
-30 min
Blood collection [serum/blood spots/whole blood (10ml)]
The process of blood collection lasts 10-15 minutes (e.g. preparations, etc.)
0
Scenario 1 (lane tracking) (KSS pre-post)
or
Scenario 2 (car following) (KSS pre-post)
The order of performance of scenarios is rotated among subjects.
+30 min
Scenario 1 (lane tracking) (KSS pre-post)
or
Scenario 2 (car following) (KSS pre-post)
The order of scenarios is counterbalanced
+60 min
Attentional performance (WinTAP) (KSS pre-post)
+90min
Subjective assessments (RSME, DQS)
Blood samples were collected by a specialist (microbiologist) in 10 ml tubes and frozen after (both whole and
serum) according to the proposed guidelines.
Whole blood, serum and blood spot specimens were
transported to the Institute of Legal Medicine and Traffic Medicine, University of Heidelberg, Germany for
further analysis.
The following detailed description of analysis and extraction was provided by the
colleagues from the University of Heidelberg.
Five calibration standards were prepared in each batch containing 2.5, 5, 10, 20, 50 ng alprazolam/mL and
0.125, 0.25, 0.5, 1.0, 2.5 ng -hydroxy-alprazolam/mL for each blood, serum and dried blood spots (DBS),
respectively. DBS calibrators were prepared by spotting 100 µL of fortified blood onto filter paper which was
dried at room temperature over night. DBS were punched out and transferred into plastic tubes. Extraction
of DBS was performed in the same way as for whole blood, serum specimens and corresponding calibrators.
1.0 mL borate buffer (pH 8.5) and the internal standards (alprazolam-d5 20 ng/mL, -hydroxy-alprazolam-d5
2 ng/mL) were added to 100 µL serum, whole blood or DBS, respectively. Samples were extracted using 1.0
mL toluene/iso-amylalcohol (95:5, v/v) and were shaken for about 10 min. Subsequently, samples were
centrifuged (10 min at 4300g); the organic layer was transferred into a silanized vial and evaporated to
dryness at 40°C. The residue was reconstituted in 50 µL of the mobile phase (4mM ammonium acetate
buffer pH 3.2/methanol/acetonitrile 45:11:55, v/v/v).
Analysis was performed on an API 4000 tandem mass spectrometer with a Turbo Ion ionization source
operated in the positive-ion mode (Applied Biosystems, Darmstadt, Germany). It was interfaced to a HPLC
pump equipped with an autosampler (1100 series, Agilent, Waldbronn, Germany).
The samples (5 µL aliquots) were eluted from a Luna 5µ C18 (2) column (2.00 x 150 mm, 5 µm particle size,
75
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Phenomenex, Aschaffenburg, Germany) at a flow rate of 250 µL/min.
ionisation
mode
with
-hydroxy-alprazolam
the
m/z
following
transitions:
325.1à297.1*
and
alprazolam
325.1à325.1;
Data were monitored in positive
m/z
309à281*
alprazolam-d5
and
309à205;
m/z
314à286;
-hydroxy-alprazolam-d5 m/z 330.1à302.1. Transitions marked with an asterisk were used for quantitation.
Calibration lines were constructed with linear least squares regression using the ratio of the target analyte
peak area to the corresponding internal standard peak area. Each specimen was tested twice, and the
respective arithmetic mean is given. The assay has been evaluated according to international standards for
the validation of bio analytical methods.
Driving and psychomotor tests
The CERTH/HIT driving simulator (Figure 1) is built around a Smart cabin equipped with sensors. The
actuation of all control levers, windshield wipers, blinker, ignition key and light switch is electronically
transmitted to the driving computer. All operational elements such as steering wheel, accelerator pedal,
brake pedal, gearshift lever and handbrake lever, provide nature-true force reactions. The visual system
includes five large-screens, each having a width of 2 meters. There visual system works with on-screen
projection with video projectors (2500 ANSI-lumen). The sound system generates original sounds according
to the situation (starter, engine noise, horn, screeching of tires, drive wind, rain, etc.). The vibration device
creates nature true vibrations of the car according to the revolvation of the simulated engine.
Figure 1: Driving simulator
The simulator is equipped with special software which allows the development of specific driving scenarios
aiming at creating a monotonous driving environment specifically for the studies conducted within the
framework of DRUID.
The simulated environment is capable of incorporating surrounding traffic with
maximum 30 road users with artificial intelligence agents, comprising passenger cars, trucks, pedestrians
and cyclists.
The primary driving variable was the Standard Deviation of Lateral Position (SDLP) for the lane tracking
scenario which is a very reliable index of weaving and overall lateral control of the vehicle. The car following
76
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
scenario was designed with stable leading vehicle speed, therefore it was decided to calculate percentages
of time (%) driven with certain Time-to-Collision (TTC) values. falling into five categories. The selected five
categories were the following:
TTC 0-1: percentage of time driven with TTC between 0 and 1 sec
TTC 1-1.5: percentage of time driven with TTC between 1 and 1.5 sec
TTC 1.5-2: percentage of time driven with TTC between 1.5 and 2 sec
TTC 2-4: percentage of time driven with TTC between 2 and 4 sec
TTC >4: percentage of time driven with TTC above 4 sec
As there is no previous literature on recommended stratification, the decision was based on consortiums’
recommendations for intervals of interest that would be of greater potential for more in depth data collection
of critical TTCs (i.e. 1-2).
Obviously, very low TTC values suggest higher risk for the driver to be involved in an accident. In addition,
more time spent in the category TTC
2-4,
implies that participants spent more time in following the leading
vehicle with safety distance keeping; thus complying with the given instructions. More time spent with TTC
>4 suggests that participants were less concentrated and not attentive to the driving task and, consequently,
did not follow the driving instructions.
The winTAP (Zimmermann and Fimm, 1993) is a standardised neuropsychological battery of attentional
performance. The selected individual tests applied were the following: a) alertness: choice between two
stimuli, b) Go/NoGo: simple choice reaction task, c) divided attention audio: reaction to two consecutive
same sounds (two low or two high), d) divided attention: visual: reaction to the formation of a small
rectangular by small stars, and e) divided attention with both visual and auditory stimuli presented
simultaneously. Reaction times (msec), omissions and errors were recorded.
Secondary measures
Several variables are derived from the driving simulator logfiles. Another parameter of interest was the time
it takes to react to an abrupt breaking of the lead vehicle. Participants’reaction times to the lead vehicle’s
abrupt breaking were recorded for the car following scenario.
Subjective measures
The Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS) is a universally accepted, validated and standardised scale
(Åkerstedt and Gillberg, 1990). Participants were asked to subjectively evaluate their vigilance state before
and after the task completion across all conditions. The 9-point KSS was used: 1=very alert, 3=alert,
5=neither alert nor sleepy, 7=sleepy (but not fighting sleep), 9=very sleepy (fighting sleep). Sleepiness was
subjectively rated before and after each driving scenario and before and after the neuropsychological tests
were carried out. Higher scoring meant less vigilance and subsequently increased sleepiness. In addition,
participants rated how much they had to try in order to complete the driving tasks [Rating Scale of Mental
Effort (0-150); RSME] (Zijlstra, 1993) and how well they thought they performed in the driving tasks
compared to their everyday driving experience (Driving Quality Scale, Brookhuis et al., 1985). In the present
77
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
study, the original subjective scales were translated into Greek and back-translated by an independent
professional and verified by a native English speaker.
Statistics
Within and between participants comparisons (superiority tests) were carried out with repeated measures of
GLM and one-way ANOVAs. In case of violation of homogeneity and homoscedacity assumptions, nonparametric equivalents were administered (Friedman and Wilcoxon rank test, respectively).
Within
comparisons were carried out in order to investigate the effect of alprazolam (0.5mg) administration.
Between comparisons were carried out in order to investigate difference between probable additive effects
and acute effects. Moreover, the relation between alprazolam and alcohol effect was investigated. The
alcohol effect was based on the alcohol criterion data collected in the CPAP study (i.e. Chapter 4). The
level was set at .05. Statistical analyses were performed with the statistical programme Statistical Package
for the Social Sciences (SPSS) (version 18.0 for Windows; SPSS, Chicago, IL).
78
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Results
This section is divided in 4 parts according to the types of collected data.
Firstly, the main primary driving
parameters are presented per driving scenario (i.e. lane tracking and car following). Secondly, the analyses
of additional neuropsychological measures are presented. Finally, subjective scales’results are provided.
Tables 4 to 6 present within participants’comparisons of the vehicle parameters for both driving scenarios
and subjective assessments. The findings are discussed in the respective sections.
Table 4: Mean (±SE) scores and 95% confidence intervals of driving measures and subjective assessments
for the treated anxiety patients
TREATED ANXIETY PATIENTS
Alprazolam
Baseline
Driving
measures
Mean±SE
SDLP(m)
0.30±0.01
TTC0-1 (sec)
TTC1-1.5 (sec)
6.46±0.21
8.09±0.24
TTC1.5-2 (sec)
13.05±0.52
(11.93 – 14.17)
TTC2-4 (sec)
20.08±0.65
TTC>4 (sec)
52.32±0.94
BRT (msec)
0.92±0.43
Mental Effort
34.37±5.13
(23.37 – 45.37)
6.51±6.91
3.44±0.33
Driving quality
Scale
KSS
[95% CI]
Mean±SE
Significance
[95% CI]
Lane Tracking
(0.29 – 0.32)
0.36±.02
Car Following
(6.02 – 6.90)
7.05±0.20
(7.57 – 8.61)
10.03±0.21
(0.33 – 0.39)
*
(6.61 – 7.48)
(9.59 – 10.48)
*
*
16.50±0.52
(15.40 – 17.61)
*
(18.69 – 21.47)
24.09±0.69
(22.62 – 25.57)
*
(50.31 – 54.34)
42.32±0.89
(40.41 – 44.24)
*
(0.87 – 1.08)
*
34.7±4.37
(25.33 – 44.07)
NS
(-8.30 – 21.32)
4.83±6.06
(-8.18 – 17.83)
NS
(2.75 – 4.14)
3.99±0.35
(3.24 – 4.74)
NS
(0.83 – 1.01)
0.97±0.05
Subjective assessments
* Significant (p<.05); NS: Non-significant (p>.05).
Table 5: Mean (±SE) scores and 95% confidence intervals of driving measures and subjective assessments
for the untreated anxiety patients
Baseline
UNTREATED ANXIETY PATIENTS
Alprazolam
Driving
measures
Mean±SE
SDLP(m)
0.27±.03
TTC0-1 (sec)
TTC1-1.5 (sec)
TTC1.5-2 (sec)
TTC2-4 (sec)
TTC>4 (sec)
BRT (msec)
7.43±0.21
12.06±0.44
14.65±0.49
24.06±0.99
41.80±1.56
0.85±0.02
Mental Effort
Driving
quality Scale
KSS
33.30±2.99
Lane Tracking
(0.29 –0.30)
0.31±.01
Car Following
(7.00 – 7.87)
8.5±0.33
(11.89–12.94)
13.51±0.49
(13.63–15.67)
16.41±0.46
(21.98–26.14)
26.64±0.69
(38.51–45.09)
34.94±1.54
(0.81 –0.90)
0.95±0.02
Subjective assessments
(27.04 –39.58)
49.50±6.69
-2.43±5.02
(-13.02 – 8.15)
3.44±0.24
(2.94 –3.94)
[95% CI]
Mean±SE
[95% CI]
Significance
(0.29 –0.33)
*
(7.81 – 9.20)
(12.47 –14.55)
(15.43 – 17.39)
(25.19 –28.09)
(31.70 – 38.18)
(0.91 –1.0)
*
*
*
*
*
*
(35.38 –63.62)
NS
-19.13±5.96
(31.7– (-6.56))
*
5.68±0.48
(4.67 –6.68)
*
* Significant (p<.05); NS: Non-significant (p>.05).
79
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Table 6: Mean (±SE) scores and 95% confidence intervals of driving measures and subjective assessments
for the control group.
Baseline
Driving
measures
Mean±SE
SDLP(m)
0.26±.01
TTC0-1 (sec)
TTC1-1.5 (sec)
TTC1.5-2 (sec)
TTC2-4 (sec)
TTC >4 (sec)
BRT (msec)
4.49±0.22
7.06±0.22
8.41±0.39
38.09±1.09
41.96±1.17
0.81±0.03
Mental Effort
Driving
quality
KSS
46.67±3.80
[95% CI]
CONTROL
Alprazolam
Mean±SE
Lane Tracking
(0.24 –0.28)
0.33±.01
Car Following
(4.03 – 4.94)
7.51±0.30
(6.60 –7.52)
9.50±0.37
(7.61 –9.20)
13.54±0.33
(35.78 – 40.40)
34.05±0.89
(39.48 – 44.44)
35.41±1.21
(0.76 –0.87)
0.84±0.03
Subjective assessments
(38.65– 54.70)
55.31±4.68
[95% CI]
Significance
(0.31 –0.35)
*
(6.88 – 8.13)
(8.72 –10.28)
(12.85 – 14.22)
(32.18 – 35.92)
(32.86 – 37.95)
(0.77 –0.91)
*
*
*
*
*
NS
(45.43 – 65.20)
NS
-6.50±3.17
(-13.19– 0.19)
-17.54±7.09
(-32.50 – 2.59)
NS
3.90±0.34
(3.18–4.61)
6.07±0.29
(5.45–6.68)
*
* Significant
(p<.05); NS: Non-significant (p>.05).
Driving parameters (primary variables)
Lane tracking
It is evident from tables 4-6 that alprazolam intake impaired driving performance in all groups. In particular,
increased weaving (SDLP= 5.8 cm) has been found in treated anxiety patients when driving an hour after
alprazolam intake (F (1, 14) =11.31, p=.005). Similarly, untreated anxiety patients showed a significant
increase of 4 cm after alprazolam intake (F (1, 17) =5.28, p=.035). On the same track, healthy participants
showed the greatest increase in weaving ( SDLP=6.8 cm; F (1, 17) =36.34, p<.001).
The following graph (figure 2) presents the percentages (%) of impaired/improved driving performance as a
function of alprazolam serum concentration levels (ng/mL). It appears that alprazolam is associated with
impairment in lateral position keeping even in low concentrations.
Figure 2: Percentage of impairment/improvement as a function of serum concentration
80
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
(ng/mL) for the control group
For the untreated anxiety patients only two concentration levels were detected and impairment in lateral
position keeping was observed.
Figure 3: Percentage of impairment/improvement as a function of serum concentration
(ng/mL) for the untreated anxiety patients’group
Higher impairment was found for the treated anxiety group for serum concentration groups of 5-10 and >15
ng/mL. Almost in all cases impairment was observed for the other two groups.
Figure 4: Percentage of impairment/improvement as a function of serum concentration
(ng/mL) for the treated anxiety patients’group
The following graph depicts the comparisons among the three experimental groups in lateral position change
81
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
SDLP). The change (i.e. simple effect size) in lateral position was compared with alcohol consumption
effect in weaving measured in the alcohol calibration study conducted within the framework of DRUID project
(see Chapter 4).
Pairwise comparisons were conducted and a significant mean difference was found
between the mean change in SDLP in the control group after alprazolam intake (0.068±0.0112 m) and the
control group after alcohol consumption (0.05%) (0.024±0.007) (p=.002). Moreover, a trend appears for the
difference between the increase in weaving due to the acute effect of alprazolam in treated anxiety patients
(0.058±0.017) and the deterioration in weaving because of alcohol consumption (0.024±0.007) (p=.061).
This probably explains the inclusion of the alcohol criterion in the lower confidence limit (95% CI) (Figure 5)
Equivalence is shown for untreated patients and alcohol. Additionally, equivalence testing showed that there
is borderline equivalence for alcohol and alprazolam intake for treated patients (additive effect) (Figure 5).
Figure 5: Mean change of SDLP (m) across groups including the alcohol criterion (0.05%)
Car following
Percentage (%) of time spent driving within Time-To-Collision (TTC) categories
The findings from TTC comparisons among conditions are described separately for each chosen TTC
category. Within and between comparisons were carried out for each TTC category.
•
Percentage (%) of time with TTC 0-1
As shown in tables 4-6, the percentage of time (%) spent driving with TTC values between 0 and 1,
82
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
increased significantly for all groups (F (3,65)=20.65, p<.05). Pairwise comparisons (Bonferroni adjusted)
revealed significantly greater percentage of time spent between 0 and 1 TTC values for untreated compared
to treated patients ( =1.46%, p=.003) in the alprazolam condition.
As shown in the graph below, control participants were more affected by the alprazolam intake when
compared to the alcohol effect (p<.001).
Equivalence is shown for the treated and untreated anxiety
patients.
Figure 6: Mean change ( ) in the percentage (%) of time driven with TTC
0-1
(sec) across group including
the alcohol criterion (0.05%)
•
Percentage (%) of time with TTC1-1.5
Likewise, percentage of time driven with TTC values between 1 and 1.5 increased significantly after
alprazolam intake for all groups with greater increase observed in the control group ( =2.45%, p<.001).
Following between groups’pairwise comparisons revealed that untreated patients spent significantly more
time driving with TTC values between 1 and 1.5 compared to both treated ( =3.47%, p<.001) and healthy
participants ( =4%, p<.001) after alprazolam intake. No significant difference was found between treated
patients and the control group (p>.05).
Both control groups after alprazolam and after alcohol consumption showed increase in dangerous close
following behaviour which is of significant equivalence as shown in Figure 7. However, untreated patients’
increase in time driven with TTC between 1 and 1.5 sec because of alprazolam intake is significantly less
than the time increase because of alcohol consumption in the control group (p=.043).
83
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Figure 7: Mean change ( ) in the % of time driven with TTC
1-1.5
(sec) across group including the alcohol
criterion (0.05%)
•
Percentage of time with TTC 1.5-2
The percentage of time spent driving with TTC values between 1.5 and 2 seconds increased significantly for
all groups as shown in tables 4 to 6.
Pairwise comparisons revealed that healthy participants spent
significantly less time with TTC values between 1.5 and 2 compared to treated ( =2.97%, p<.001) and
untreated patients ( =2.87%, p<.001).
The change in percentage of time (%) spent driving with TTC values between 1.5 and 2 seconds was
significantly less after alcohol consumption compared to the change in treated, untreated patients and
control participants (p<.001, p=.003, p<.001, respectively).
•
Percentage of time with TTC
2-4
Significant increase in time spent driving with TTC values between 2 and 4 seconds (safety distance) was
observed in the treated and untreated groups. On the other hand, significant decrease was observed for the
control group. Healthy participants spent significantly more time with TTC values between 2 and 4 seconds
than treated ( =9.96%, p<.001) and untreated patients ( =7.41%, p<.001) after alprazolam intake.
Overall following behaviour was impaired as alprazolam serum concentration increased (Figure 8).
84
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Figure 8: Percentage of improvement/impairment as a function of serum concentration (ng/mL) (Control
group -TTC 2-4)
On the other hand, untreated patients improved significantly in distance keeping behaviour as shown in
figure 9.
Figure 9: Percentage of improvement/impairment as a function of serum concentration (ng/mL) (Untreated
group -TTC 2-4)
85
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Figure 10: Percentage of improvement/impairment as a function of serum concentration (ng/mL) (Treated
group -TTC 2-4)
Treated patients distance keeping behaviour showed only improvement after alprazolam intake (Figure 10).
Participants spent significantly less time driving with TTC values between 2 and 4 seconds when they had
consumed alcohol compared to the change due to alprazolam intake in both treated and untreated anxiety
patients (p<.001). No difference was found for the comparison with the control group (alprazolam) but, also,
no equivalence was shown.
•
Percentage (%) of time with TTC>4
The time spent with TTC values higher than 4 seconds decreased significantly for all groups after alprazolam
intake as shown in tables 4 to 6. Further pairwise comparisons revealed significantly greater decrease in
percentage of time spent with TTC values greater than 4 seconds in treated patients ( =7.38%, p<.001)
compared to untreated and control groups ( =6.96%, p<.001). However, variations were extremely large in
this last TTC category; therefore results may not be reliable.
Alprazolam intake significantly decreased the time spent with TTC values above 4 seconds in the treated,
untreated, and control groups when compared to the alcohol consumption condition (p<.001).
The following graph presents the distribution of percentages (%) within and between categories in order to
get an overview of distance keeping behaviour. Participants from the control group (baseline condition)
spent the most time with safe distance keeping and overall spent less time with dangerously low TTCs.
86
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
TB: Treated Baseline
TA: Treated Alprazolam
UB: Untreated Baseline
UA: Untreated Alprazolam
CB: Control Baseline
CA: Control Alprazolam
Figure 11: Percentages (%) of driven time per TTC category as a function of the experimental conditions
Brake reaction time (sec)
As shown in tables 4 to 6 significant increase in brake reaction time is observed in treated and untreated
patient groups but not in control group. Pairwise comparisons showed trends towards significant differences
2
between treated, untreated and control group (p=0.028 and p=0.056, respectively).
The treated group showed the greatest deterioration in brake reaction time as a function of serum
concentration as it is evident from graph 14. With increasing concentration, impairment increased as well.
Impairment in brake reaction time is evident in most groups.
2
Bonferroni adjustment leads to lower p-values
87
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Figure 12: Percentage of improvement/impairment as a function of serum concentration (ng/mL) (Control
group -BRT)
Figure 13: Percentage of improvement/impairment as a function of serum concentration (ng/mL) (Untreated
group -BRT)
88
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Figure 14: Percentage of improvement/impairment as a function of serum concentration (ng/mL) (Treated
group -BRT)
Following comparisons between the decrease in BRT in the experimental and alcohol group (p>.05) were of
no statistical significance. In spite of non significant results, no equivalence to alcohol was found. This may
be the result of great variation in braking reaction time for the alcohol consumption group (0.222±0.501).
Secondary variables (Neuropsychological measures)
The secondary measurements included in this section are the results from the neuropsychological tests of
the WinTAP battery.
Alertness was significantly decreased in healthy participants (p=.015). No other
significant difference was found in both within and between comparisons (p>.05).
89
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Figure 15: Mean reaction time (msec) in alertness test
Subjective scales
As shown in Tables 4 to 6 non significant differences in sleepiness, mental effort and driving quality were
found for the treated patients (p>.05). Untreated patients felt significantly less vigilant and believed they
drove really badly in alprazolam condition (p=.001 and p=.021, respectively). Similarly, healthy participants
felt significantly less vigilant after alprazolam intake (p=.018).
90
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Discussion
The findings of this study support the main hypothesis that alprazolam will impair driving performance in all
three groups.
Indeed significant increase in weaving (SDLP) was found in all groups after alprazolam
administration. Specifically, alprazolam’s detrimental effects were evident in weaving in all groups with
higher lateral deviation in the control group by more than 6 cm. Therefore vehicle lateral control is affected
by alprazolam intake. Relevant literature is in agreement with these findings. Alprazolam intake deteriorates
lateral control but acute effect (Curran, 1986; Leufjkens et al., 2007) is greater than chronic administration
due to tolerance to the sedating effects because of repeated use, as it seems to be the case with lateral
deviation in the treated anxiety patients’group in this study. Likewise, most alprazolam studies have found
high increaments of SDLP. Verster and colleagues (2002) observed increaments of SDLP of approximately
9 cm which they compared to driving impairment with a blood alcohol concentration of 0.15% and stated that
BAC above 0.15% corresponds to a 25 times increase of accident risk (Louwerens et al., 1986; Borkenstein
et al, 1964). Similarly, in a review on effect of antidepressants on driving tests’changes it was found that
changes in SDLP after acute doses of sedating effects of antidepressants were comparable to blood alcohol
concentrations of 0.8 mg/mL (Ramaekers, 2003). In the present study, we found supportive evidence for
alprazolam effect; however, not of such increment.
Deterioration in weaving because of alcohol
consumption was found to be equivalent to alprazolam effect in treated and untreated patients and
significantly less compared to alprazolam effect to the control group.
Therefore, the acute effect of
alprazolam in anxiety patients may be comparable to alcohol BAC=.05% effect. However, the detrimental
effects to healthy individuals could be of great interest for future research efforts on recreational/occasional
use of alprazolam by drivers.
The findings indicate probability for higher accident risk but there are no simulator studies to directly
compare the findings. Moreover, lane deviations in real traffic deviate from corresponding measurements in
a simulated environment. Thus ramification and extrapolation of results should be made with this difference
taken into consideration. Further simulator studies could add upon these findings in order to provide a base
of comparability for these increments.
Respective between groups’comparisons showed no significant differences among groups after alprazolam
oral administration (p<.05). Therefore impairment may be comparable among groups as the difference
among groups was approximately around 2-2.5 cm. However, according to figure 4, only in treated patients’
group a percentage of improvement was observed.
weaving.
This improvement may depict (overall
The other two groups had almost solely impaired
20% improvement) tolerance.
Non-sedative
antidepressants were found not to affect SDLP values (Ramaekers, 2003). Therefore a bottom up thinking
process could lead researchers to focus on the same sedating effect in all groups in an attempt to
understand the non-significant differences among groups.
This research idea could be investigated in
another experiment as inferences from studies on antidepressants to experimental findings of
benzodiazepines have less validity.
Similarly, statistically significant differences were found in percentage of time spent across TTC categories
for all groups. The time spent within each category increased after alprazolam intake, except for really high
91
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
values for which following the leading vehicle ceased. In general, safety keeping behaviour was affected.
For treated and untreated anxiety patients the time spent in risky following behaviour increased but the time
of safe following was increased, as well. On the contrary, the control became riskier overall, i.e. they spent
more time dangerously following the lead vehicle and less time in safety distance keeping. These findings
are difficult to interpret especially under the prism of clear and specific guidelines given to all participants to
keep a safety distance from the leading vehicle. Regarding extremely dangerous close following the picture
is similar for all participants; they became riskier. However, with regards to safety distance only the control
group showed decrease after alprazolam intake.
These findings cannot lead to generalisations about
protective or impairing role of alprazolam in safety distance keeping behaviour. The findings derived by the
car following scenario are inconclusive. Figures 8 to 10 reflect the aforementioned findings as they show that
alprazolam improves following behaviour for anxiety patients (regardless if treated or not) and clearly impairs
distance keeping in healthy participants.
Furthermore, extremely risky close following (% of time spent driving with TTC values between 0 and 1
second) of a leading vehicle has been shown to be equivalent, again, for anxiety patients and alcohol
consumption participants. It seems that alprazolam effect was more detrimental for the healthy controls as
they spent ever more time driving with very low TTC values. It seems that alprazolam intake “made”controls
more prone to riskier close following and less safe in distance keeping from the leading vehicle. This effect
disappeared for higher TTC values but driving still remained very risky for the control group and it was
transformed into equivalent to the alcohol consumption group. In other words, healthy participants who
consumed alcohol or alprazolam intake increased the time spent driving with risky TTC values. Regarding
safe keeping behaviour, alcohol was shown to make healthy controls significantly less “conforming”when
compared to anxiety patients with alprazolam intake. However, differences in safe distance keeping were
not found for the control group who received alprazolam when compared to alcohol. Hence, the effect of
alcohol might be more deteriorating in safety keeping behaviour than alprazolam in patients, probably
because of inhibition alleviation.
Furthermore, alprazolam affected brake reaction time in treated and untreated anxiety patients. As treated
group showed the greatest deterioration in reaction time (sec), additive effect of alprazolam intake may be
greater than acute for reaction time.
It appears that additive effects are more powerful and treated anxiety patients do not show tolerance effects
in psychomotor tests or psychomotor related driving skills, without of course, isolating them from the rest of
parameters (i.e. tolerance might not be revealed for other variables). Subjective assessments of treated
anxiety patients are in favour of the tolerance proposition as there was no significant difference in their
evaluation of sleepiness and driving quality during driving scenarios in the simulated tasks. On the contrary,
untreated and healthy participants reported increased sleepiness after alprazolam administration which
reflects their driving performance. In addition, untreated patients reported that they thought they drove really
badly, although greater deviations were recorded for the control group. It is important to keep in mind,
though, that anxiety patients are overly self-conscious, pay high self-attention after the activity, or have high
performance standards for themselves.
It should be borne in mind that anxiety patients’ subjective
assessments of their driving behaviour may be influenced, also, by their symptomatology.
An overall depiction of the car following pattern for all groups and conditions is given in figure 11. The less
“safety distance keeping”behaviour is observed for treated anxiety patients after alprazolam intake. For that
92
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
reason this behaviour may result from additive effect were tolerance does not show. It was expected healthy
participants to show safer keeping distance behaviour and they indeed kept safety distance for the longer
period of time during the driving task. These findings suggest that alprazolam may not only affect alertness
and psychomotor functions but, in addition, decision making, similar to inhibitions’alleviation as alcohol does
in small amounts.
Alprazolam significantly affected alertness in the control group but no other significant impairment was
observed for the neuropsychological tests. Relevant studies have found differences. The greater effect was
the acute effect in healthy individuals. It is important to note that this battery is standardised to driving
behaviour but has not been applied in drug related research before.
Subjective scales confirmed the effect perceived by participants. Treated patients did not perceive any
difference in vigilance, mental effort, and driving quality. On the contrary, the other two groups-not used in
alprazolam medication-reported that they felt significantly less vigilant (untreated and control) and that they
drove badly (untreated). It is alarming that treated patients did not report any difference or change in the
way they drive which might imply that their everyday driving performance is affected and they may not be
aware of it and their risk of accidents due to lack of awareness may be increased.
The effect of alprazolam in healthy participants was stronger than in treated and untreated patients.
Alprazolam intake (0/5 mg) might improve the driving performance of anxiety patients but might have
deteriorative effect in healthy controls’driving performance.
This study clearly showed an alprazolam effect (0.5mg) in driving performance in treated, untreated anxiety
patients and healthy participants. Future studies might focus on dose-related relationships for the same
groups.
Additionally, comparisons with other medicinal drugs would be desirable reflecting the real
medicinal drug users and, consequently, the real driving performance.
Conclusively, the main findings of this study are in agreement with current research that people under
alprazolam medication should be informed about the potential detrimental effects of alprazolam
administration to their everyday activities and driving. Likewise, physicians and medical practitioners should
be educated and trained on how the adverse effects of alprazolam prescriptions may affect driving
performance.
Acknowledgements
This work was conducted with the assistance of two teams of specialists led by Dr. Chrysoula Papadeli and
Prof. Pavlidis. We would like to thank them for being attentive and patient in the recruitment, conduction
process and debriefing. We are grateful to Dr. Gisela Skopp and Dr. Ricarda Jantos for the analysis and
extraction of alprazolam samples and the respective description of the procedure included in this report.
93
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
References
Åkerstedt, T. and Gillberg, M. (1990). Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual.
International Journal of Neuroscience, 52, 29–37.
Borkenstein, R.F. (1974) The role of the drinking driver in traffic accidents, the Grand Rapids study.
Blutalkohol 11.
Brookhuis, K.A., DeVries, G., Prins Van Wijngaarden, P., Veenstra, G., Hommes, M., Louwerens, J.W.,
O’
Hanlon, J.F. (1985). The effects of increasing doses of Meptazisol (100, 200, 400 mg) and Glaferine
(200mg) on driving performance. Report VK 85-16. Haren, The Nethrlands: Traffic Research Center,
University of Groningen.
Busto, U.E., Kaplan, H.L., Wright, C.E., Gomez-Mancilla, B., Zawertailo, L., Greenblatt, D.J., Seller, E.M.
(2000) A comparative pharmacokinetic and dynamic evaluation of alprazolam sustained-release,
bromazepam, and lorazepam. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 20, 628–635.
Curran, H.V. (1986) Tranquillising memories: a review of the effects of benzodiazepines on human memory.
Biological Psychology, 23, 179–213.
de Gier, J.J., Hart, B.J., Nelemans, F.A. The effects of lorazepam and bromazepam on actual driving and
psychomotor performance of patients. In: de Geir J.J., ed. Drugs and driving. London: Taylor and
Francis, 1986:137 – 52.
Ellinwood, E.H., Jr, Heatherly, D.G., Nikaido, A.M., Bjornsson, T.D., Kilts, C. (1985). Comparative
pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics of lorazepam, alprazolam and diazepam.
Psychopharmacology, 86, 392–399.
European Monitoring Centre for Drugs and Drug Addiction. Literature review on the relation between drug
use, impaired driving and traffic accidents (CT.97.EP.14). Lisbon: EMCDDA, 1999.
Hamilton M. (1959). The assessment of anxiety states by rating. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 32,
50–55.
Kroboth, P.D., Folan, M.M., Bauer, K.S., Tullock, W., Wright, C.E., Sweeney, J.A. (1998) Do alprazolaminduced changes in saccadic eye movement and psychomotor function follow the same time course?
Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 38, 337–346.
Leufkens, T.R.M., Vermeeren, A., Smink, B.E., van Ruitenbeek, P., Ramaekers, J.G. (2007). Cognitive,
Psychomotor and actual driving performance in healthy volunteers after immediate and extended
release formulations of alprazolam 1 mg. Psychopharmacology, 191, 951-959.
Longo, M.C., Lokan, R.J., White, J.M.(2001). The relationship between blood benzodiazepine concentration
and vehicle crash culpability. Journal of Traffic Medicine, 29, 36 – 43.
Mortimer, R.G., Howat, P.A. (1986). Effects of alcohol and diazepam, slightly and in combination, on some
aspects of driving performance. In: de Geir J.J., ed. Drugs and driving. London: Taylor and Francis.
Petty, P., Madhukar, H., Trivedi, Fulton, A. M., Rush, J. (1995). Benzodiazepines as antidepressants: Does
GABA play a role in depression? Biological Psychiatry, 38, (9), 578-591.
Ramaekers, J.G. (2003). Antidepressants and driver impairment: empirical evidence from a standard on-theroad test, Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 64, 20–29.
Seppala, T, Mattila, M.J., Palva, E.S., Aranko, K. (1986) Combined effects of anxiolytics and alcohol on
psychomotor performance in young and middle-aged subjects. In: de Geir J.J., ed. Drugs and driving.
London: Taylor and Francis, 179 – 90.
Smiley, A., Moskowitz, H.M., Ziedman, K. (1985). Effects of drugs on driving: driving simulator tests of
94
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
secobarbital, diazepam, marijuana, and alcohol. Los Angeles: National Institute on Drug Abuse.
Snyder, P.J., Werth, J., Giordani, B., Caveney, A.F., Feltner, D., Maruff, P. (2005). A method for determining
the magnitude of change across different cognitive functions in clinical trials: the effects of acute
administration of two different doses alprazolam. Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and
Experimental, 20, (4), 263-273.
Vermeeren, A., Jackson, J.L., Muntjewerff, N.D., Quint, .PJ., Harrison, E.M., O’Hanlon, J.F. (1995)
Comparison of acute alprazolam (0.25, 0.50 and 1.0 mg) effects versus those of lorazepam 2 mg and
placebo on memory in healthy volunteers using laboratory and telephone tests. Psychopharmacology,
118,1–9.
Verster, J.C., Volkerts, E.R. (2004) Clinical pharmacology, clinical efficacy, and behavioral toxicity of
alprazolam: a review of the literature. CNS Drug Review, 10, 45–76.
Verster, J.C., Volkerts, E.R., Verbaten, M.N. (2002). Effects of alprazolam on driving ability, memory
functioning and psychomotor performance: a randomized, placebo-controlled study.
Neuropsychopharmacology, 27, 260–269.
Verster, J.C., Veldhuijzen, D.S., Volkerts, E.R. (2005) Is it safe to drive a car when treated with anxiolytics?
Evidence from on-the-road driving studies during normal traffic. Current Psychiatry Review, 1, 215225.
Zijlstra, F.R.H. (1993). Efficiency in work behavior. A design approach for modern tools. PhD thesis, Delft
University of Technology. Delft, The Netherlands: Delft University Press.
Zimmermann, P. & Fimm, B. (1993). Testbatterie zur Erfassung von Aufmerksamkeitsstörungen. Version
1.02. Freiburg: Psytest.
95
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Chapter 4: Daytime driving in treated (CPAP) and
untreated sleep apnoea patients
Katerina Touliou, CERTH/HIT, Greece
96
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Abstract
Obstructive Sleep Apnoea Syndrome (OSAS) has been shown to be an increased risk factor for traffic
accidents. Studies performed in driving simulators have reported improvements in driving performance with
the application of Continuous Positive Airway Pressure (CPAP) treatment. In the present study, 18 OSAS
patients were treated with CPAP treatment for 7 consecutive days. The experiment involved both baseline
and after CPAP condition. A second group of healthy participants (N=18) was included in the study with a
baseline and an alcohol consumption (BAC=0.05%) condition. All participants drove two scenarios in a
simulated environment; a lane tracking and a car following scenario.
No improvement due to CPAP treatment was found (p>.05) in weaving control (SDLP) for the lane tracking
scenario. On the contrary, statistically significant impairment was found in SDLP due to alcohol (p=.027).
Percentage (%) of time spent driven in clustered TTC categories (0-1,1-1.5,1.5-2,2-4,>4) was calculated.
Treated OSAS patients spent significantly more time with safe keeping distance than untreated patients
(p<.001). Likewise, intoxicated participants spent significantly less time with safe distance from lead vehicle
in car following scenario (p=.008). Equivalence in impairment level was found in BRT (sec) for the car
following scenario between OSAS and alcohol.
Similarly, alcohol consumption significantly decreased
alertness in healthy participants. Intoxicated participants were statistically significant less alert than sober
(p=.007) and showed increased reaction times (msec) compared to OSAS untreated patients in a divided
attention task. OSAS untreated patients reported higher sleepiness but, also, better quality of driving in
general (self-assessment scales).
Consequently, OSAS patients probably showed less awareness of
impairment in driving performance which makes driving even more risky.
In conclusion, the effect of sleep apnoea appears to be detrimental compared to the alcohol effect at 0.05%.
The application of intermediate alcohol BAC levels (i.e. 0.02, 0.08., 0.1) could provide insight in finding
comparable levels of impairment. Probably higher levels of alcohol levels are necessary to be included in a
future research effort for the chosen types of driving parameters in order to perform comparisons to effects in
driving fitness due to Obstructive Sleep Apnoea Syndrome.
97
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Introduction
A large number of subjective (i.e. self-report) and objective (e.g., insurance or police records) studies have
looked at the prevalence of MVAs for patients suffering from Obstructive Sleep Apnoea Syndrome (OSAS)
as compared to the general population (e.g., George, 1995; Maycock, 1996; Wu & Yan-Go, 1996). The
majority of these studies have suggested that OSAS presents an increased risk factor for Motor Vehicle
Accidents (MVAs). However, the results obtained from these studies have been criticised due to the various
confounding variables that were not controlled for (e.g. gender, age, driving experience, etc.) and because of
other possible biases that could have affected the results (i.e. informational, recall, and selection bias; cf.,
George, 2004).
In order to obtain a more accurate measure regarding the risk of MVAs for OSAS patients as compared to
healthy participants, researchers have turned to experimental testing using various types of off-road driving
simulators.
Results from these studies have shown that patients with OSAS have an increased accident
rate in driving simulation tests (e.g. Findley et al., 1989, 1995; George, Boudreau, & Smiley, 1996; Juniper
et al., 2000; Mazza et al., 2005), estimated to be around two to seven-times higher compared to healthy
participants (e.g., Horne & Reyner, 1999; George, 2004; George et al., 1987; Findley et al., 1989). It has
also been reported that OSAS patients exhibit slower reaction times than controls in road obstacle
avoidance, resulting in four times more object collisions than normals (Findley et al., 1989). In addition, it
has been demonstrated that OSAS patients perform poorer than controls in steering ability (referred to as
“tracking error”; George et al., 1996; Juniper et al., 2000), with half of the patients being worse than any one
control participant, and with some patients showing worse performance than healthy controls under the
influence of alcohol (George et al., 1996). Finally, research conducted to-date have concluded that the
OSAS patients face an increased difficulty in sustaining attention while driving, thus exhibiting poorer
performance and lower vigilance during experimental testing when driving on a monotonous highway route
(e.g. George, Boudreau, & Smiley, 1996; Juniper et al., 2000; Turkington et al., 2001).
Generally, research findings strongly suggest that patients suffering from OSAS have a higher risk of having
MVAs as compared to their healthy counterparts. If sleep apnoea patients are prone to traffic accidents, how
fit are they to drive?
Given that driving is an essential part of everyday life for the majority of people, a series of treatments have
been developed, in order to assist OSAS patients in driving and other daily activities.
Continuous Positive Airway Pressure (CPAP) represents the most commonly used treatment and it is
considered to be the most effective one (Cassel et al., 1996; Yamamoto et al., 2000). Studies have shown
that CPAP treatment can reduce the number of accidents in patients with OSAS, both in simulated driving
(Findley et al., 1989) and in real-life situations (e.g. Cassel et al., 1996; Findley et al., 2000; George,
Boudreau, & Smiley, 1997; Yamamoto et al., 2000). Specifically, studies have shown that regular use of
CPAP improves self-reported (Cassel et al., 1996; Yamamoto et al., 2000) and objective MVA rates (Findley
et al. 2000; George, 2001). Relevant studies have shown that CPAP treatment may effectively reduce the
MVA risk of OSAS patients in experimental tests conducted in a simulated driving setting (e.g. Engleman et
al., 1994; Findley et al., 1989; Note, however, that the task utilised in some of these studies was actually a
choice reaction task that required sustained vigilance rather than a simulated driving task).
If OSAS is a significant contributing factor to MVAs, then it affects both individual and public safety.
98
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Treatment of OSAS using CPAP can improve patient’
s driving performance and, thus, reduce the risk of
MVAs. The findings of previous studies, however, are inconclusive due to the several limitations that can be
identified in their experimental design. For instance, the majority of these studies have predominantly used
male patients as their participants (except in the case of Turkington et al., 2004, where gender was not
reported), thus generalisability of findings to general OSAS population is difficult to be attained.
patients are over-represented in OSAS population compared to female.
Male
One should also note that the
majority of these studies have utilised only a single, brief practice session with a simulated driving task,
which may have not allowed patients to fully understand the task and get familiarised with driving in a
simulated environment.
Another important limitation that can be identified in previous research is the issue of circadian rhythms
influence on participants’performance.
For example, in Orth and colleagues (2005) study, it was reported
that testing was completed over different time periods (i.e. some tests took place in the morning and other at
night) with a number of patients and healthy participants performing similarly in the driving task.
Similalry,
Turkinston and colleagues (2004) did not include a control group in their study, thus it was not possible to
compare patients’performance to control participants in order to investigate whether it was impaired or not.
Finally, a major limitation noted in past research concerns the choice of experimental methodology that may
inadvertently have led to inconclusive findings. For instance, Orth and colleagues (2005) reported null
results in participants’ vigilance testing, while the opposite was reported in the Cassel et al. Study.
Therefore, the choice of the attentional measure used could have been not suitable or not comparable.
Similarly, Turkington et al.’s choice of very brief post- and pre- CPAP treatment time measurements could
have failed to provide representative results of the CPAP effectiveness and therapeutic time course. Last
but not least, Orth and colleagues (2005) utilised a simulator setting that required manual scoring by a
technician, which could have led to possible recording errors, while the majority of simulated driving studies
have only focussed on the participants tracking error measurements, while ignoring the possible wide range
of impairments that could be present in OSAS patients.
On the whole, research to-date identifies OSAS as a significant risk factor to MVAs and supports that
treatment of OSAS using CPAP can reduce this risk. As yet, however, there are no generally accepted
regulations within the Europe Union concerning driving licensing and OSAS. Therefore, it seems pertinent
that these regulations are established both for the benefit of the OSAS patients and the safety of all road
users.
Alcohol remains the greatest documented risk factor in driving performance and the literature is vast on
alcohol effects on fitness to drive. Alcohol is the only substance affecting driving behaviour that legal limits
apply.
Alcohol-impaired driving is a major cause of serious and fatal car accidents. Drunk drivers are
clearly over-represented in road traffic crashes. In Germany, for example, the severity of drink-drive crashes
(expressed as fatalities per 1,000 injury crashes) is nearly twice as high as that of crashes in general
(Swendler et al., 2004).
The relative crash rate for a driver with a BAC of 1.5 g/l is about 22, but drivers’
relative crash rate for fatal crashes with that amount of alcohol in their blood is about 200 (Simpson &
Mayhew, 1991). Individual differences play a sizeable role in the elimination of alcohol from the human
organism. Difference in accident risks is, also, an outcome of causation.
The driving behaviour theoretical perspectives may be clustered into major theoretical grounds; external and
internal. Internal retribution is the research focus of this paper. The next level is a hierarchical distinction
99
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
about the complexity of involved processes; ranging from low-level (e.g. steering) to high level processes
(e.g. speed and route choice). Most theoretical perspectives focus on levels of control and information
processing. Driving is a demanding, complex and multi-aspect task encompassing various skills and their
interactions, such as mental alertness, dexterity, eye-hand coordination, automotised and not , and
perceptual skills (from visual and auditory to sharp decision making) (Weiler et al., 2000).
Drink related driving impairment is well documented in literature and quantified in thresholds or legal limits.
The legal limit in Greece is Blood Alcohol Concentration of 0.05%.
A discussion on zero tolerance is
beyond the scope of this paper, but interest is shifted towards types of skills impaired and/or affected with
taking into account assumptions of inference (i.e. tasks that would manifest themselves as automatic or
more complex).
Alcohol is classified as a depressant, due to its effects to the central nervous system (CNS). Existing
diversity in findings across studies leads to no consensus on the effects on driving impairment in
performance by a given amount of alcohol (34% of studies report impairment by .05%). Current research
techniques have revealed deterioration in driving performance at lower BAC levels. However, Moskowitz
and Robinson (1987) reported in their review that impairment was recorded in psychomotor tasks at a level
of 0.07%.
In addition, simple reaction time score (RT) was found to be an unreliable and insensitive
measure. On the contrary, tracking and divided attention tasks were shown to be impaired at much lower
levels (0.01-0.02%). In most studies deterioration is present above 0.08%.
In the present study, the research team aimed to investigate the level of risk for MVAs for OSAS patients,
while controlling for some of the previous limitations, and to measure whether or not CPAP treatment can
significantly reduce this risk. In order to accomplish this, the researchers compared the performance levels
of CPAP-treated OSAS patients, untreated OSAS patients, and healthy control participants in a series of
simulated driving and laboratory cognitive tasks. Participants were required to complete a series of practice
sessions in order to ensure familiarity with the experimental setting; all practice and experimental sessions
were conducted at the same time of day, hence, avoiding circadian influence on participant’
s performance.
Multiple simulated driving measures were recorded for each participant in order to establish the whole range
of OSAS driving impairment. In the present study, researchers were also interested in comparing the level of
driving impairment of OSAS patients to healthy participants that are intoxicated. This association would
allow to directly defining the level of severity of OSAS with comparing it to alcohol consumption while driving.
Overall the main aims of the study were the following:
•
Investigate OSAS patients’driving performance
•
Measure efficacy of CPAP treatment to improve driving performance of OSAS patients
•
Compare impairment levels caused by OSAS and alcohol consumption
Method
Participants
Eighteen OSAS patients (17 male/1 female; 51.9±11.54 years old) and 18 healthy controls (14 male/4
100
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
2
2
female; 45.5±16.4 years old) with Body Mass Index (BMI) 33.4±7.13 kg/m 26.26±3.25 kg/m , respectively
matched for driving experience volunteered in this study. Participants received reimbursement for their
participation.
The OSAS patients were selected by the collaborating research doctor from a list of patients from the Centre
of Air Medicine IASI. OSAS patients were selected after their participation in a polysomnographic study.
Healthy participants were individuals who responded to an advertisement placed by the research team. All
patients and control participants underwent medical examination to ensure that no other medical condition
was present (except sleep apnoea for patients) and were free of medication. In addition, psychiatric and
cognitive evaluation was performed in both groups in order to ensure that participants had no mental
disorders and/or cognitive impairments.
Moreover, discrete inclusion and exclusion criteria applied.
Exclusion criteria for both groups were positive testing for alcohol (using the LifeLoc Technologies FC20
portable breath tester) or positive testing for cocaine, amphetamines, methamphetamines, opiates,
marijuana/hashish and benzodiazepines (using the Medimpex United, Inc. XALEX 6 Panel Multi Drug Urine
Testing Kit). Participants were tested each time they visited HIT premises.
Inclusion criteria for sleep apnoea patients OSAS was based on a diagnosis of an Apnoea-Hypopnoea Index
(AHI) 10 (after the polysomnographic study). The healthy group was matched as much as possible to the
OSAS group in terms of age and gender. Healthy participants were screened by the affiliated medical
professionals. The healthy group had no medical history in general and, specifically, did not suffer from any
respiratory conditions. Participants in both groups were active and experienced drivers defined as having a
driving license for at least 3 years and driving an annual distance of at least 10000 km. Written informed
consent was obtained from all participants before enrolment and after briefing.
Study design
A mixed design was applied. A repeated within participants’for both OSAS and alcohol consumption groups
of the experiment. In addition, a between participants’design was applied for comparisons between the
control and the patient (OSAS) groups. Independent variables were: a) the CPAP treatment (two conditions:
treated and untreated) and b) alcohol 0.05% consumption (two conditions: intoxicated and sober).
Scenarios’order was counterbalanced to minimise order effects. The experimental protocol was reviewed
and accepted by the Biethics Committee of CERTH3.
Procedure
Untreated OSAS patients were tested and then re-tested after having used the CPAP treatment continuously
for at least 7 days. The healthy group was tested in two conditions, with BAC 0% and with BAC 0.05%,
which is the legal limit for driving in Greece. Tests were performed at the same time of day for each
participant, in the afternoon between 14:00 and 17:00. Control participants had no history of alcohol abuse.
In order to calculate the BAC of the participants the equation described by the National Highway Traffic
Safety Administration (NHTSA, 1994)4 was applied ([23.36 * (SD * 0.5)] / [W * GC * 1000]) * 0.806 * 100- [T *
3
CERTH: Centre for Research and Technology is a non-profit organisation
and Hellenic Institute of Transport (HIT) is part of this large research centre
4
National Highway Traffic Safety Administration (Herbert Moskowitz, Ph.D.)
http://www.nhtsa.dot.gov/people/injury/alcohol/bacreport.html
101
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
0.017], where W = the weight of the participant in kg; GC = 0.58 for males/0.49 for females; SD = the
number of standard drinks consumed; and T = time in hours since the start of the first sip. This formula is an
updated Widmark’s formula and was utilised because it takes in to account the following variables: total body
water (from weight and gender); alcohol dose (from number of standard drinks), which is used to calculate
the amount of alcohol in body water and in the bloodstream; and rate of elimination (using averaged
metabolism rates and hours since start of drinking event).
Adequate time (approximately 15 minutes) had to pass in order to check the alcohol level with the
breathanalyser. Time had to pass in order to avoid measuring residuals in the mouth cavity. In case
participants did not reach the required level, additional shots of alcohol were administered (3 ml) till 0.05%
was reached. The level of alcohol had to be sustained throughout the experiment; hence breathanalysis was
performed prior and post driving scenarios and in-between neuropsychological tasks.
Participants were asked to arrive at HIT at the same time of day for both of their tests as described above.
At their first appointment they completed a background questionnaire and written consent was obtained after
briefing about the main objectives of the study. After both alcohol screening with breathanalyser and urine
drug screening were found negative, participants had a familiarisation drive in order to get used to the driving
simulator.
Participants had to complete the following tasks at the driving simulator:
a) a lane tracking scenario for about 20 minutes on a highway environment maintaining a constant speed of
90 km/h and
b) a car following scenario for about 20 minutes on a highway environment maintaining a safe distance from
the lead vehicle that was moving with a steady speed of 90 km/h. The vehicle ahead would brake abruptly
suddenly and unexpectatly during the scenario.
Participants were instructed to maintain a constant speed of 90 km/h and steady lateral position (lane
tracking scenario) and to maintain a safe distance to the lead vehicle (car following scenario). The order of
the two scenarios was counter-balanced between the two tests and among participants. Participants were
driving alone with no radio on and researchers were controlling for environmental noise for both driving and
laboratory tests. After the simulator drives the participants had to complete a test battery on attentional
performance (winTAP) (Zimmermann and Fimm, 1993).
Participants were asked not to have coffee or alcohol at least 24 hours prior testing. EOG recordings were
obtained whilst driving and during the neuropsychological tests.
Participants had breaks in between
scenarios and if they required more time they had extra breaks. Participants were closely observed in order
to check for any simulator sickness symptoms (e.g. sweating, yawning). Subjective scales were filled in
before and after each driving scenario and before and after each attentional test. Detailed accounts of the
driving scenarios, subjective scales, attentional battery, and physiological measures are provided in the
respective sections.
Driving and psychomotor tests
The CERTH/HIT driving simulator (Figure 1 in Chapter 3) is built around a Smart cabin equipped with
sensors.
The actuation of all control levers, windshield wipers, blinker, ignition key and light switch is
electronically transmitted to the driving computer.
102
All operational elements such as steering wheel,
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
accelerator pedal, brake pedal, gearshift lever and handbrake lever, provide nature-true force reactions. The
visual system includes five large-screens, each having a width of 2 m. There is on-screen projection with
video projectors (2500 ANSI-lumen). The sound system generates original sounds according to the situation
(starter, engine noise, horn, screeching of tires, drive wind, rain, etc.). The vibration device creates nature
true vibrations of the car according to the revolvation of the simulated engine.
The simulator is equipped with special software which allows the development of specific driving scenarios
aiming at creating a monotonous driving environment. There is surrounding traffic with maximum 30 road
users with embedded artificial intelligence elements including passenger cars, trucks, pedestrians and
cyclists.
The primary driving variables were the standard deviation of lateral position (SDLP) for the lane tracking
scenario and the percentage (%) of time driven with certain Time-To-Collision (TTC) values. SDLP is a very
reliable index of weaving and overall lateral control of the vehicle. The car following scenario was designed
with stable leading vehicle speed. In this scenario the percentage of time (%) spent with Time-to-Collision
(TTC) falling into five categories was calculated. The selected five categories were the following. As there is
no previous literature on recommended stratification, the decision was based on the interest to investigate
more clusters for the riskier TTCs, one cluster reflecting following behaviour, and one cluster for investigating
non-conforming to given instructions behaviour.
TTC 0-1: percentage of time driven with TTC between 0 and 1 sec
TTC 1-1.5: percentage of time driven with TTC between 1 and 1.5 sec
TTC 1.5-2: percentage of time driven with TTC between 1.5 and 2 sec
TTC 2-4: percentage of time driven with TTC between 2 and 4 sec
TTC >4: percentage of time driven with TTC above 4 sec
Obviously, very low TTC values suggest there is higher risk to be involved in an accident in the car following
scenario. In addition, more time spent in the category TTC
2-4
implies that participants spent more time in
following the leading vehicle with keeping a safety distance, hence complying with given instructions. More
time spent with TTC >4 suggests that participants were less concentrated and not attentive to the driving
task and, consequently, did not follow the driving instructions all the time. Failure to follow instructions might
be a sign of fatigue and lowered concentration levels.
The winTAP (Zimmermann and Fimm, 1993) is a standardised neuropsychological battery of attentional
performance. The selected individual tests applied were the following: a) alertness: choice between two
stimuli, b) Go/NoGo: simple choice reaction task, c) divided attention audio: reaction to two consecutive
same sounds (either low or high), d) divided attention visual: reaction to the formation of a small rectangular
by small stars, and e) divided attention with both visual and auditory stimuli: presented simultaneously.
Reaction times (msec), omissions and errors were recorded.
Secondary measures
Another parameter of interest was the time it takes to react to an abrupt breaking of the lead vehicle.
Reaction times (sec) to abrupt breaking of the lead vehicle was recorded for the car following scenario. In
103
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
addition EOG recordings were gathered.
Subjective measures
Participants were asked to subjectively evaluate their vigilance state. The Karolinska Sleepiness Scale
(KSS) was applied.
KSS is a universally accepted, validated and standardised scale which was
administered before and after the task completion across all conditions. The 9-point KSS (Åkerstedt and
Gillberg, 1990) was used: 1=very alert, 3=alert, 5=neither alert nor sleepy, 7=sleepy (but not fighting sleep),
9=very sleepy (fighting sleep). Sleepiness was subjectively rated before and after each driving scenario and
before and after the neuropsychological tests were performed. Higher scoring meant less vigilance and
subsequently increased sleepiness.
In general, participants were less vigilant just before the
neuropsychological tests and more vigilant at the beginning. In addition, participants rated how much they
had to try in order to complete the driving tasks [Rating Scale of Mental Effort (0-150); RSME] (Zijlstra, 1993)
and how well they thought they performed in the driving tasks compared to their everyday driving experience
(Driving Quality scale, Brookhuis et al., 1985). tThe original subjective scales were translated into Greek and
back-translated by an independent professional and then verified by a native English speaker.
Statistical analysis
Within and between participants comparisons (superiority tests) were carried out with repeated measures
ANOVAs and one-way ANOVAs. In case of violation of homogeneity and homoscedacity assumptions, nonparametric equivalents were administered (Friedman and Wilcoxon rank test, respectively).
Within
comparisons were carried out in order to investigate the effect of CPAP treatment and alcohol level (0.05%).
Between comparisons were carried out in order to investigate the relationship between OSAS and alcohol in
driving impairment. The
level was set at .05. Statistical analyses were carried out with SPSS 18.0 for
Windows (SPSS 18.0, Chicago, IL.).
Results
This section is divided further in 4 subsections according to the types of collected data. Firstly, the main
primary driving parameters are presented per driving scenario (i.e. lane tracking and car following).
Secondly, additional neuropsychological measures are presented. Thirdly, EOG findings are presented and
finally subjective scales results are given.
Driving parameters (primary variables)
Lane tracking
OSAS patients showed more deteriorated performance in lane keeping (SDLP) than any other condition.
CPAP treatment did not seem to significantly improve lane keeping behaviour, although decreased swerving
is recorded. The following graph depicts mean SDLP (m) values per condition.
104
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
*p = .027
**p <.001
***p <.001
****p <.001
Figure 1: Mean SDLP (m) values per condition in lane tracking scenario
As shown above, the OSAS patients had less control over the lateral position of the vehicle before CPAP
treatment. After treatment, lateral control increases but not significantly (p>.05). However, the tracking
control is still impaired compared to suggested thresholds (Brookhuis et al., 2003) and when compared to
the control group before (F (1,34) = 10.57, p<.001)** and after alcohol consumption (F (1,34) = 8.23,
p<.001)***. In addition, intoxicated control group weaving was greater compared to the no alcohol condition
(F (1,17) = 5.9, p=.027)*. Alcohol had an effect on participants’lateral control but the magnitude was not as
great as the sleep apnoea’
s effect on OSAS patients****.
Car following
Percentage of time (%) spent with Time-to-Collision (TTC)
The results for TTC comparisons among conditions are described separately for each chosen percentage of
time (%) driven TTC category and are summarised in the following two tables:
105
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
CONTROL GROUP
Baseline
Driving
measures
Alcohol
Significance
Mean±SE
[95% CI]
Mean±SE
[95% CI]
Car Following
TTC0-1 (sec)
6.35±0.44
(5.41 – 7.28)
5.7±0.36
(4.93 – 6.46)
*
TTC1-1.5 (sec)
7.17±0.49
(6.14 – 8.21)
10.05±0.77
(8.43 – 11.66)
*
TTC1.5-2 (sec)
13.52±1.03
(11.35 – 15.68)
13.53±1.28
(10.82 – 16.23)
NS
TTC2-4 (sec)
35.97±1.26
(33.32 – 38.62)
33.44±1.33
(30.63 – 36.24)
*
TTC>4 (sec)
37.64±1.95
(33.53 – 41.75)
36.64±2.11
(32.18 – 41.1)
NS
Table 1: Mean (±SE) scores and 95% confidence intervals of percentage of time spent (%) driven with TTC
values according to the TTC clusters for the control group (alcohol consumption).
* Significant (p<.05); NS: Non-significant (p>.05).
OSAS PATIENTS
No treatment
Driving
measures
CPAP treatment
Significance
Mean±SE
[95% CI]
Mean±SE
[95% CI]
Car Following
TTC0-1 (sec)
6.81±0.57
(5.67 – 7.94)
6.37±0.54
(5.16 – 7.58)
*
TTC1-1.5 (sec)
11.83±1.33
(9.02 – 14.47)
11.95±1.39
(9.01 – 14.89)
NS
TTC1.5-2 (sec)
16.94±1.36
(13.33 – 19.06)
14.58±1.39
(11.65 – 17.5)
*
TTC2-4 (sec)
29.94±1.27
(27.26 – 32.62)
32.37±1.15
(29.95 – 34.79)
*
TTC>4 (sec)
35.23±3.21
(28.46 – 41.99)
34.73±3.27
(27.82 – 41.64)
NS
Table 2: Mean (±SE) scores and 95% confidence intervals for the time spent (%) driven with TTC values
according to the TTC clusters for the OSAS patients. * Significant (p<.05); NS: Non-significant (p>.05).
•
Percentage (%) of time with TTC0-1
Within and between comparisons were carried out for each TTC category. Untreated (6.81±0.57% of time)
OSAS patients spent significantly more time with TTC values between 0 and 1 sec which is extremely risky
(F (1,17) = 5.46, p = .032) compared to the CPAP treated condition (6.37±0.54% of time).
106
DRUID 6th framework programme
Similarly,
Deliverable 1.2.2
participants from the control group (6.35±0.44% of time) spent almost significantly more time with TTC
values between 0 and 1 second when compared to the alcohol consumption condition (5.7±0.36% of time)
(F(1,17) = 4.27, p = .054). Between groups’comparisons were not significant (p>.05).
•
Percentage (%) of time with TTC 1-1.5
Intoxicated participants (10.05±0.77% of time) spent approximately three percent more time with TTC
between the values 1-1.5 seconds compared to the no alcohol condition (7.18±0.49% of time) (F(1,17) =
34.68, p<.001). Similarly, untreated (11.83±1.33% of time) and treated OSAS patients (11.95±1.39% of
time) spent statistically significant more time (around 4%) with TTC values between 0 and 1 second
compared to the control group (7.17±0.49% of time) (F(1,34) = 10.73, p=.002 and F(1,34) = 10.46, p=.003,
respectively). All other comparisons were of no statistical significance (p>.05).
•
Percentage (%) of time with TTC 1.5-2
The only statistically significant difference was found between OSAS treated (14.58±1.39% of time) and
untreated patients (16.94±1.36% of time) (F(1,17) = 8.63, p =.009).
No other statistically significant
difference was found within and between groups. Nevertheless, untreated OSAS patients and intoxicated
participants spent more time than the other two conditions within the TTC values 1.5 and 2.
•
Percentage (%) of time with TTC 2-4
Almost all comparisons were statistically significant (Figure 2) except the comparison between the treated
OSAS group and the alcohol group (p>.05). As shown in the graph below, treated OSAS patients spent
significantly more time (3%) compared to the time they spend driven with TTC
2-4
before treatment ( F(1,17)
= 18.05, p = .001). Likewise, participants in the control group spent more time (approx. 2.5%) with TTC
2-4
(F(1,17) = 8.93, p = .008) than the intoxicated group. Untreated OSAS patients spent more time than the
control group in TTC 2-4 (F(1,34) = 11.42, p = .002).
This TTC category is strongly connected to safety distance keeping; hence the significant difference among
groups could reflect risk taking behaviour. The control group spent significantly more time (3%) than the
treated OSAS patients in car following (F(1,34) = 4.49, p =.041).
On the other hand, no statistically
significant differences between the OSAS treated group and the alcohol group (p>.05) were found. Similarly,
no significant difference were found between the alcohol and the untreated OSAS patients groups (p = .066;
trend).
107
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Figure 2: Mean percentage (%) of time driven with TTC 2-4
•
Percentage (%) of time with TTC >4
No statistically significant differences were found for both within and between group comparisons (p>.05).
However, the control group spent more time with TTC values greater than four and treated OSAS group
spent less time than the rest of groups/conditions with TTC values greater than four; the range of
differences, though, was not greater than 3%.
Braking reaction time (sec)
Greater reaction times (sec) were recorded for the untreated OSAS patients and the lowest reaction times
(sec) were recorded for the CPAP treated patients (F (1,17)=12.37, p =.003). Moreover, it seems that
alcohol did not have an impact in braking reaction time for the alcohol group (p>.05) at the legal limit. On the
contrary, participants from the control group reacted (brake) much faster when they were sober compared to
the untreated OSAS patients (F (1,17)=6.55, p=.015).
Secondary variables
This section includes other measurements supporting the primary variables. The secondary measurements
included are the results from the neuropsychological tests performed (WinTAP battery) and the EOG
measurements.
108
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Neuropsychological measures
Control participants were statistically significant less alert when intoxicated (F (1,17) = 9.39, p=.007).
Intoxicated participants showed statistically significant higher Reaction times (RTs) (ms) in the divided visual
attention task compared to OSAS untreated patients (F (1,34) = 5.04, p<.032).
Figure 3: Mean reaction time (msec) in neuropsychological tests
Participants after alcohol consumption (0.05%) had significantly more omissions than CPAP treated OSAS
patients (F(1,158) = 8.55, p=.004) when they had to react to both visual and auditory stimuli. No other
statistically significant differences were found (either in errors and/or omissions).
Electrooculograph (EOG)
The following parameters were statistically analysed in both car following and lane tracking scenarios:
•
Blink duration
•
Number of blinks
•
Closing time (time from baseline to peak)
•
Reopening time (time from peak to return to baseline)
109
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
The parameters were calculated in continuous time windows of 1 minute. During lane tracking task, none of
the parameters were significantly different between CPAP and no CPAP runs. However, during car following
task the number of blinks was significantly (p<.001) higher in patients with no CPAP (figure 4). It appears
that OSAS patients before CPAP treatment had more eye-blinks than after CPAP treatment.
Figure 4: Number of blinks
Moreover, the closing time was significantly (p<.001) lower in patients with no CPAP (figure 5). It is evident
that patients with CPAP had longer closing time than those without it.
Figure 5: Closing time (sec)
No other statistically significant differences were found regarding EOG measurements (p>.05).
Subjective scales
Higher scores are observed for OSAS patients in almost all conditions.
Higher KSS scores for
neuropsychological tests may be due to the fact that attention tests were carried out at the end of the
experiment.
Therefore, participants could have been tired near the end of the experiment (i.e. added
tiredness from all conditions) and not only because of the effort required to perform the attentional tests.
110
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Figure 6: Mean RSME and median KSS scores
Figure 7: Median KSS scores
Regarding subjective feeling of the quality of their driving experience (DQS) in the simulated environment, in
comparison to how they normally drive, only 3 participants with OSAS mentioned that they drove worse than
usual. Similarly, the same participants thought that they drove worse than usual even after CPAP treatment.
One participant stated that he drove worse than usual in the control condition. The number of participants
that they thought they drove worse than usual after alcohol consumption increased to 5.
111
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Discussion
The main findings demonstrated that sleep apnoea affects driving performance. Continuous Positive Airway
Pressure (CPAP) treatment did not seem to improve significantly driving performance. Furthermore, the
present experiment suggests that alcohol consumption at BAC=.05% impairs driving performance; however
deterioration is less when compared to sleep apnoea. Equivalence might be present for brake reaction time
(sec) between sleep apnoea and alcohol effect.
The present study showed that sleepiness and hypo-vigilance induced by sleep apnoea may be an
increasingly contributing factor in road accidents which are sometimes overlooked in driving research
focusing mainly on alcohol and illicit drugs. In the present experiment, OSAS patients showed almost one
third more weaving than the control group participants (31.46%). Many studies have shown increased
accident risk for OSAS patients because of micro-sleeps, though a considerable amount of research is
based on questionnaires and self-reporting assessments.
According to self reports, OSAS patients
experience sleepiness while driving because of lack of sleep or lack of quality sleep and, thus, report higher
rate of driving related accidents. Treatment with nasal Continuous Positive Airway Pressure (nasal CPAP)
has been found to decrease self-reported automobile crashes in patients with sleep apnoea. These studies
have been carried out with knowledge of subjectivity error and biases involved.
The step towards objectification of this type of research was conducted by Findley and colleagues (2000)
who based their hypothesis about CPAP treatment decreasing automobile accidents based on accidents
reports from the Department of Motor Vehicle. These data were supported by telephone interviews on the
effect of the nasal CPAP treatment on their fitness to drive, description of sleeping hours, miles driven per
week etc. It is essential to note that compliance to treatment was based on self-reports and although
objective measures were taken into account –as far as reporting is concerned- the absence of a control
group is important.
In this study, OSAS patients reported higher sleepiness rates compared to the other groups which is in line
with findings from self-reported based studies discussed in the previous paragraph.
OSAS patients showed deteriorated driving performance in monotonous simulated environment which is in
agreement with similar studies. For instance, Orth and colleagues (2005) found improvement due to nasal
CPAP application. The analysis was based on accident crashes and concentration faults which are different
parameters from the ones recorded in this study. Although accident risk is of fundamental importance in
driving safety research and driving scenarios included random elements (e.g. obstacles’occurrence such as
a deer or a vehicle) there was no control group in order to compare crashes across conditions.
Furthermore, this study targeted different types of vehicle parameters and assessment variables than the
one performed by CERTH/HIT.
Moreover, several studies have shown significant improvement in driving behaviour as a result of using the
nasal CPAP for different periods of time. For instance, Loredo and colleagues (1999) found differences in
CPAP treatment after 7 days. The same treatment period was used in this study based on Loredo et al.’
s
(1999) findings. The differences were not found on the quality of sleep (sleep architecture) and, thus,
changes in this type of sleep may be influential in order to find improvement in driving variables such as the
ones measured in this experiment (SDLP, %TTC, and BRT). In other words, the period of treatment time
112
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
may not suffice in order to reveal improvement in driving behaviour when the driving task is performed in a
monotonous simulated environment with these specifically set and derived parameters. In addition, the
monotonous environment may be the most “dangerous”choice or the most “accident-evoking”but it is far
from a real traffic situation and induces sleepiness beyond control which reflects probably the worse-case
scenario and not necessarily the most frequent situation.
Turkington and colleagues (2004) focused on similar driving parameters.
They aimed to find the most
effective period of time for CPAP application and found that the threshold is seven consecutive days (similar
to Loredo and colleagues findings but for different reasons). While they found significant tracking error
(p=.004), reaction time (p=.036), and number of off road events per hour (p=.032) -which are parameters
related to the ones measured in this experiment-it is important to note that the cases of OSAS patients
included were much more severe cases than the general population and the ones included in this study.
Therefore significant findings in 7 days treatment may be generalised only to a certain percentage of severe
patients and not to other groups of mild and less severe OSAS. As a result, it is inconclusive if the
significant improvement in tracking error –which is the most relevant measure- in 7 days treatment with nasal
CPAP, would be found if the cases included were milder. The size of effect might have been inflated due to
the initially measured deterioration.
The percentage (%) of time spent driving within certain categories of TTC values has not been investigated
in the literature before, thus, it is difficult to examine the TTC results under the prism of research-to-date and
to step forward towards any generalisable inferences. Decrease in crashes, as mentioned above, might be
associated with the significant less time spent with TTC values less than 2 seconds in treated OSAS patients
(p=.032). Therefore, the accident risk might be reduced. This logical induction should serve simply to show
probable relations in driving variables between the two studies and not actually compare effects and findings.
Overall significant changes in percentage of time spent with TTC values less than two seconds were found in
both treated and control group, meaning that CPAP treatment reduced risky behaviour and that alcohol even
at legal limit (0.05%) increases risky behaviour.
Likewise, safety distance keeping from lead vehicle
temporally increases for both control participants and CPAP treated patients. Safer behaviour is observed in
treated patients and the opposite seems to hold true for untreated patients and intoxicated participants.
Driving impairment due to alcohol consumption, even for the legal national limit (0.05%), is evident in both
lane and car following scenarios. One of the main objectives was to investigate the relation of sleep apnoea
effect and alcohol in specific driving variables. Inferences should be conservative and differences were not
found for brake reaction time (sec). This finding suggests that braking delay might be similar for sleep
apnoea patients and intoxicated participants (alcohol consumption at legal limit).
Brake reaction time (BRT) is the time to respond to sudden changes in the driving environment by fully
depressing a brake pedal. Previous research has identified possible risk factors associated with delayed
brake reaction time, such as alcohol use (Kuypers et al., 2006) and medications causing sedation such as
antihistamines or psychotropic agents (Vuurman et al., 2004). Previous research has identified two primary
components of Brake Reaction Time (BRT).
The first component is the Initial Reaction Time (IRT,
sometimes referred to as perception-reaction time), and the second is Physical Reaction Time (PRT, also
called brake-movement time). IRT is the time from when the stimulus first appears to the start of foot
movement off the accelerator; the PRT is the time from the first physical movement off the accelerator to the
depression of the brake pedal.
113
Research suggests these components could be influenced by different
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
factors, but they may not be entirely independent like gender and age (Warshawsky-Livne and Shiner,
2002). Therefore, by controlling these factors, it might be possible to reveal effects. A future research effort
could focus on investigating the effect of different levels of alcohol (BAC) specifically in the two
aforementioned brake reaction components. Such inferences could even support the refining of existing
rehabilitation programmes.
Regarding neuropsychological assessment the findings are not in accordance with relevant studies of sleep
apnoea as CPAP treatment did not seem to improve patients’alertness. In general, no major differences
were found in alertness, divided attention, and simple choice task. Alcohol impairing effect was found in
alertness. Intoxicated participants felt significantly less alert after alcohol consumption. However, the effect
of awareness of being intoxicated was not controlled. A placebo-alcohol group would accommodate for this
possibility. It seems that the effect in visual divided attention was significantly higher in alcohol group
compared to OSAS patients. The findings suggest that alcohol at 0.05% affects more participants’alertness
and visual attention after alcohol consumption than obstructive sleep apnoea does.
A great deal of research has been carried out for determining the effects of alcohol on the capacity to divide
attention (e.g. Patel, 1988; Moskowitz and Burns, 1990). As most of these studies investigated the effects of
high or moderate blood-alcohol levels, the deterioration revealed was detrimental. Investigation of potential
effects of low levels of alcohol (i.e. legal limit) could yield interesting results and shift research interest
towards the processes underlying the distribution of attentional resources in intoxicated drivers (i.e. their
scores in divided attention tasks). No significant differences were found between control and intoxicated
participants. The alcohol level was either low for revealing such deterioration or participants tried too hard to
compensate for it.
Moreover, a future investigation of alcohol effect in distribution of attention would be focussing on how this
“mobility”of attention –as it is often called- affects attentional shift in driving tasks. The latter could move
research beyond increased or decreased measures of effect and provide valuable input for the development
of alcohol related guidelines. Also, the definition of the qualitative characteristics of attentional mobility could
prove valuable for further development of rehabilitation measures.
Subjective assessments showed that participants under alcohol consumption thought that they drove worse
than the rest of the participants. OSAS patients did not report any difference in driving quality which is in
agreement with CPAP treatment results.
As expected, OSAS patients reported higher sleepiness and
increased mental effort while driving.
Indeed, OSAS participants showed greater weaving and,
consequently, less control than the rest of participants. However, not great differences were found for
mental effort within each group.
Overall, it seems that the effect of sleep apnoea is detrimental compared to the alcohol effect at 0.05%. The
application of intermediate alcohol BAC levels (i.e.
0.02, 0.08., 0.1) could provide insight in finding
comparable levels of impairment. Probably higher levels of alcohol are required in order to reveal any
equivalence to OSAS for the chosen types of driving parameters.
Following the need for further stratification of alcohol BAC levels within the same study, research focus could
also be shifted towards the quality of sleep affected in OSAS patients and the appropriate and/or associated
variables. For example, automatic processes might be more difficult to control while driving, hence easier to
investigate improvements in less treatment time (e.g. in seven consecutive days) than other driving skills
(e.g. safety distance may need 30 days).
114
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Participants said they used the nasal CPAP machine on a daily basis. The research team had only this
information about the quality and frequency about the application of the CPAP machine during the night as
they were all outpatients.
Last, equivalence testing could be oriented to the study of the two components of brake reaction time for
alcohol and other medicines.
Acknowledgements
This work was conducted with the assistance of a specialised team of medical experts, led by Dr. Chrysoula
Papadeli, experienced in diagnosis and treatment of OSAS patients. Medical professionals were responsible
for the patients’recruitment, EOG measurements and analyses of respective results.
115
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
References
Åkerstedt, T. and Gillberg, M. (1990). Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual.
International Journal of Neuroscience, 52, 29–37.
Brookhuis, K.A., De Waard, D., Fairclough, S.H. (2003). Criteria for driver impairment. Ergonomics, 46, 433445.
Brookhuis, K.A., DeVries, G., Prins Van Wijngaarden, P., Veenstra, G., Hommes, M., Louwerens, J.W.,
O’
Hanlon, J.F. (1985). The effects of increasing doses of Meptazisol (100, 200, 400 mg) and Glaferine
(200mg) on driving performance. Report VK 85-16. Haren, The Nethrlands: Traffic Research Center,
University of Groningen.
Cassel, W., Ploch, T., Becker, C., Dugnus, D., Peter, J. H., von Wichert, P. (1996). Risk of traffic accidents in
patients with sleepdisordered breathing: reduction with nasal CPAP. European Respiratory Journal, 9,
2606-2611.
Engleman, H. M., & Wild, M. R. (2003). Improving CPAP use by patients with the sleep apnoea/hypopnea
syndrome (SAHS). Sleep Medicine Review, 7, 81-99.
Findley, L. J., Fabrizio, M. J., Knight, H., Norcross, B. B., Laforte, A. J., & Suratt, P. M. (1989). Driving
simulator performance in patients with sleep apnea. American Review of Respiratory Disease, 140,
529-530.
Findley, L., & Suratt, P. M. (2001). Serious motor vehicle crashes: the cost of untreated sleep apnea. Thorax,
56, 505.
Findley, L. J., Unverzagt, M., Guchu, R., Fabrizio, M., Buckner, J., & Suratt, P. (1995). Vigilance and
automobile accidents in patients with sleep apnea or narcolepsy. Chest, 108, 619-624.
George, C. F. P. (2001). Motor vehicle collisions are reduced when sleep apnoea is treated with nasal
CPAP. Thorax, 56, 508-512.
George, C. F. P. (2004). Driving and automobile crashes in patients with obstructive sleep
apnoea/syndrome. Thorax, 59, 804-807.
George, C. F. P., Boudreau, A. C., & Smiley, A. (1996). Simulated driving performance in patients with
obstructive sleep apnea. American Journal of Respiratory Critical Care Medicine, 154, 175-181.
George, C., Nickerson, P., Hanley, P., Millar, T., & Kryger, M. (1987). Sleep apnea patients have more
automobile accidents. Lancet, 2, A447.
Hack, M. A., Choi, S. J., Vijayapalan, P., Davies, R. J. O., & Stranling, J. R. (2001). Comparison of the
effects of sleep deprivation, alcohol, and obstructive sleep apnoea (OSA) on simulated steering
performance. Respiratory Medicine, 95, 594-601.
Horne, J. A., & Reyner, L. A. (1995). Driver sleepiness. Journal of Sleep Research, 4, 23-29.
Horne, J. A., & Reyner, L. A. (1999). Vehicle accidents related to sleep: a review. Occupational
Environmental Medicine, 56, 289-294.
Juniper, M., Hack, M. A., George, C. F. et al. (2000). Steering simulation performance in patients with
obstructive sleep apnoea and matched control subjects. European Respiratory Journal, 15, 590-595.
Kuypers, K.P., Samyn, N., Ramaekers, J.G. (2006). MDMA and alcohol effects, combined and alone, on
objective and subjective measures of actual driving performance and psychomotor function.
Psychopharmacology. 187,467–475.
Maycock, G. (1996). Sleepiness and driving: the experience of UK car drivers. Journal of Sleep Research, 5,
229-37.
Moskowitz, H., Robinson, C.D. (1987). Effects of low doses of alcohol on driving–related skills: a review of
116
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
the evidence. In: Technical Report, United States Department of Transportation, Washington, D.C.,
USA.
Moskowitz, H., Burns, M. (1990). Effects of alcohol on driving performance. Alcohol Health & Research
World, 14, 12-14.
Naegele, B., Thouvard, V., Pepin, J. L., et al. (1995). Deficits of cognitive executive functions in patients with
sleep apnea syndrome. Sleep, 18, 43-52.
Orth, M., Duchna, H.-W., Leidag, M., Widdig, W., Rasche, K., Bauer, T. T., Walther, J. W., de Zeeuw, J.,
Malin, J.-P., Schultze-Werninghaus, G., & Kotterba, S. (2005). Driving simulator and
neuropsychological testing in OSAS before and under CPAP therapy. European Respiratory Journal,
26, 898-903.
Patel, R.M. (1988). Ethanol’s effect on human vigilance during a simple task in the presence of an auditory
stressor. Psychological Reports, 63, 363-366.
Simpson, H.M. & Mayhew, D.R. (1991). The hard core drinking driver. Traffic Injury Research Foundation of
Canada, Ottawa.
Sullivan, C. E., Issa, F. G., Berthon-Jones, N., et al. (1981). Reversal of obstructive sleep apnoea by
continuous positive airway pressure applied through the nars. Lancet, 1, 862-865.
Swedler, B.M, Biecheler, M.B., Laurell, H., Kroj, G., Lerner, M., Mathijssen, M.P.M., Mayhew, D. &
Tunbridge, R.J. (2004). Worldwide trends in alcohol and drug impaired driving. In: Traffic Injury
Prevention Volume 5, Issue 3. p. 175-184.
Teran-Santos, J., Jimenez-Gomez, A., Cordero-Guevara, J., et al. (1999). The association between sleep
apnea and the risk of traffic accidents. New England Journal of Medicine, 340, 847-851.
Turkington, P. M., Sircar, M., Saralaya, D., & Elliott, M. W. (2004). Time course of changes in driving
simulator performance with and without treatment in patients with sleep apnoea hypopnoea syncrome.
Thorax, 59, 56-59.
Vuurman EF, Rikken GH, Muntjewerff ND, de Halleux F, Ramaekers JG. Effects of desloratadine,
diphenydramine and placebo on driving performance and psychomotor performance measurements.
Eur J Clin Pharmacol. 2004;60:307–313.
Warshawsky-Livne L, Shiner D. Effects of uncertainty, transmission type, driver age and gender on brake
reaction and movement time. J Safety Res. 2002;33:117–128.
Weiler, J. M., Bloomfield, J. R., Woodworth, G. G., Grant, A. R., Layton, T. A., Brown, T. L., McKenzie, D. R.,
and Baker, D. W. (2000). Effects of Fexofenadine, Diphenidramine, and Alcohol on driving
performance. Annals of Internal Medicine, 132, (5), 354-363.
Wu, H., & Yan-Go F. (1996). Self-reported automobile accidents involving patients with obstructive sleep
apnea. Neurology, 46, 1254-1257.
Yamamoto, H., Akashiba, T., Kosaka, N., Ito, D., Horie, T. (2000). Longterm effects of nasal continuous
positive airway pressure on daytime sleepiness, mood and traffic accidents in patients with obstructive
sleep apnoea. Respiratory Medicine, 94, 87-90.
Zijlstra, F.R.H. (1993). Efficiency in work behavior. A design approach for modern tools. PhD thesis, Delft
University of Technology. Delft, The Netherlands: Delft University Press.
Zimmermann, P. & Fimm, B. (1993). Testbatterie zur Erfassung von Aufmerksamkeitsstörungen. Version
1.02. Freiburg: Psytest.
117
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Chapter 5 : effects of codoliprane and zolpidem,
alone or in combination, on elderly drivers’
behavior
ML Bocca (UNICAEN), P. Denise (UNICAEN), C. Berthelon (IFSTTAR).
118
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Abstract
The majority of older adults regularly use several medications, in particular due to an increase of insomnia
with age, but also due to an increase of pain. Paradoxically, most of experimental studies on drugs effects
are conducted on healthy young subjects.
The aim of the present study was to evaluate the effects of the combination of zolpidem 10 mg taken at
bedtime and codoliprane taken at awakening (codeine 20 mg - paracetamol 400 mg) in aged subjects. It
was a cross-over, double-blind, placebo-controlled study. Four combinations were tested: placebo-placebo,
zolpidem-placebo, zolpidem-codoliprane, placebo-codoliprane.
Subjects performed three driving test at 9.00 am : monotonous, urban and car following. Sixteen healthy
subjects aged 55 to 65 years participated in this experiment.
Standart Deviation of Lateral Position (SDLP) was significantly increased both with zolpidem and with
codeine-paracetamol alone. The combination of these two drugs are less clear but tendencies were found.
Results are not significant with urban driving test.
Globally, results confirm that zolpidem or codoliprane in aged subjects impair driving performance, a result
no found in young subjects. Although unexpected, the combination of two drugs did not clearly impair the
driving behaviour in the highway driving task.
119
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Introduction
Epidemiological data clearly indicate that hypnotic drugs were present in a significant percentage of drivers
involved in accidents (Barbone et al. 1998, Neutel 1998, Orriols et al., 2010), whatever the driver’
s age
(Movig et al., 2004), thus suggesting that using these drugs increases the risk of accident . This risk has also
been demonstrated in many experimental studies (for a review, see Vermeeren 2004) when taken at
therapeutic or at higher doses (Walsh et al. 2004).
Although the elderly, and notably women, account for a growing share of the driving population (Maycock
2000) and that the use of psychotropic drugs is common in this population (Legrain 2005), information on the
effects of these drugs in this population are scarce. Recent experimental studies however showed that some
hypnotics, without residual effects on young drivers’behaviour, do have residual effects on elderly drivers’
behaviour (Meskali, Berthelon et al., 2009; Bocca, Marie et al. under press). Older people may thus be
differently and more often affected by drugs than younger people, due to age-related changes in
pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics, such as reduced clearance and increased sensitivity (Glass,
Lanctot et al., 2005) and physiological changes that affect how older people metabolize the medications.
In another hand, a large part of elderly regularly use antalgics and this type of drug could also increase the
risk of accident (McGwin, Sims et al. 2000, Orriols et al. 2010). In particular, epidemiological studies find low
relative risk for opioid users (Fishbain et al. 2002; Kelly et al. 2004), but some recent studies show that
natural opium alkaloids could increase the risk of being involved in an accident (Bachs et al. 2009; Dubois et
al. 2010; Engeland et al. 2007; Kelly et al. 2004). Our own study does not show acute effects of antalgics on
young healthy drivers’behavior (Amato, Marie et al., submitted).
Whatever the case, all data are in agreement with the fact that using multiple medications or polypharmacy
increases with higher age and is linked with an increased risk of accident (Gjerde, Normann et al. in press).
The main objective of the present study is to evaluate residual effects of a hypnotics (zolpidem 10mg), an
acute effect of an analgesic (codeine / paracetamol 20mg/400mg) and of the combination of these two
molecules in healthy elderly subjects (55 to 65 years old) to assess a potential increase effect of this
combination on driving performance.
Method
Subjects
The study was carried out on 16 healthy volunteers (8 men and 8 women, mean age = 59.6 + 2.2 years).
Inclusion and non-inclusion criteria were verified at a pre-selection visit with a sleep-neurophysiologist
clinician. Subjects had a medical check-up to confirm their good physical condition, the absence of sleep,
alertness, neurological, cardiovascular, respiratory, hepatic, renal, or metabolic disorders, a poor hygiene or
abnormal usual sleep patterns, such as night workers or shift-workers, and substance abusers (caffeine,
drug, or alcohol). They had no treatment at the time of their inclusion and during the previous two months.
Subjects for whom taking one of the drugs studied presented a health risk, were also excluded:
hypersensitivity to one of the drugs studied, current or past dependence on alcohol, opiates,
benzodiazepines or any illicit drug. Subjects smoking more than 5 cigarettes per day, drinking more than 28
units of alcohol a week or consuming more than 150 mg of caffeine per day were also excluded. All
participants had normal or corrected to normal vision (visual acuity greater than or equal to 7/10). They had
120
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
driven regularly for at least 30 years.
The participants signed a consent form and an informed commitment form. The experimental protocol was
approved by a French local ethics committee (CCPPRB – Consultative Committee for the Protection of
People in Biomedical Research – of the Basse-Normandie region).
Study design
There was four experimental sessions conducted according to a double-blind, balanced, cross-over design.
For each subject, a washout period of two weeks was respected between each session. Before each
session, subjects took two pills administered in identical capsules (see table 1).
at home, 11 pm
at lab, 8 am
Placebo
placebo
Zolpidem (10mg)
placebo
Placebo
Codeine/paracetamol (20mg/400mg)
Zolpidem (10mg)
Codeine/paracetamol (20mg/400mg)
Table 1. Place and time of the different treatments. Each line of the table corresponds to one session.
Urine was collected at 7 am, when subjects awoke, to verify the absence of BZD, ethanol, cannabis,
cocaïne, amphetamines. Experimental session started at 9 am.
Driving tests
The driving experiment was carried out on the FAROS fixed-base driving simulator equipped with an
ARCHISIM object database. This simulator comprised a steering cab made up of a quarter of a vehicle (Fig.
1). The images, generated at a frequency of approximately 30 Hz, were projected by a video projector onto a
screen (H: 60°; V: 49°) located 1.90 m from the driver’
s eyes. The acquisition frequency for the different
signals (position, speed, acceleration, etc.) was 30 Hz.
There were three driving tests:
1) 60 min along a motorway in monotonous conditions (no traffic, repetitive landscape). Subjects were
instructed to drive as straight as possible within the right (slower) traffic lane while maintaining a constant
speed of 110 km/h.
2) 15 min along a simulated urban route into which prototypical scenarios of accident were introduced. Each
participant drove the route twice. Six scenario of accident were introduced in a counterbalanced order during
the two trips on the circuit. Subjects had to drive at a speed of approximately 50 km/h, and to follow the road
signs indicating the town centre.
"Pedestrian crossing”scenario (pedestrian). A pedestrian, initially hidden from the driver’
s vision by a bus,
starts to cross when the driver arrives. The pedestrian appears in the driver’
s field of vision 2.4 seconds
before the driver reaches his level, which corresponds to a time-to-obstacle of 2.4 s. This configuration only
remains effective if the driver does not perform any manoeuvre to avoid the pedestrian (braking or avoidance
by swerving).
"Sudden stop of the vehicle ahead at a traffic light" scenario (traffic light). The driver is driving in a row of
121
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
traffic on a straight, two-lane carriageway with two-way traffic. Vehicles ahead block his view of an upcoming
“T”intersection and the traffic light at the intersection. The light turns red and the vehicles ahead brake over
a distance of approximately 25 m to stop at the light.
"Vehicle overtaking and merging back into the lane" scenario (overtaking). The driver is driving in the righthand lane of a straight, major 3-lane urban road with two lanes going in his direction. A vehicle overtakes the
driver at a speed 10 km/h faster than him. When it is positioned 20 m ahead of the driver, it merges back into
the right-hand lane ahead of him and brakes to a speed of 30 km/h.
"Vehicle pulling out from a parking space" scenario (parking). A vehicle parked on the right-hand side of the
carriageway pulls out of its parking space when the driver is at a distance of 20 m.
"Left-hand turn by the vehicle ahead" scenario (left hand turn). The driver is driving in a straight carriageway
with 2 lanes separated by axial markings. Ahead of him (quite far), another vehicle is preparing to turn to the
left. The speed of the vehicle driving ahead of the driver at first depends on the subject’
s, so that the
distance between the two vehicles is 25 m. Suddenly, this coupling is broken and the obstacle vehicle slows
down to a speed of 10 km/h after 1.9 s.
“Opposite vehicle crossing” scenario (opposite vehicle). The subject is driving in a straight line on a
carriageway with two lanes separated by central marking and carrying two-way traffic. Another vehicle at
some distance in front of the driver gets ready to turn left into a petrol station. This vehicle starts to cross the
opposite lane when the subject’s vehicle is at a distance of 25 m from the potential point of impact. It
executes its manoeuvre at low speed, 8 km/h.
Note that to avoid any learning effect during the experimental session, participants were largely trained to the
urban circuit and to the different scenarios.
3) 15 min of following a car which can accelerate and deccelerate (+/- 10 or 20 km/h) and can adopt three
speeds, 70, 80 or 90 km/h during 20, 30 or 40 s. Subjects’task was to maintain a safe distance between this
car and their own vehicle.
Subjective tests
After each driving session subjects were asked to rate the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS) and their selfreported quality of driving performance. Visual Analog Scale (VAS) was filled in four times.
Statistics
Dependent variables varied in function of the test (see below). Analysis of data was performed with ANOVA
to evaluate effects of treatment. For each significant effect of the treatment, a Dunnett’s post hoc test, a
Tuckey’
s post hoc test or a Wilcoxon test was applied. P-values < .05 were considered statistically
significant. For monotonous situation, we analysed the standard deviation of the lateral position (SDLP, in
meters), standard deviation of speed (SDS, in km/h), mean lateral position (mLP), mean speed (mS) and
number of road exit. For urban situation with accident scenarios, number of collision, response time and
mean speed at the start point of the scenarios were analysed. For car following situation, SDLP and
response time to the changing speed of the followed vehicle were analysed.
Results
Monotonous driving test
SDLP significantly varied as a function of treatment [F(16, 3)=3.28, p<.05]. It was higher - with zolpidem at
night and placebo at morning (zolp/pla, m = 0.496 m) and - with placebo at night and codeine-paracetamol at
122
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
morning (pla/cod-para, m = 0.503 m) than with placebo at night and at morning (pla/pla, m = 0.451 m)
(respectively: p<.05 and p<.05). SDLP did not vary significantly after the intake of the two active molecules
relatively to placebo treatment (p=.13, m = 0.486 m) (Figure 1).
*
0.7
*
Amplitude
des(cm)
SDLP (m)
SDLP
0.6
0.5
0.4
0.3
0.2
Pla/Pla
Zolpi/Pla
Pla/cod-para
Zolpi/cod-para
Type
de traitements
Treatment
condition administrés
* : p<.05
Figure 1: Monotonous test. Mean Standard Deviation of Lateral Position (SDLP) in each treatment condition.
The number of “road exits” was also significantly affected by the treatment (F (3, 16)=8.66, p<.05). This
number was significantly higher after each treatment than after placebo (respectively Pla/Pla versus
Zolpi/Pla T=5,5, Z=2,63, p=.0085 ; Pla/Pla versus Pla/cod-para T=5, Z=2,67, p=.008 ; Pla/Pla versus
Zolpi/cod-para T=10,5, Z=2, p=.045) (Figure 2).
*
*
*
60
Number of roadexit
50
40
30
20
10
0
Pla/Pla
Zolpi/Pla
Pla/cod-para
Zolpi/cod-para
traitment condition
Treatment condition
* : p<.05
123
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Figure 2: Monotonous test. Mean number of road exit in each treatment condition.
Mean speed and standard deviation of speed did not significantly vary between treatment (respectively F(3,
113
10
112
8
111
6
Amplitude SDLP
110
4
109
2
108
Standard Deviation of Speed (SDSP) (km/h)
Average speed (km/h)
42)=.83, p=.48; F(3, 39)=2.09, p=.11) (Figure 3).
0
Pla/Pla
Zolpi/Pla
Pla/cod-para
Zolpi/cod-para
Traitement
condition
Treatment condition
Average speed
SDSP
Figure 3: Monotonous test. Mean average speed and Mean SDSP as a function of treatment.
Urban driving test
A total of 13 crashes were noted across all scenarios and the combination of placebo and
codéine/paracétamol produced the higher number of collisions (Table 1). These collisions are largely due to
seven females, only three males collided.
Number
collisions
of
Pla/Pla
Zolp/Pla
Pla/cod-para
2
2
6
Zolp/codpara
3
Total
13
Table 1: Urban test. Number of collisions as a function of treatment.
ANOVA ([G2]*treatment) (G2 corresponded to the sex of the participant) did not show any effect of treatment
on response time (RT) (F(3,42)=.73, p=.54) but males always react more rapidly than females (F(1,14)=6.02,
p=.028) (Figure 4).
124
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
2
1.8
Response time (s)
1.6
1.4
1.2
female
1
male
0.8
0.6
0.4
0.2
0
pla/pla
zolp /p la
p la/cod-para
zolp /cod-para
Treatment
Figure 4. Urban test. Mean response time as a function of treatment and sex.
ANOVA [G2]*scenario*treatment showed a scenario effect (F (5,70)=30.63, p<.001) and an interaction
treatment and sex (F (3,42)=4.67, p<.05) on mean speed at the moment origin of the scenarios. The
overtaking scenario produced the highest speed, lowest speed were produce by parking and traffic light
scenario (p<.01). Mean speed adopted by males was significantly higher with zolp/cod-para treatment than
with pla/pla treatment, conversely mean speed adopted by females was lower with zolp/cod-para treatment
than with pla/pla treatment (Figure 5).
60
Mean speed (km/h)
50
40
male
30
female
20
10
0
pla/pla
zolp/pla
pla/cod-para
zolp/cod-para
Treatment
Figure 5. Urban test. Mean response time as a function of treatment and sex.
Car following test
Response time to the variation of speed of the followed car did not show any effect of sex or treatment
(respectively ANOVA ([G2]*treatment; F(3,42)=0.54, p<.66 et F(1,14)=.69, p<.42) (Figure 6).
125
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
7
Response time (s)
6
5
4
female
3
male
2
1
0
pla/pla
pla/zolp
pla/cod-para
zolp/cod-para
Treatment
Figure 6. Car following test. Mean response time as a function of sex and treatment.
SDLP only tended to vary as a function of treatment as showed by ANOVA ([G2]*treatment; F(3,42)=2.39,
p<.08). A contrast analysis showed that Zolp/cod-para involved higher SDLP than pla/pla (F(1,14)=4.77,
p<.046, figure 7).
60
SDLP (cm)
50
40
female
30
male
20
10
0
pla/pla
pla/zolp
pla/cod-para
zolp/cod-para
Treatment
Figure 7. Car following test. Mean SDLP as a function of sex and treatment.
KSS subjective test
Results showed that drug/pharmacological condition (Zolpi/Pla, Pla/cod-para, Zolpi/cod-para) did not
differ from the Pla/Pla condition (F(16,3)=0,094, p=.99; figure 8).
126
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
KSS evaluation
8
6
4
2
Pla/Pla
Zolpi/Pla
Pla/cod-para
Zolpi/cod-para
Treatment
Figure 8. Average ratings on the KSS after one hour of monotonous driving.
Visual Analog Scale (VAS)
Average ratings of drug/pharmacological condition did not significantly differ from the Pla/Pla condition
(F(3,45)=.53, p=.66) but differed as a function of the hour (F(3,45)=4.74, p<.005; figure 9). Tuckey’
s post hoc
test showed higher score value after driving test than before driving test (VAS 1 vs VAS 2; p=.01) and higher
value before driving test than three hours after driving test (VAS 1 vs. VAS 4; p=.001). There was no
interaction between treatment and time on VAS (F(9,135)=.35, p=.95).
**
6
**
Drivingsimulator task
5
VASScore
4
3
2
1
0
VAS 1 - 9h
VAS 2 - 11h
VAS 3 - 12h
VAS 4 - 13h
Repeating VAS - time
** : p<.005
Figure 9. Average value on the VAS scores as a function of the time.
Self-reported quality of driving performance.
Results did not show any significant effect of the treatment on self-reported quality scale performed one
hour after monotonous driving test (F(16,3)=. 093, p=.99; Figure 10).
127
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
2
0
0= "I drove normally"
Scores on self-reported quality of driving performance
4
-2
-4
Pla/Pla
Zolpi/Pla
Pla/cod-para
Zolpi/cod-para
Treatment
Figure 10. Average scores of self-reported quality of driving performance as a function of treatment.
Discussion
The main objective of the present study was to evaluate, in healthy elderly subjects (55 to 65 years old),
residual effects on driving performance of an hypnotic zolpidem (10mg), acute effect of an analgesic
codeine/paracetamol (20mg/400mg), and effects of an association of these two treatments to assess a
potential increased effect of this combination.
Three driving tests were used: monotonous, urban and car following. In monotonous test, results showed
that SDLP, a measure known to be sensible to the residual and acute effects of licit and illicit drugs,
increased with a nighttime intake of zolpidem or a morning time intake of codeine/paracetamol but did not
significantly vary with an intake of these two drugs. Conversely the number or road exits increased whatever
the treatment and in car following test SDLP only increased after the intake of the two active molecules.
In light of the results obtained with monotonous driving, zolpidem alone does thus not appear to be
completely devoid of residual effects as it is usually described with highway driving, but studies were done
with healthy young subjects or on middle-aged insomniac patients. Our previous studies, conducted with
older middle-age drivers under zolpidem, already showed driving performance impairment (SDLPs
significantly increased) in highway situations (Bocca et al., 2011). These results are also in line with some
recent epidemiological data suggesting that zolpidem could not be devoid of residual effects (Gustaven et al.
2008).
Speed and variation of speed, measured during one hour of monotonous driving, were not affected by
treatment. Response times, measured in urban and in car following tests, was not affected by treatment but
we noted that men reacted more quickly than women when confronted to sudden events in urban test.
Crashes were sparse during urban tests, and statistical analysis was not possible due to the size of the
sample. Note also that participants had a good knowledge of the different scenarios, acquired during the
training runs, which can have activated a process of anticipation of their appearance and reduced the
potential number of collisions. In another hand, in urban test, a nighttime intake of zolpidem with a morning
intake of codeine-paracetamol produced differential effects on mean speed adopted by men and women:
men increased their speed, conversely women diminished their speed.
Finally, treatment seemed to be devoid of significant effect on scores of subjective tests. Only visual analog
128
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
scores varied as a function of time.
While these results cannot be straightforwardly interpreted they show a residual effect of zolpidem and also
a combined effect of hypnotic and antalgic on driving performance. These effects would be more effective in
monotonous and car following test than in urban test.
Acknowledgements
This work was conducted as part of the Driving under the influence of drugs, alcohol, and medicines
(DRUID) research consortium funded by European Union grant TREN-05- FP6TRS07.61320-518404DRUID. This report reflects only the authors’view. The EuropeanCommunity is not liable for any use of the
information contained herein.
None of the authors have any financial or other relationships that might lead to a conflict of interest.
References
Amato JN, Marie S, Berthelon C, Lelong-Boulouard V, Denise P, Bocca ML (submitted) Effects of Analgesics
doses: Up to 3 doses of the association of codein and paracetamol did not impair driving performance in
healthy young volunteers.
Barbone F, Mac Mahon A, Davey P, Morris A, Reid I, MacDevitt D, MacDonald T (1998) Association of roadtraffic accidents with benzodiazepine use. Lancet 352 (9137): 1331-1336
Bachs LC, Engeland A, Morland JG, Skurtveit S (2009) The risk of motor vehicle accidents involving drivers
with prescriptions for codeine or tramadol. Clin Pharmacol Ther 85: 596-9
Bocca ML, Marie S, Lelong-Boulouard V, Bertran F, Couque C, Desfemmes T, Berthelon C, Amato JN,
Moessinger M, Paillet-Loilier M, Coquerel A, Denise P (2011) Zolpidem and zopiclone impair similarly
monotonous driving performance after a single nighttime intake in aged subjects. Psychopharmacology 214:
699–706
Dubois S, Bedard M, Weaver B (2010) The association between opioid analgesics and unsafe driving
actions preceding fatal crashes. Accid Anal Prev 42: 30-7
Engeland A, Skurtveit S, Morland J (2007) Risk of road traffic accidents associated with the prescription of
drugs: a registry-based cohort study. Ann Epidemiol 17: 597-602
Fishbain DA, Cutler RB, Rosomoff HL, Rosomoff RS (2002) Can patients taking opioids drive safely? A
structured evidence-based review. J Pain Palliat Care Pharmacother 16: 9-28
Gjerde H, Normann PT, Christophersen AS, Samuelsen SO, Mørland J (in press) Alcohol, psychoactive
drugs and fatal road traffic accidents in Norway: A case–control study. Accident Analysis & Prevention, In
Press, Available online 21 January 2011
Glass J, Lanctot KL, Herrmann N, Sproule BA, Busto UE. (2005) Sedative hypnotics in older people with
insomnia: meta-analysis of risks and benefits. Bmj. 19;331(7526):1169.
Gustavsen I, Bramness JG, Skurtveit S, Engeland A, Neutel I, Mørland J. (2008) Road traffic accident risk
related to prescriptions of the hypnotics zopiclone, zolpidem, flunitrazepam and nitrazepam. Sleep Med
9(8):818-22
Kelly E, Darke S, Ross J (2004) A review of drug use and driving: epidemiology, impairment, risk factors and
risk perceptions. Drug Alcohol Rev 23: 319-44
McGwin G, Sims RV, Pulley L, Roseman JM (2000) Relations among chronic medical conditions,
129
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
medication, and automobile crashes in the elderly: a population-based case-control study. American Journal
of Epidemiology 152: 424-431
Maycock G, (2000) Forecasting older-driver accidents and casualties. Road Safety Research Report N°23,
DLTR
Movig KLL, Mathijssen MPM, Nagel PHA, van Egmondd T, de Gier JJ, Leufkens HGM, Egberts ACG (2004)
Psychoactive substance use and the risk of motor vehicle accidents. Accident Analysis and Prevention 36:
631–636
Neutel I (1998) Benzodiazepine-related traffic accidents in young and elderly drivers. Human
Psychopharmacology 13 (suppl. 2) : S115-S123
Legrain S (2005) Prescription médicamenteuse du sujet âgé. EMC-Médecine 2 : 127–136
Meskali M, Berthelon C, Marie S, Denise P, Bocca ML (2009) Residual effects of hypnotics drugs in aging
drivers submitted to simulated scenarios of accidents: an exploratory study. Psychopharmacology 207(3):
461-467
Orriols L, Delorme B, Gadegbeku B, Tricotel A, Contrand B, Laumon B, Salmi LR, Lagarde E; CESIR
research group (2010) Prescription medicines and the risk of road traffic crashes: a French registry-based
study. PLoS Med. November 7(11):e1000366.
Vermeeren A (2004) Residual Effects of Hypnotics: Epidemiology and Clinical Implications. CNS Drugs
18(5): 297-328.
Walsh JM, Gier JJ de, Christopherson AS, Verstraete A G (2004) 'Drugs and Driving', Traffic Injury
Prevention 5(3): 241 – 253
130
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Chapter 6 : Acute effects of 3 doses of analgesics
on simulated driving performance in healthy
volunteers
ML Bocca (UNICAEN), P Denise (UNICAEN), C Berthelon (INRETS)
131
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Abstract
The objective of this study was to evaluate the dose-effect relationship of the usual therapeutic doses of
codeine/paracetamol (20/400, 40/800 and 60/1200 mg) on driving ability, psychomotor performance,
subjective alertness, in link with blood concentrations in 16 healthy young volunteers.
Driving performance, psychomotor vigilance tests and scales reflecting alertness were evaluated during the
morning after drug intake in a double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled study. Two blood samples were
collected, 1 hour and 4 hours after drug intake.
None of the classical parameters used to quantify the effects of drugs on driving and on psychomotor
performance were affected by any of the three codeine/paracetamol doses. However, significant correlations
were observed between the driving parameters and both morphine and codeine blood concentrations.
The relationships between blood concentration and behavioral measures indicated that the inter-subject
variability in drug effects, possibly linked to genetic factors, is hidden when analysis by treatment is used.
132
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Introduction
Among factors suspected to increase the risk of road accidents, drug consumption is regularly implicated
(Bachs et al. 2009; Chesher 1985; Engeland et al. 2007; Kelly et al. 2004; Leveille et al. 1994). Narcotic
analgesics are among the most prescribed drugs and they appear to be involved in between 3 % of the
accidents in Europe (De Gier 1995; Gaulier et al. 2003) and 10 % in the USA (Dubois et al. 2010). Several
epidemiological studies have found low relative risk for opioid users (Fishbain et al. 2002; Kelly et al. 2004),
but some recent studies have shown that drivers using natural opium alkaloids have an increased risk of
being involved in an accident (Bachs et al. 2009; Dubois et al. 2010; Engeland et al. 2007; Kelly et al. 2004).
Among analgesics, codeine is a light opioid of level II which is often associated with paracetamol, an
analgesic of level I. These two drugs have synergetic activity in reducing moderate to strong pain (Quiding et
al. 1982). In some studies, codeine is associated with an elevated risk of accidents (Bachs et al. 2009;
Engeland et al. 2007). As codeine reduces vigilance, these accidents could be linked to the sedative effects
of codeine (Leveille et al. 1994). Indeed, reduced vigilance and falling asleep while driving are the most
common and severe causes of accidents (Schmidt et al. 2009).
Pharmacological properties of opioids, particularly codeine, are well-known (Caraco et al. 1999; Kim et al.
2002; Lotsch et al. 2006; Somogyi et al. 2007). Codeine interacts with mu, delta and kappa opioid receptors
in the CNS, which play an important role in the control of pain, particularly in the spinothalamocortical
pathways (Rainville 2002). The effects of codeine on the CNS depend on the dose (Quiding et al. 1982), the
route of administration and previous exposure (Huestis and Smith 2009). Codeine, like morphine but to a
lesser degree, also has a depressive effect on the respiratory center and alertness.
Experimental studies are complementary to the epidemiological studies to evaluate the risks of accidents
following drug intake. With performance tests, several studies have been performed to evaluate the effects of
codeine on driving-related skills and have shown that some impairments occurred with high doses (60-100
mg) (for review see (Zacny 1995)). Only one study has evaluated the effects of a single dose of 50 mg of
codeine on driving performance with a simulator in young healthy subjects (Linnoila and Hakkinen 1974).
The authors showed that 50 mg of codeine increased the risk of collisions in two driving tests, i.e. in
emergency situations and in a monotonous driving test. The 50 mg dose, used in this study, is a current
dose used in patients for reducing pain. However, the common therapeutic dose in one intake can vary
between 20 and 60 mg, which, in France, represents 1 to 3 tablets of codeine/paracetamol. The primary
objective of this study was therefore to evaluate the effects of the most common 3 therapeutic doses of
codeine/paracetamol on driving performance in healthy young subjects to assess a potential dose-effect
relationship of this drug. As driving a motor vehicle requires the possession of sufficient cognitive, visual and
motor skills and involves managing attention to perform various driving- and non-driving-related tasks
(Michon 1985; Salvucci et al. 2007), the secondary objective was to assess the effects of the 3 doses on
cognitive and psychomotor functioning related to driving and on subjective feelings. For this purpose we
used the Psychomotor Vigilance Task (PVT) (Dinges and Powell 1985), based on a simple visual reaction
time, which is a test of vigilance most often used in sleep deprivation studies. Assessment of subjective
feelings in change of mood and driving performance estimation were evaluated with several Visual Analog
Scales (VAS) (Akerstedt and Gillberg 1990; Bond and Lader 1974; Brookhuis et al. 1985). Blood
concentration of codeine and paracetamol was also assessed 1 hour and 4 hours after taking an oral dose.
133
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Method
Subjects
The study was carried out on 16 healthy volunteers (8 men and 8 women). Their average age was 22.37
years + 2.7, weight was 64.15 kg + 8.53, and height was 171.80 cm + 5.27. All participants were graduates
from at least the first cycle of University. Exclusion criteria were sleep, alertness, neurological,
cardiovascular, respiratory, hepatic, renal, or metabolic disorders, chronic or transitory use of oral medication
except contraceptives during the 2 years prior to the experiment, cigarette consumption > 5 cigarettes per
day, and alcohol consumption > 28 units per week.
The study was approved by the Caen Northwest III ethics committee and French Health Products
Safety Agency (number 060702), and each subject gave consent in accordance with the requirements of the
committee.
Our 16 subjects completed the Horne and Ostberg morningness-eveningness questionnaire (Horne
and Ostberg 1976). Subjects did not have extreme scores when typed for fluctuations in morning or evening
alertness (10 Intermediate types, 4 Morning types and 2 Evening types).
Study design
Before the study, subjects were trained to drive the simulator during 30 mn.
The study was conducted according to a balanced, double-blind, cross-over design. Each volunteer
participated in four sessions held at intervals of at least 2 weeks, and received the following treatments
during these sessions : 20 mg of codeine and 400 mg of paracetamol, 40 mg of codeine and 800 mg of
paracetamol, or 60 mg of codeine and 1 200 mg of paracetamol, or a placebo. All drugs were administered
in identical gelatin capsules and the subjects received the same number of capsules at each session.
The experiments took place from Tuesday to Friday. Subjects performed their four sessions on the same day
of the week and at the same time of the day in order to avoid any possible interference with changes in
biological rhythms from one day of the week to the next.
The subjects were asked not to drink coffee in the morning of the medication intake and to abstain from
alcohol and sport for 24 h prior to testing. They took their usual breakfast in the morning of the test but
abstained from smoking. There were two subjects per day. The medication was administered to the first
subject at 8:00 AM and to the second at 9:00 AM in the laboratory under the supervision of an experimenter.
The starting time of the experiment was 9:00 AM for the first subject and 10:00 AM for the second (1 hour
after oral administration of the drugs, at the theoretical maximal concentration (Cmax) of codeine). Subjects
completed several analogue subjective scales during the morning, before and after the driving and PVT test.
After the driving test and the PVT, subjects remained in the laboratory for one hour before leaving.
Consumption of benzodiazepines, opiates, cannabis, cocaine, amphetamines was screened at each session
by urine control. No subjects were positive.
Driving and psychomotor tests
The driving task took place with the FAROS driving simulator that reproduces several aspects of a current
134
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
medium-sized car (dashboard, five-speed gearbox, pedals and steering wheel). The simulator produces
engine and tire-squeal sound effects that correlate with the speed of the vehicle but has no system that
enables simulation of the movements of the car. The cabin was associated with an interactive display unit
which reproduced the motorway scenery with computer-generated pictures on a 1.3x1.7 meter screen
located in front of the cabin at 2.3 meters from the subject (Meskali et al. 2009).
The test involved driving for 60 minutes during the day along a motorway in monotonous conditions (no
traffic, occasional long, wide curves, and repetitive landscape). Subjects were instructed to drive on the right
and to respect the speed limit set at 110 km/h during the complete test session. They had to drive as much
as possible in the center of the right lane and at constant speed. The driving task used for this study was a
monotonous motorway, yet it was realistic in order to maintain ecological validity. Driving parameters were
collected with a 30 Hz frequency.
After completion of the driving test, a program calculated the mean speed (km/h) and the standard deviation
of speed (SD speed, km/h), the standard deviation of the lateral position (SDLP, m) and the number of line
crossings (n). The SDLP was described as the most sensitive measure for evaluating drug effects in real
driving and is an index of vehicle control (O'Hanlon et al. 1982). We previously used the FAROS simulator
for evaluating monotonous driving after medication intake and have shown that the simulator was sensitive
enough to reveal drug impairments in monotonous conditions (Bocca et al. online).
Psychomotor vigilance task
This is a simple RT test to assess sustained attention with a cue that occurs at random inter-stimulus
intervals (ISI).The ISI varied randomly from 2 to 10 s. The standard test is 10 minutes in duration. During this
time, subjects were seated comfortably and instructed to respond by pushing a button when a small circular
area on a dark screen appeared. They were told to respond as rapidly as they could whenever they
perceived the circular area. After their response, the reaction time appeared in a bright millisecond counter
inside this circular area and remained on the screen for the duration of 1 second, which served as feedback.
The percentage of lapses (number of responses greater than 500 ms/number of total stimuli), which is a
parameter often used as the primary dependent variable in the test, was analyzed. The mean RT (RT, ms)
and the variability in RT (SD RT, ms) were also analyzed to study the sedative effects of the compounds.
Subjective measures:
This study employed multi- objective and subjective measures to characterize variations of driver fatigue and
sedative effects of analgesics.
After the driving task, all subjects rated their sleepiness on the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS). This
scale is a 9-point verbally-anchored scale with the following steps: (1) very alert, (3) alert, (5) neither alert nor
sleepy, (7) sleepy, and (9) very sleepy, fighting sleep (Akerstedt and Gillberg 1990). The other points
represent intermediate stages between the two neighboring points without definitions.
Each driver’
s feeling parameters were recorded with “subjective driving quality”, using a continuous scale for
self-rating their driving quality (Brookhuis et al. 1985). This scale consisted of a 100 mm vertical visual
analog scale at three levels: (1) “I drove exceptionally poorly”at 0 mm, (2) “I drove normally”at 50 mm and
(3) “I drove exceptionally well”at 100 mm. Responses were given on a visual analog scale of 100 mm with
135
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
an “x”at the appropriate level. If the response was near ±50 mm the subjects estimated that they drove
normally and safety.
The subjects rated their subjective feelings in change of mood with a VAS (Bond and Lader 1974). We used
the 9-visual analog scale items grouped into factor 1, reflecting “alertness”(Bond and Lader 1974) since we
focused on the sedative effects of codeine/paracetamol drug. The nine 100 mm scales were given to each
subject every hour during the experiment: when they arrived at the laboratory (baseline level: 8:00 or 9:00
AM), 1 hour after the medication intake (+1 hour: 9:00 or 10:00 AM), after the driving test (+2 hours: 10:00 or
11:00 AM) and at the end of the morning test (+3 hours: 12:00 or 1:00 PM). The subjects placed a mark on a
horizontal line equivalent to the strength of a particular feeling at that time. The scales were scored by
measuring from the end of the line to the subject’
s mark in centimeters. The mean of the 9 values was
calculated to reflect the “alertness factor”.
Pharmacokinetic assessment
Concentrations of paracetamol, codeine and morphine which is one of the main metabolites of codeine
(O'Neal et al. 1999), were measured in serum. The blood sample collected was 7,5 mL. Morphine was
quantified because it is an active metabolite which is at least in part responsible for the analgesic activity of
codeine. Moreover, this quantification is an indication of the presence of possible poor metabolizers in our
selected subjects (Alvan et al. 1990).
Two blood samples were collected; the first (T1) 1 hour after the oral administration (at the theoretical Cmax)
and the second (T2) at the end of the experiment (i.e. 4 hours after the oral administration).
Codeine and morphine were determined in serum after simple liquid-liquid extraction and were quantified by
high-performance liquid chromatography-electrospray ionization-tandem mass spectrometry (HPLC-ESIMS/MS) using a Shimadzu (Columbia, MD) Prominence HPLC system interfaced on an AB SCIEX 3200
QTRAP (Concorde, Ontario, Canada), with a TurboIonSpray™ source in positive ionization mode (adapted
to (Coles et al. 2007) method). The cut-off was 1
g/L, for both drugs. Paracetamol was quantified by
fluorescence polarization immunoassay (Abbott, France) and the cut-off was 1 mg/L.
Statistics
The statistical analyses were performed with STATISTICA software version 7.1 (StatSoft®). For SDLP,
mean speed, SD speed, subjective scales (KSS, driving quality) a one-way ANOVA for repeated measure
was performed in order to compare the 4 treatments. Post hoc comparisons to placebo were assessed with
Dunnett’
s test to evaluate each treatment effect. The effect of the treatments on the number of line crossings
was tested with a non parametric Friedman ANOVA.
A two-way repeated measures ANOVA with treatment (4 levels) by time (3 times) was used for the
subjective alertness factor (only post ingestion values were included in this analysis). In the case of
significant time effect, a post hoc Fisher’
s LSD was performed. Linear regressions between performance
task parameters and serum levels of codeine, morphine and paracetamol were calculated. P-values < 0.05
were considered statistically significant.
136
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Results
Plasma concentration of drugs after oral administration of codeine and paracetamol (table 1).
There are no significant differences between T1 and T2 for each drug.
Driving tests
Overall treatment effect
The repeated measures ANOVA did not reveal any significant effect of codeine/paracetamol on the SDLP
(F(3, 45)=0.60, p=0.61), on the mean speed (F(3, 45)=0.49, p=0.68) or on the SD speed (F(3,45)=1.72,
p=0.17). The only significant result was found with the mean LP (F(3,45)=3.55, p=0.02), with a significant
difference between the 20 mg dose and placebo (p < 0.006). Non parametric ANOVA performed on the
number of road exits did not reveal any significant effect (F(3,45)=2.77, p=0.42), however, a significant
increase was observed with the 40 mg dose compared to placebo (p < 0.04) if separate Wilcoxon tests were
performed.
137
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Psychomotor Vigilance Test (PVT): (Table 2)
The ANOVA revealed no significant influence of codeine/paracetamol on mean RT
(F(3, 45)=0.88, p=0.45), on the SD RT (F(3, 45)=0.41, p=.74). The percentage of lapses did not differ
statistically between doses of codeine/paracetamol or placebo (F(3,45)=3.48, p=0.32).
Subjective scales (Table 2 and figure 2)
The ANOVA revealed a significant effect of codeine/paracetamol on the KSS score (F(3,45)=10.50, p=0.01).
Dunnett’
s test did not reveal any significant treatment effect for any of the 3 doses of codeine/paracetamol,
Fisher’
s LSD test revealed a significant difference between 20 mg and 40 mg (p=0.01).
No significant effect was found for the self-rated quality of driving performance (F(3, 45)=5.11, p=0.16). It
appears that subjects judged their own driving quality, on average, as positive and nearly normal.
For the alertness factor, the ANOVA did not show any treatment effect (F(3, 45)=1.86, p=0.14), but did show
a time effect (F(2, 30)=11.05, p=0.00025). No treatment*time interaction was found (F(6, 90)=1.08, p=0.37).
The significant time effect was seen in the significant difference in the alertness factor values between the +1
hour (just before the driving test) and both the + 2 hours (just after the driving test) and + 3 hours (at the end
of the experiment) (P < 0.005) (Figure 2).
138
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Correlations between behavioral measures and blood concentrations
Multiple correlations were performed between the different variables and the blood concentrations of
codeine, morphine and paracetamol. As the driving test followed the T1 blood samples, the driving
parameters were correlated with blood sample concentrations collected at T1. As the PVT was performed at
the end of the morning and just before the T2 blood samples, the PVT parameters were correlated with
blood sample concentrations collected at T2.
Interestingly, a trend was found between SDLP changes and codeine T1 (r=0.27; p=0.07) and morphine T1
(r=0.33, p = 0.08) (Figures 3AB). The mean speed changes was also significantly and negatively correlated
with morphine T1 (r=0.51, p<0.005) (Figure 3C).
139
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
140
120
8
A
7
100
[Codeine] T1
[Morphine] T1
6
5
4
B
T1
r = 0.27
p = 0.07
80
60
40
3
20
2
T1
r = 0.33
p = 0.07
1
0
-0.15
-0.10
-0.05
0.00
0.05
0.10
0.15
0.20
0
0.25
-0.3
-0.2
-0.1
0.0
0.1
0.2
0.3
SDLP changes
SDLP changes
8
7
T1
r = 0.51
p = 0.005
[Morphine] T1
6
5
4
C
3
2
1
0
-3
-2
-1
0
1
2
Mean speed changes
Figures 3 A, B, C. Scatter plots showing relationships between (A) morphine concentration at T1
and SDLP changes, (B) codeine concentration at T1 and SDLP changes, (C) morphine concentration at T1
and mean speed changes. SDLP: standard deviation of lateral position.
No significant correlations were found between the PVT parameters and the blood sample concentrations
collected at T2.
Discussion
This study was designed to examine the effects of three growing therapeutic doses of codeine/paracetamol
on the parameters used to quantify simulated driving performance. Our study did not reveal any effects of
codeine/paracetamol, at the 3 doses tested, on the SDLP, the most sensitive parameter used to investigate
drug effects on driving performance (O'Hanlon et al. 1982). Moreover, the other driving parameters, i.e.
speed variability and the number of road exits were also unaffected. The only modification of behavior was
observed on the mean lateral position and with the lowest dose. The lateral position to the left of the road
lane in comparison to placebo could be hypothetized as a sort of safety margin used by the subjects when
140
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
driving. Such behavior has already been observed after hypnotic use in urban driving (Meskali et al. 2009).
Overall results suggested that up to 60 mg codeine and 1 200 mg paracetamol in a single intake,
codeine/paracetamol drug did not impair monotonous driving performance in healthy young subjects.
Among the different parameters studied, only one can be compared with the previous study (Linnoila and
Hakkinen 1974). The authors did not find any effect of the 50 mg of codeine on the mean speed during a 40
min monotonous driving test. This can be compared to our monotonous driving test although they included
accidents during this task contrarly to our test. Our results agreed with this study regarding the monotonous
driving test. However, (Linnoila and Hakkinen 1974) found that the 50 mg dose of codeine increased the risk
of accidents in emergency situations. As our objective was to determine a dose-effect relationship with the
standard and sensitive test used in the field of assessment of drugs on driving performance (Vermeeren
2004), i.e. the monotonous driving test, previously used in our laboratory (Bocca et al. Minor revision; Bocca
et al. 1999), we did not perform such an evaluation of behavior, and it is thus difficult to really compare these
studies. Nevertheless, as we have developed a methodology to evaluate accident scenarios in urban driving
with a simulator, and have shown its usefulness (Meskali et al. 2009), it would be interesting to evaluate the
effects of different doses of codeine on such situations in a further study. Consequently, we can conclude
that, in our study, the 3 doses of codeine/paracetamol did not impair monotonous driving performance in
healthy young subjects.
The absence of a codeine/paracetamol effect on driving performance is supported by the similar absence of
impairment of performance in the PVT and on the subjective measure to characterize variations in driver
fatigue and sedative effect of analgesics. All of these results indicated, despite the high sensitivity of the PVT
test in various conditions (sleep loss, age, caffeine, etc) (Dinges and Powell 1985; Lim and Dinges 2008),
that codeine had no effect on arousal and attentional state at the doses used, which could explain the
absence of driving performance impairment in the monotonous driving test. Moreover, the absence of effects
on the subjective feelings of alertness, KSS and driving assessment, revealed that the subjects felt well, and
judged their own driving quality as nearly normal. Only a significant difference between the two doses of
codeine 40 and 60 mg was observed with the KSS, which is difficult to interpret. All of these results are in
agreement with previous findings (Walker and Zacny 1998). Indeed, this study evaluated the effects of 60
and 120 mg of codeine and did not find any effect of codeine on VAS, reflecting subjective feelings and
various psychomotor performance tests. Our results suggest that codeine at therapeutic doses, i.e. up to 60
mg in a single intake in healthy young subjects, did not impair both driving performance and vigilance.
The serum concentrations collected at the theoretical Cmax and at the end of the morning sessions served
to evaluate the elimination course of codeine/paracetamol and codeine metabolites, i.e. morphine, and
helped to interpret our behavioral results. Firstly, as morphine was found in all samples, the absence of
impairment with codeine doses could not be linked to the fact that our subjects might be poor metabolizers.
Secondly, as codeine concentrations were found to be higher at the end of the experiment (4 hours after the
oral administration) than 1 hour after oral administration (theoretical Cmax) although no significant difference
between T1 and T2 concentrations was found, this suggests that the effect of codeine may have not been
maximal during the driving test performed during the 1 hour just after the first sample. This delay could be
due to the fact that codeine/paracetamol was ingested in capsules for blind ingestion, which could delay the
maximal concentration observed in the classical galenic form, i.e. pill. This hypothesis is also supported by
the time effect found in the alertness factor, with alertness decreasing across the morning. Nevertheless, as
141
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
subjects performed two tests consecutively during the morning session (driving and PVT), we cannot
exclude the possibility that the decreased alertness could also be due to an increase in fatigue. Thirdly,
despite non significant effects of the 3 doses of codeine/paracetamol tested in this study, some significant
correlations were found between behavioral measures and blood concentrations of codeine or morphine.
Such relationships have been described with alcohol, with increasing impairment of driving performance with
increasing dose (Louwerens et al. 1987; Verster et al. 2009). It should be noted that positive trend were
found between SDLP changes and both morphine and codeine concentrations. This suggests that higher
concentrations of morphine and codeine corresponded to the subjects driving outside the road. Moreover,
these relationships indicate that the more the subjects drove outside the road the more they reduced their
speed. This could be a safety mechanism developed by the subjects.
Our results also indicate that evaluating the effects of treatments on performance did not reflect the
individual
variations
in
drug
pharmacokinetics.
Indeed,
on
average,
the
different
doses
of
codeine/paracetamol did not impair the driving performance even though subjects with higher concentrations
of morphine or codeine in the blood had slightly more impaired driving performance. This result is interesting
since codeine is metabolized into morphine via the CYP2D6, which is subject to genetic polymorphism
(Sindrup and Brosen 1995). We can hypothetize that, for a small fraction of the population who are fast
metabolizers, the proportion of morphine in the blood will be greater and thus should increase the risk of
accidents. It would be interesting to evaluate if the genetic differences in codeine metabolism could
determine the extent of the impairment, by comparing the genotype and the phenotype (ratio
morphine/codeine) of the volunteers. However, due to a small number of subjects (n=16), this hypothesis
cannot be verified in the present study. Interestingly, we found correlations with morphine at T1 although the
morphine concentration should not be maximal at this time, probably due to the fact that morphine is a
metabolite of codeine. The correlations revealed that although the level of morphine is low, in agreement
with (Shah and Mason 1990) results, a link between morphine level and performance can be observed.
Stronger relationships need to be hypothesized between driving parameters and morphine if blood samples
are collected after T1. Interestingly, we also performed correlations for each dose and found that only with
the 60 mg dose positive and significant correlations were found between SDLP difference and both morphine
and codeine (p<0.05, for both). These results suggest that with higher doses (greater than 60 mg) behavioral
impairments are likely to be observed. Finally, these correlations suggest that both morphine and codeine
seem to have a concentration-dependent effect that may lead to impairment of driving performance. These
results may be compared to those of Bachr et al (Bachs et al. 2003), who suggested that codeine effects
may be independent from those of morphine but their cut-off value of morphine concentration was higher
than ours (15 ng/mL in the study of Bachs et al. (2009) and 1 ng/mL in the present study). Our results
indicate that morphine may lead to impairments even at low levels.
Contrary to the driving parameters, no correlations were found between the 3 PVT parameters (RT, SDRT
and % of lapses) and codeine/morphine/paracetamol T2 concentrations. This absence of relationships is due
to the fact that PVT was performed at the end of the morning, 2.5 hours after capsule intake. Consequently,
the concentrations in serum collected at T2 could not reflect the drug effects measured 1.5 hours before the
samples were collected.
Another study was performed to assess the effects of another opioid analgesic using a methodology similar
to that used in our study (Verster et al. 2006). The authors revealed that oxycodone/paracetamol did not
142
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
impair monotonous driving performance, the psychomotor task or the mood. Although oxycodone is a
different compound than that tested in our study, it is similar to codeine in that it reduces pain by acting
centrally, i.e. on the central nervous system. Our results, in addition to a previous study (Verster et al. 2006),
could provide information for general practioners, pharmacists and patients. This information suggests that,
to date, no noticeable driving impairment has been demonstrated after the use of single, usual doses of
these opioid analgesics. Nevertheless, both studies were performed on healthy young subjects. Despite the
fact that the choice of assessing a dose-effect relationship in young subjects was justified by the fact that this
is the classic methodology to evaluate drug effects and that 26 percent opioid consumers are under 25
(Cadet-Taïrou et al. 2008), it would be useful to assess the effects of codeine in aged subjects before
generalizing these results. Indeed, it has been shown that with age the amount of mu receptors decreases
and the affinity increases, leading to an increased sensitivity to opioids (Wilder-Smith 2005).
In conclusion, our study, which was performed to establish a dose-effect relationship with the usual
therapeutic doses of codeine/paracetamol in a single intake in healthy young subjects, did not reveal any
impairment of driving and vigilance. From a road safety point of view, it must be noted that a concordance
between feelings of driving quality and objective measures of performance was found. Additional research is
needed to assess the effects on other critical driving situations such as urban accident scenarios, on aged
drivers, and at higher doses since linear relationships have been observed between behavioral measures
and blood concentrations of codeine, morphine and paracetamol.
Acknowledgements
This work was conducted as part of the Driving under the influence of drugs, alcohol, and medicines
(DRUID) research consortium funded by European Union grant TREN-05- FP6TR-S07.61320-518404DRUID. This report reflects only the authors’view. The European Community is not liable for any use of the
information contained herein.
None of the authors have any financial or other relationships that might lead to a conflict of
interest.
References
Akerstedt T, Gillberg M (1990) Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. Int J Neurosci 52:
29-37
Alvan G, Bechtel P, Iselius L, Gundert-Remy U (1990) Hydroxylation polymorphisms of debrisoquine and
mephenytoin in European populations. Eur J Clin Pharmacol 39: 533-7
Bachs L, Skurtveit S, Morland J (2003) Codeine and clinical impairment in samples in which morphine is not
detected. Eur J Clin Pharmacol 58: 785-9
Bachs LC, Engeland A, Morland JG, Skurtveit S (2009) The risk of motor vehicle accidents involving drivers
with prescriptions for codeine or tramadol. Clin Pharmacol Ther 85: 596-9
Bocca M, Marie S, Lelong-Boulouard V, Bertran F, Couque C, Desfemmes T, Berthelon C, Amato J,
Moessinger M, Paillet-Loilier M, Coquerel A, Denise P (Minor revision) Zolpidem and zopiclone impair
143
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
similarly
monotonous
driving
performance
after
a
signle
nighttime
intake
in
aged
subjects.
Psychopharmacology
Bocca ML, Le Doze F, Etard O, Pottier M, L'Hoste J, Denise P (1999) Residual effect of zolpidem 10 mg and
zopiclone 7.5 mg versus flunitrazepam 1 mg and placebo on driving performance and ocular saccades.
Psychopharmacology 143: 373-9
Bond A, Lader M (1974) the use of analogue scales in rating subjective feelings. Br J Med Psychol 47: 211218
Brookhuis KA, De Vries G, Van Wijngaarden PP, Veenstra G, Hommes M, Louwerens JW, O'Hanlon JF
(1985) The effects of increasing doses of Meptazinol (100, 200, 400 mg) and Glafenine (200mg) on driving
performance. . Verkeerskundig Studiecentrum: Traffic Research Center, Haren, pp 40
Cadet-Taïrou A, Gandilhon M, Toufik A, Evrard I (2008) Phénomènes émergents liés au drogues en
2006/2007. Huitième rapport national du dispositif TREND. In: OFDT (ed). Observatoire Français des
drogues et des toxicomanies, Saint Denis, pp 189
Caraco Y, Sheller J, Wood AJ (1999) Impact of ethnic origin and quinidine coadministration on codeine's
disposition and pharmacodynamic effects. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 290: 413-22
Chesher GB (1985) The influence of analgesic drugs in road crashes. Accid Anal Prev 17: 303-9
Coles R, Kushnir MM, Nelson GJ, McMillin GA, Urry FM (2007) Simultaneous determination of codeine,
morphine, hydrocodone, hydromorphone, oxycodone, and 6-acetylmorphine in urine, serum, plasma, whole
blood, and meconium by LC-MS-MS. J Anal Toxicol 31: 1-14
De Gier JJ (1995) Drugs other than alcohol and driving in the European Union. In: Communities DGfTotCotE
(ed). The institute for human psychopharmacology, The Netherlands
Dinges DF, Powell JW (1985) Microcomputer analyses of performance on a portable, simple visual RT task
during sustained operations. BRM 17: 652-655
Dubois S, Bedard M, Weaver B (2010) The association between opioid analgesics and unsafe driving
actions preceding fatal crashes. Accid Anal Prev 42: 30-7
Engeland A, Skurtveit S, Morland J (2007) Risk of road traffic accidents associated with the prescription of
drugs: a registry-based cohort study. Ann Epidemiol 17: 597-602
Fishbain DA, Cutler RB, Rosomoff HL, Rosomoff RS (2002) Can patients taking opioids drive safely? A
structured evidence-based review. J Pain Palliat Care Pharmacother 16: 9-28
Gaulier JM, Marquet P, Lachatre G (2003) Traitements de substitution et conduite automobile. Opiate
addiction treatment and driving. Ann Toxicol Anal 15: 123-127
Horne JA, Ostberg O (1976) A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in
human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol 4: 97-110
Huestis MA, Smith ML (2009) Pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics of drugs abused in driving. In:
Verster JC, Pandi-Perumal SR, Ramaekers JG, De Gier JJ (eds) Drugs, Driving and Traffic Safety.
Birkhäuser Verlag, switzerland, pp 151-185
Kelly E, Darke S, Ross J (2004) A review of drug use and driving: epidemiology, impairment, risk factors and
risk perceptions. Drug Alcohol Rev 23: 319-44
Kim I, Barnes AJ, Oyler JM, Schepers R, Joseph RE, Jr., Cone EJ, Lafko D, Moolchan ET, Huestis MA
(2002) Plasma and oral fluid pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics after oral codeine administration.
Clin Chem 48: 1486-96
144
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Leveille SG, Buchner DM, Koepsell TD, McCloskey LW, Wolf ME, Wagner EH (1994) Psychoactive
medications and injurious motor vehicle collisions involving older drivers. Epidemiology 5: 591-8
Lim J, Dinges DF (2008) Sleep deprivation and vigilant attention. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1129: 305-22
Linnoila M, Hakkinen S (1974) Effects of diazepam and codeine, alone and in combination with alcohol, on
simulated driving. Clin Pharmacol Ther 15: 368-73
Lotsch J, Skarke C, Schmidt H, Rohrbacher M, Hofmann U, Schwab M, Geisslinger G (2006) Evidence for
morphine-independent central nervous opioid effects after administration of codeine: contribution of other
codeine metabolites. Clin Pharmacol Ther 79: 35-48
Louwerens JW, Gloerich ABM, De Vries G, Brookhuis KA, O'Hanlon JF (1987) The relationship between
drivers' blood alcohol concentration (BAC) and actual driving performance during high speed travel. In:
Noordzij PC, Roszbach, R. (Eds.) (ed) Alcohol, Drugs and Traffic Safety-T86. Excerpta Medica, Amsterdam,
pp 183-186
Meskali M, Berthelon C, Marie S, Denise P, Bocca ML (2009) Residual effects of hypnotic drugs in aging
drivers submitted to simulated accident scenarios: an exploratory study. Psychopharmacology 207: 461-467
Michon JA (1985) A critical view of driver behavior models: What do we know, what should we do? In: Evans
L, Schwing RC (eds) Human behavior and traffic safety. Plenum Press, New-York, pp 485-520
O'Hanlon JF, Haak TW, Blaauw GJ, Riemersma JB (1982) Diazepam impairs lateral position control in
highway driving. Science 217: 79-81
O'Neal CL, Crouch DJ, Rollins DE, Fatah A, Cheever ML (1999) Correlation of saliva codeine concentrations
with plasma concentrations after oral codeine administration. J Anal Toxicol 23: 452-9
Quiding H, Persson G, Ahlstrom U, Bangens S, Hellem S, Johansson G, Jonsson E, Nordh PG (1982)
Paracetamol plus supplementary doses of codeine. An analgesic study of repeated doses. Eur J Clin
Pharmacol 23: 315-9
Rainville P (2002) Brain mechanisms of pain affect and pain modulation. Curr Opin Neurobiol 12: 195-204
Salvucci DD, Boer ER, Liu A (2007) Toward an Integrated Model of Driver Behavior in Cognitive
Architecture. T R R: 9-16
Schmidt EA, Schrauf M, Simon M, Fritzsche M, Buchner A, Kincses WE (2009) Drivers' misjudgement of
vigilance state during prolonged monotonous daytime driving. Accid Anal Prev 41: 1087-93
Shah JC, Mason WD (1990) Plasma codeine and morphine concentrations after a single oral dose of
codeine phosphate. J Clin Pharmacol 30: 764-6
Sindrup SH, Brosen K (1995) The pharmacogenetics of codeine hypoalgesia. Pharmacogenetics 5: 335-46
Somogyi AA, Barratt DT, Coller JK (2007) Pharmacogenetics of opioids. Clin Pharmacol Ther 81: 429-44
Vermeeren A (2004) Residual effects of hypnotics: epidemiology and clinical implications. CNS Drugs 18:
297-328
Verster JC, Pandi-Perumal SR, Ramaekers JG, de Gier JJ (2009) Drugs, Driving and Traffic Safety
Verster JC, Veldhuijzen DS, Volkerts ER (2006) Effects of an opioid (oxycodone/paracetamol) and an NSAID
(bromfenac) on driving ability, memory functioning, psychomotor performance, pupil size, and mood. Clin J
Pain 22: 499-504
Walker DJ, Zacny JP (1998) Subjective, psychomotor, and analgesic effects of oral codeine and morphine in
healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology 140: 191-201
Wilder-Smith OH (2005) Opioid use in the elderly. Eur J Pain 9: 137-40
145
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Zacny JP (1995) A review of the effects of opioids on psychomotor and cognitive functioning in humans.
Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology 3: 432-466
146
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Chapter 7 : Dose related effects of dronabinol on
actual driving performance of occasional and
heavy cannabis users
W.M. Bosker*, A. Surinx*, R. Blankespoor*, J.G. Ramaekers*
Maastricht
University,
Faculty
Psychology
and
Neuroscience,
Dept
Neuropsychology
Psychopharmacology, Experimental Psychopharmacology Unit, Maastricht, The Netherlands
Email: [email protected]
Tel: +31 43 3881518
Fax: +31 43 3884560
147
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
and
Abstract
Dronabinol (Marinol®) is a cannabinoid that is used for the treatment of chronic pain, anorexia in
AIDS and other wasting diseases, and as an antiemetic medication in cancer patients undergoing
chemotherapy. The active ingredient dronabinol is synthetic 9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC), which is know
to cause driving impairment when smoked. The effects of dronabinol on driving have never been assessed
before. This study investigated the effects of dronabinol (Marinol®) on actual and simulated driving
performance and the Field Sobriety Tests (FST). Participants, 12 occasional and 12 daily cannabis users,
were administered 10 and 20 mg dronabinol and placebo according to a double-blind, randomized, 3-way
cross-over design. Performance assessments consisted of actual driving tests and the FST. The aim was to
assess the acute effects of THC on actual driving performance in occasional cannabis users and the degree
of behavioural tolerance to such effects in heavy cannabis users. Results demonstrated that single doses of
dronabinol impaired road tracking performance of occasional cannabis users during on-the-road driving tests
in a dose related manner. Relative to placebo, SDLP significantly increased by approximately 2 and 3 cm
respectively, after dronabinol 10 and 20 mg. The upper limits of the 95%CI associated with change SDLP
exceed the alcohol criterion limit indicating that dronabinol effects after both doses on SDLP were
comparable to the effects of a BAC=0.5 mg/ml. The effects of dronabinol on driving performance of heavy
cannabis users however were less pronounced or even absent. Overall, dronabinol did not affect any driving
measure as compared to placebo in superiority tests. Equivalence testing however indicated that the 95%CI
associated with change scores in SDLP after dronabinol 10 and 20mg contained both the alcohol criterion.
The latter basically indicates large individual variation in change SDLP after both doses dronanbinol. This
suggest that tolerance to the impairing effects of dronabinol is not complete and may cause impairment in
some regular users of cannabis but not in others. In general it is concluded that dronabinol impaired driving
performance in occasional and heavy users in a dose-dependent way. Equivalence tests demonstrated that
dronabinol induced increments in SDLP were bigger than impairment associated with BAC of 0.5 mg/mL in
occasional and heavy users, although the magnitude of driving impairment was generally less in heavy
users.
148
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Introduction
Cannabis is one of the most widely used drugs. Past month users were 15.2 million persons in 2008 in the
US, which is about 6% of the general population aged 12 and older (Substance Abuse and Mental Health
Services Administration, 2009). In Europe this number was estimated to be 12 million (3.6%) in recent years
(European Monitoring Centre for Drugs and Drug Addiction, 2009). Because of its widespread use, the
prevalence of cannabis in the general driving population is also one of the highest after alcohol. A recent
European survey (European Monitoring Centre for Drugs and Drug Addiction, 2008) showed that 2.4% of the
general driving population had ever driven under the influence of cannabis. However, amongst young drivers
this was raised to 30%. This is a cause for concern, especially since about 10% of drivers involved in an
accident were found to have cannabis in their blood.
Several studies have shown that cannabis causes impairment in cognition, psychomotor
performance and actual driving (e.g. Fernandez-Serrano, Perez-Garcia, & Verdejo-Garcia, 2010;
Grotenhermen, et al., 2007; Lamers & Ramaekers, 2001; Ramaekers, Berghaus, van Laar, & Drummer,
2004; Ramaekers, Kauert, Theunissen, Toennes, & Moeller, 2009; Ramaekers, et al., 2006; Ramaekers,
Robbe, & O'Hanlon, 2000). The actual driving impairment caused by cannabis is generally equal to or
greater than that caused by alcohol at a BAC of 0.5 mg/mL (Ramaekers, et al., 2004; Ramaekers, et al.,
2000). Since this BAC is the legal limit in most European countries it makes the impairment caused by
cannabis clinically relevant.
Cannabis is becoming more and more accepted as a medicine. Not only smoked cannabis, but also
orally administered cannabis is being developed for this purpose. Dronabinol (Marinol®) for example is a
cannabinoid that is used to treat anorexia in AIDS patients and other wasting diseases, anti-emesis in cancer
patients undergoing chemotherapy and for chronic pain and is administered orally in capsules. The active
ingredient dronabinol is synthetic 9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) that also occurs as a natural component of
Cannabis sativa L (Marijuana), which is recreationally smoked. The therapeutic range of dronabinol is
between 2.5 and 20 mg/day.
Only a few studies have investigated the pharmacodynamic effects of orally administered dronabinol
in an experimental design. Almost all studied the subjective effects of dronabinol and found significant
positive effects with doses ranging from 5 to 15 mg (Curran, Brignell, Fletcher, Middleton, & Henry, 2002;
Gray, Hart, Christie, & Upadhyaya, 2008; Kirk & de Wit, 1999). In these studies subjects generally scored
higher on items like “Good drug effect”, “Feel drug” and “Feeling stoned” when under the influence of
dronabinol. Kirk and de Wit (1999) studied frequent and infrequent users and found that frequent users were
more tolerant to these subjective effects than infrequent users. However, other studies did not find these
subjective effects with doses of 7.5 mg in daily and weekly users (Goodwin, et al., 2006) and even 20 mg in
occasional users (Menetrey, et al., 2005). In both of these studies only 6 respectively 8 subjects participated,
which could have decreased statistical power. However, Menetrey et al. (2005) did find an effect on
willingness to drive. Subjects indicated on a VAS scale that they were significantly hampered in their
willingness to drive while under the influence of 20 mg dronabinol. Moreover, they also showed significant
impairment on a tracking task in a driving simulator. Psychomotor performance as measured with the digit
symbol substitution task (DSST) was not impaired in another study (Gray, et al., 2008). Neither was
perceptual priming and working memory, but episodic memory and learning were impaired in a dose
149
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
dependent way (Curran, et al., 2002). Cardiovascular measures are generally not affected by dronabinol
(Goodwin, et al., 2006; Gray, et al., 2008). Thus, it seems that subjective effects of dronabinol are generally
positive and there are no indications for cardiovascular effects. Psychomotor and memory effects are more
inconsistent and need to be investigated more thoroughly.
Oral administration of cannabis has a different pharmacokinetic profile than smoked cannabis.
Dronabinol has an onset of action of approximately 0.5 to 1 hour. Its peak effect is at 2 to 4 hours and lasts
for 4 to 6 hours. The bioavailability is only 4-12%. Smoked cannabis on the other hand has on onset of
action within 5-10 minutes, when it reaches peak plasma levels (McGilveray, 2005; Ramaekers, et al., 2006).
Subjective effects also peak within 5-10 minutes and significantly decrease after 1 hour (Ramaekers, et al.,
2009). The bioavailability of smoked THC is on average 30% (McGilveray, 2005). Hence, oral administration
of cannabis has a slower onset of action and is less effective, but the effects last longer compared to smoked
cannabis.
Due to the differences in pharmacokinetics between orally administered and smoked cannabis, the
pharmacodynamic effects might differ as well. Since smoked cannabis is known to cause driving impairment,
it is important to study the effects of orally administered dronabinol on driving performance. We therefore
designed a study that investigated the effects of dronabinol on actual driving, simulated driving and the Field
Sobriety Test (FST) in occasional and daily cannabis users. The daily users represent a model for chronic
use of dronabinol. The current paper will only focus on the actual driving tests. Therefore, the objective was
to assess the differential effect of dronabinol on actual driving in both user groups. We expected the daily
users to be tolerant to the effects of dronabinol. Furthermore, it was hypothesized that the occasional users
would show driving impairment. Results on simulated driving and the FST will be reported elsewhere.
150
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Method
Subjects
Twelve recreational occasional cannabis users and twelve daily users participated in this study.
Fourteen males and ten females equally divided over both user groups received placebo, 10 mg and 20 mg
dronabinol on three separate occasions. Their mean (SE) age was 23.6 (0.6) years. The mean (SE) lifetime
cannabis use was 274.1 (89.6) times for the occasional users and 2444.2 (708.8) times for the daily users.
Subjects were recruited by advertisements at Maastricht University and were paid upon completion of the
study. Before enrollment, all subjects were screened by means of a telephone interview to determine
whether they qualified for the study. The inclusion criteria were experience with cannabis, i.e., 5-36 times in
the last year for occasional users and >160 times in the last year for daily users; free from psychotropic
medication; good physical health as determined by a medical examination; absence of any major medical,
endocrine, and neurological condition; body mass index between 18 and 28; possession of valid driving
license without revocation; and written informed consent. The exclusion criteria were history of drug abuse or
addiction as assessed by means of a medical questionnaire by the physician at the medical checkup;
pregnancy or lactation; cardiovascular abnormalities on electrocardiogram; excessive drinking, i.e., more
than 20 alcoholic consumptions a week; hypertension, i.e., systolic blood pressure over 170 mmHg or
diastolic blood pressure over 100 mmHg; history of or current psychiatric disorder; susceptibility to simulator
sickness; and allergy to sesame oil. If subjects met the inclusion criteria, they received a medical history and
a drug questionnaire to get a more precise view on their health and drug use. Finally, subjects underwent a
medical examination and took part in a training session to get familiar with the tests.
This study was conducted according to the code of ethics on human experimentation established by
the declaration of Helsinki (1964) and amended in Seoul (2008). Approval for the study was obtained from
the Medical Ethics committee of the Academic Hospital of Maastricht and Maastricht University. A permit for
obtaining, storing, and administering cannabis was obtained from the Dutch drug enforcement
administration.
Study design
The study was conducted according to a double-blind, placebo-controlled, randomized, three-way,
cross-over design. Treatments consisted of single doses of placebo, 10 and 20 mg dronabinol. Treatment
orders were balanced over subjects and treatment periods. Placebo and dronabinol were administered orally
in four identically appearing capsules. The wash-out period between treatments was at least 1 week for
occasional users and at least four days for daily users.
Procedure
Subjects were asked to refrain from any drugs 1 week before the medical examination until study
completion. Daily users were allowed to continue their cannabis consumption as they were used to before
enrollment in the study. Subjects were not allowed to drink alcohol and caffeine or smoke tobacco during a
24-h period prior to testing. Subjects were always tested for alcohol and drugs, i.e., tetrahydrocannabinol,
opiates, amphetamine/ecstasy, benzodiazepines, cocaine, and methamphetamine/ecstasy, in breath and
urine respectively upon arrival (between 11:00 and 11:30 a.m.) at the laboratory on test days. Baseline
151
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
measures, i.e. bloodpressure, heartrate, temperature, subjective measures, blood and saliva samples, were
taken between 11:30 a.m. and 12:00 p.m. At 12:00 p.m. subjects received placebo or one of the two
dronabinol doses after which they had a break of half an hour and lunch between 12:30 and 1:00 p.m. After
lunch performance testing started and the timeline of this is displayed in Fig. 1. A testing day ended at 6:15
p.m. at which time subjects were driven home.
Figure 1: Study time table
Actual driving tests
The road tracking test (O'Hanlon, 1984) consists of driving in a specially instrumented car with a
constant speed of 95 km/h and as straight as possible on the right lane of primary highway during a 1 hour
test ride. A video-camera mounted on the rear end of the car registers its lateral position relative to the road
delineation. The images are recorded onto a hard drive in the car with a frequency of 4 Hz and are
transformed into a file containing the measures of the lateral position. An off line editing routine is applied for
removal of all data segments that reveal signal loss, disturbance or occurrence of passing maneuvers. The
edited dataset is then used to calculate means and variances for lateral position. The primary dependent
measures of this test is the standard deviation of lateral position (SDLP; i.e. a measure of weaving). Speed
and standard deviation of speed (SDSP) are recorded as secondary control measures. The highway driving
test has been calibrated in a manner allowing expression of any sedative drug effect in terms of the BAC
required to achieve the equivalent level of driving impairment (Louwerens, Gloerich, de Vries, Brookhuis, &
O’
Hanlon, 1987). The alcohol calibration curve demonstrates that drinkers’mean SDLP rises exponentially
with BAC. Results from the alcohol calibration study can be used for describing drugs’effects on SDLP in
terms of respective BAC equivalencies. The change in SDLP at a BAC of 0.5 mg/ml (i.e. 2.4 cm) has been
used as a criterion level to quantify drug effects. Any drug induced changes in SDLP that exceed this
criterion value are defined as clinically relevant impairing drug effect in the present study.
The car-following test (Brookhuis, de Waard, & Mulder, 1994; Ramaekers, Muntjewerff, & O'Hanlon,
1995) consists of two cars driving in tandem on a secondary road. The leading vehicle is operated by a study
staff member, the following vehicle is operated by the subject who is accompanied by a driving instructor.
The test begins with the two vehicles traveling in tandem at speeds of 100 km/h on a primary highway.
Subjects attempt to drive 50-60 m behind the preceding vehicle and to maintain that headway as it executes
a series of deceleration maneuvers. During the test, the speed of the leading car is automatically controlled
by a modified cruise-control system. At the beginning it is set to maintain a constant speed of 100 km/h, and
by activating a microprocessor, the investigator can start sinusoidal speed changes reaching an amplitude of
-10% and returning to the starting level within 50 sec. The maneuver is repeated 6-10 times. Speed signals
collected during speed maneuvers enter a power spectral analysis for yielding phase-delay between the
vehicle’
s velocities at the maneuver cycle frequency (0.02) Hz. Phase delay converted to a measure of Time
to Speed Adaptation (TSA, in s) is the primary measure. Gain and coherence are secondary control
152
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
measures. Gain is the amplification factor between both speed signals collected from the leading and
following vehicle and indicates the magnitude of overshoot in reaction. Coherence is a measure to control for
correspondence between both speed signals. Test duration is 25 minutes.
Subjective measures
Subjective measures were taken at baseline and throughout a testing day. They consisted of two
questionnaires: the revised Addiction Research Centre Inventory Marijuana Scale (ARCI-M) (Chait,
Fischman, & Schuster, 1985) and a Visual Analogue Scale (VAS). The revised ARCI-M is a twelve item truefalse questionnaire related to marijuana intoxication. The dependent measure was a composite score that
was calculated by adding the answers on the twelve items, with false scored as 0 and true as 1. The seven
item VAS consisted of a 100 mm line anchored with “not at all”and “most ever”. The subjects were asked to
indicate to what extent they experienced “good drug effect”, “high”, “stoned”, “stimulated”, “sedated”,
“anxious”and “depressed”and their score was registered in mm.
At the end of a testing day subjects were asked to indicate on the treatment questionnaire which
drug they thought they had received that day, i.e. placebo, 10 or 20 mg dronabinol.
Pharmacokinetic assessment
Blood samples ( 10 mL) were collected 3 times during a testing day, i.e 1.5, 4.25 and 6 hours postdrug. From daily users an extra sample was taken as a baseline measure, because they continued smoking
their own cannabis in between testing days as they were used to. THC, 11-OH-THC and THC-COOH
concentrations were determined in serum afterwards. The blood sample was centrifuged and the resulting
serum was frozen at -20ºC until analysis.
Statistical analysis
All statistical analyses were conducted by means of SPSS 18.0 for Mac. Statistical analyses
consisted of 2 steps: 1) Assessment for overall treatment effects by means of superiority testing; 2)
Equivalence or non-inferiority testing of drug effects based on difference scores from placebo (within group)
relative to the pre-established alcohol criterion, and 3) determination of concentration effect relations. Steps
2 and 3 were only conducted in case of treatment effects, and only for the primary measures of driving
performance.
During step one all data entered the general linear model (GLM) repeated measures ANOVA
procedures with Drug (3 levels) and Time (4 levels in case of subjective measures) as main within subject
factors. User group (2 levels) were entered as between subject factors. If the sphericity assumption was
violated or not applicable, the Greenhouse-Geisser correction was used. In case of an overall effect of Drug,
separate drug-placebo contrast analyses were conducted for each dronabinol dose. Since the objective of
this study was to investigate the differential effects of dronabinol on driving performance in occasional and
daily users and because we had an apriori hypothesis we repeated the same analysis on the two user
groups separately whenever a main effect or interaction was found.
Step two assessed whether a pre-established alcohol criterion falls within the 95% confidence
interval (CI) of the drug effect. If yes, than the drug effect was considered to be equivalent or bigger than a
153
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
BAC of 0.5 mg/mL, and thus relevant for traffic safety. If the 95% CI was lower than the alcohol criterion
value, than a drug effect was considered of no clinical relevance.
In step 3, concentration-effect relations were determined according to the following procedure. Data
collected during different doses of a drug were converted to change scores from placebo for analyses of the
association between drug concentration and performance. A linear regression analysis was conducted to
establish linear relationships between changes (from placebo) in task performance during drug treatment
and log-transformed drug concentrations in serum. The total number of data points included in these
equations was defined by the number of subjects x maximal number test repetitions x the number of drug
doses. Individual drug concentrations in serum prior to performance assessments in each of the drug dose
conditions were divided over three mutually exclusive categories covering the full range of drug
concentrations. The concentration ranges in serum were 0-50, 50-100 and > 100 ng/mL during evening
sessions and 0-25, 25-50 and >50 ng/mL during morning sessions. The concentration ranges in oral fluid
were 0-250, 250-1000 and >1000 ng/mL during evening sessions and 0-100, 100-500 and >500 ng/mL
during morning sessions Corresponding change scores of task performance were then classified either as
showing “impairment”or “no impairment”for all individual cases within each of these categories. Impairment
was defined as a positive change score from placebo. Binomial tests were applied to measure whether the
proportion of observations showing impairment or no impairment significantly differed from the hypothesized
proportion. It was hypothesized that in case of no effect of a drug on task performance the proportion of
observations showing impairment or no impairment would be equal, i.e. 50%.
154
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Results
Dropouts and missing data
In total 3 occasional users dropped out and they were replaced by other subjects. In all cases
subjects did not want to continue because of the strong drug effects, which they experienced as unpleasant.
Three occasional users had missing data in the highway driving. This was due to heavy rain, which
made it impossible to measure SDLP (placebo condition), computer failure (10 mg condition) and in one
case the subject felt unable to drive because of the medication (20 mg condition). In case of missing data in
the placebo condition the training measurement was taken instead. The other two subjects with missing data
were not used in the analysis. During car following computer failure caused missing data in 6 cases of
occasional users and in 4 cases of daily users. In 3 of these cases of daily users data was missing in 2
conditions. Subjects with missing data were excluded from the analysis. Two occasional users and one daily
user had missing data on the VAS and were not used in the analysis.
Prematurely terminated driving tests
In three cases the highway driving test was prematurely terminated by the driving instructor. In one
case because of heavy snowfall. This condition was replaced on a later day by the same subject. The other
two cases were terminated because subjects were falling asleep while driving. In one case the available data
entered the analysis. In the other case the training measurement was used instead, because it happened in
the placebo condition.
Table 1: Mean (SE) performance on road tracking test and car following test and percentage of subjects
impaired on SFST in every treatment condition. Significance indicated by p-value.
Drug
ANOVA (overall)
Test
Cannabis
Placebo
10 mg
20 mg
Dronabinol x Can- Dronabinol
use history
dronabinol
dronabinol
nabis use history
Road tracking
SDLP (cm)
Mean speed
(km/h)
SD Speed
(km/h)
Occasional
Heavy
Occasional
Heavy
Occasional
Heavy
17.9 (0.8)
19.7 (1.3)
96.1 (0.4)
95.8 (0.2)
2.1 (0.1)
2.2 (0.2)
20.4 (1.2)
21.0 (1.2)
95.9 (0.3)
95.8 (0.4)
2.2 (0.2)
2.3 (0.2)
22.1 (1.4)
21.4 (1.2)
96.1 (0.6)
95.6 (0.3)
2.1 (0.1)
2.3 (0.2)
Occasional
Heavy
Occasional
Heavy
Occasional
Heavy
2.5 (0.4)
3.1 (0.1)
0.9 (0.01)
0.9 (0.01)
1.2 (0.07)
1.2 (0.06)
4.1 (0.3)
3.6 (0.4)
0.9 (0.02)
0.9 (0.02)
1.2 (0.05)
1.2 (0.04)
2.9 (0.3)
3.8 (0.8)
0.9 (0.01)
0.9 (0.01)
1.2 (0.07)
1.4 (0.10)
0,008
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
Car Following
TSA (s)
Coherence
Gain
0,011
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
2
SFST
(% Occasional
impaired)
Heavy
155
8.3
0.0
8.3
16.7
8.3
16.7
NS
NS
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Table 1: Mean (SE) performance on road tracking test and car following test and percentage of subjects
impaired on SFST in every treatment condition. Significance indicated by p-value.
HGN
(% Occasional 0.0
8.3
0.0
NS
impaired)
Heavy
0.0
0.0
0.0
NS
WAT
(% Occasional 8.3
33.3
33.3
NS
impaired)
Heavy
16.7
41.7
50.0
NS
OLS
(% Occasional 16.7
16.7
25.0
NS
impaired)
Heavy
8.3
25.0
16.7
NS
Actual driving performance
The main dependent variable for driving performance is the SDLP of the highway driving test. The
results showed that SDLP increased linearly from placebo to 20 mg dronabinol in a dose dependent fashion
for both user groups (Table 1). Repeated measures analysis indicated a main effect on SDLP for drug
(F2,40=7.812 , p=0.001). Simple contrasts indicated that this effect was due to a significant difference
between placebo and 10 mg dronabinol (p=0.014) as well as a significant difference between placebo and
20 mg dronabinol (p=0.001). Even though the interaction between drug and user group was not significant,
the analysis for the two user groups separately showed that the drug effect was significant for occasional
users (F2,18=6.493 , p=0.008), but not for the daily users (p=0.218). Simple contrasts for the occasional
user group indicated that the effect was significant for 10 mg (0.039) as well as 20 mg versus placebo
(p=0.016). The two control measures of the highway driving test (mean speed and SDSP) did not show a
significant effect on drug (p=0.947 and p=0.460 respectively).
Figure 2. Mean change in SDLP (95%CI) after doses of 10 and 20mg dronabinol in occasional and daily cannabis
users.
The equivalence testing for SDLP (Figure 2) shows that occasional users are significantly impaired
compared to placebo, because zero is not included in the 95% confidence intervals (CIs). It also shows that
156
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
their impairment is relevant for traffic safety, since the SDLP values of at least 0.5 mg/mL BAC are included
in the CIs of both doses. In case of 20 mg even 0.8 mg/mL BAC falls within the CI of the occasional users.
Equivalence tests also demonstrated that in dialy users, the 95%CIs for 10 and 20 mg did exceed the
alcohol criterion limit. In case of 10 mg dronabinol, the CI actually included 0 as well as the 0.5 mg/mL
criterion. (Figure 3).
Figure 3. Individual SDLP change scores from placebo after dronabinol 10 and 20 mg in occasional and daily
users
The primary dependent measure of the car following test is TSA. Results showed a trend effect for
drug on TSA (F2,24=3.083 , p=0.064). Simple contrasts indicated that this effect was significant for placebo
versus 10 mg dronabinol (p=0.005). The interaction between drug and user group was non-significant.
However, the same analysis for the user groups separately showed a significant effect of drug on TSA for
occasional users (F2,10=7.269 , p=0.011) and not for daily users (p=0.609). Simple contrasts for the
occasional user group indicated that this was due to the difference between placebo and 10 mg (p=0.029).
The two control measures of the car following tests (coherence and gain) did not show any significant effect.
Table 2: Mean (SE) scores on the subjective questionnaires. Significance indicated by p-value.
Measure
User
157
Occasional
Daily
Occasional
DRUID 6th framework programme
Daily
Occasional
Daily
Deliverable 1.2.2
Table 2: Mean (SE) scores on the subjective questionnaires. Significance indicated by p-value.
Measure
Scale
Placebo
Drug
10 mg
dronabinol
20 mg
dronabinol
Drug
WS
ANOV Time
A
Drug x Time
Drug x User
BS
ANOV Time x User
A
User
1
2
3
4
1
2
3
4
1
2
3
4
ARCI-M Total
VAS Good drug effect
VAS High
0.00 (0.00)
0.25 (0.25)
0.08 (0.08)
0.08 (0.08)
0.00 (0.00)
3.75 (1.05)
3.92 (1.10)
2.42 (0.67)
0.00 (0.00)
4.25 (0.94)
4.42 (0.91)
4.17 (0.93)
0.4 (0.8)
5.9 (6.0)
0.5 (0.3)
1.1 (0.2)
0.5 (1.1)
16.5 (6.4)
15.4 (1.3)
6.6 (0.4)
0.4 (0.8)
27.4 (9.1)
29.6 (3.8)
25.3 (1.2)
1.2 (0.8)
7.1 (6.0)
1.0 (0.3)
0.6 (0.2)
1.5 (1.1)
17.0 (6.4)
3.4 (1.3)
1.1 (0.4)
1.5 (0.8)
28.2 (9.1)
12.6 (3.8)
2.1 (1.2)
0.5 (0.8)
3.6 (4.5)
0.8 (0.7)
0.7 (0.1)
0.5 (1.0)
12.6 (4.2)
12.0 (1.2)
3.7 (0.4)
0.6 (0.3)
23.4 (8.8)
19.2 (3.0)
12.5 (0.9)
1.2 (0.8)
5.8 (4.5)
1.9 (0.7)
0.4 (0.1)
1.4 (1.0)
10.8 (4.2)
3.4 (1.2)
1.0 (0.4)
0.8 (0.3)
28.9 (8.8)
11.0 (3.0)
2.2 (0.9)
0.42 (0.15)
0.08 (0.08)
0.00 (0.00)
0.00 (0.00)
0.25 (0.18)
1.75 (0.54)
0.25 (0.13)
0.00 (0.00)
0.25 (0.18)
2.17 (0.59)
1.33 (0.53)
0.33 (0.26)
0,000
0,009
0,000
0,021
0,004
0,006
0,000
0,010
0,002
0,012
0,001
0,010
0,031
0,006
NS
0,099
NS
0,020
0,001
0,098
NS
0,000
NS
NS
0,000
NS
NS
Subjective measures
Results on the VAS showed a main effect for drug on all scales except depression, i.e. good drug
effect, high, stoned, stimulated, sedated, anxious and a main effect of time on all scales except anxious and
depression (see Table 2). Simple contrasts indicated that for all significant drug effects placebo differed
significantly from 10 mg as well as 20 mg dronabinol and for all significant time effects all measures differed
significantly from baseline. Interaction between time and user group were significant for sedated and
anxious. Contrasts indicated that this was due to a difference from baseline for measure 3 and 4 for sedated
and a difference from baseline for measure 2 and 3 for anxious. The interaction between drug and time was
also significant for all measures except anxious and depression, meaning that the drug effect differed for the
various time points. Contrasts generally showed that for the 10 mg dronabinol versus placebo less time
points differed significantly from baseline than for the 20 mg dronabinol versus placebo. The between
subjects factor user group was significant on the anxious scale.
When these analyses were repeated for the two user groups separately, results were generally
confirmed (Figure 4). The main effect of drug was significant in both user groups for good drug effect, high,
stoned and stimulated. The difference between daily and occasional users was that for occasional users this
effect was true for the 10 mg as well as 20 mg dronabinol versus placebo and for the daily users only the
difference between placebo and 20 mg dronabinol reached significance. Daily users also had a significant
drug effect on the sedated scale for both drug conditions. The same pattern is visible for the main effect of
time. Good drug effect, high, stoned and sedated differed significantly from baseline at all time points for
158
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
occasional users and at all time points except the last one for daily users. On the scale stimulated occasional
and daily users were comparable with a significant difference from baseline for all time points except the last
one. The drug by time interaction was non-significant for the occasional users, but the daily users had a
significant interaction on the high, stoned and sedated scales. Contrasts showed that only 20 mg dronabinol
versus placebo differed significantly from baseline at time points 2 and 3. Additionally, on the sedated scale
10 mg versus placebo differed significantly from baseline at time point 2.
Figure 4: subjective ratings after 10 and 20mg dronabinol in occasional users (upper pane;) and daily users
(lowe panel) as a function of time after dosing
The repeated measure analysis of the ARCI-M showed a main effect of drug (F2,44=25.118,
p=0.000), a main effect of time (F3,66=16.921, p=0.000), an interaction of drug by user (F2,44=8.728,
p=0.001), an interaction of time by user (F3,66=8.799, p=0.000) as well as a drug by time interaction
(F2.752,60.555=5.929, p=0.002). Simple contrasts indicated that both drug conditions were significantly
different from placebo for the main drug effect and the interaction with user group and that all measures
differed significantly from baseline in the main effect of time as well as the interaction with user group. The
drug by time interaction was also significant for all drug levels and at all time points compared to placebo and
baseline respectively. The between subjects factor user group was also significant (F1,22=17.770, p=0.000).
The analysis for the two user groups separately generally confirmed the above findings (see Table 2
and Figure 5 for details). The results for the occasional users did not change, but in the daily user group the
main effect of time was only significant for the change from baseline at time 2. The drug by time interaction in
the daily user group was significantly different from baseline at time 2 for 10 mg dronabinol versus placebo
and at time 2 and 3 for 20 mg dronabinol versus placebo. The main drug effect of the daily users stayed the
same as above.
159
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Figure 5. Mean ARCI-M after all treatments in occasional and daily users.
Pharmacokinetic assessment
Pharmacokinetic data was missing on 18 occasions. The mean (SE) values for THC, 11-OH-THC and THCCOOH are displayed in Table 3. The baseline measures for the daily users did not differ significantly
between the three drug conditions (F1.086,8.686=1.971, p=0.196).
Table 3: THC, 11-OH-THC and THC-COOH concentrations in blood.
User
Occasional
Measure
Placebo
1
2
3
0.0 (0.0)
0.0 (0.0)
0.0 (0.0)
4.9 (1.7)
1.8 (0.5)
37.2 (11.6)
4
0.0 (0.0)
0.0 (0.0)
0.0 (0.0)
4.4 (1.6)
1.5 (0.4)
31.2 (9.4)
2.4 (0.7)
3.5 (0.7)
20.8 (5.5)
7.6 (3.6)
9.2 (2.1)
2.9 (1.2)
6.2 (0.9)
41.8 (11.2)
54.4 (8.8)
3
1.2 (0.5)
1.6 (0.3)
18.5 (2.8)
4.6 (1.1)
3.1 (0.4)
43.4 (7.4)
4
0.9 (0.2)
1.7 (0.3)
16.8 (1.3)
4.2 (1.0)
2.3 (0.4)
37.2 (7.6)
4.5 (1.3)
5.0 (1.4)
30.5 (10.3)
11.2 (5.3)
10.8 (2.0)
3.9 (1.7)
7.7 (1.0)
48.1 (13.9)
57.5 (9.7)
3
1.4 (0.3)
2.6 (0.5)
24.0 (3.7)
6.8 (1.2)
4.7 (0.5)
54.8 (9.3)
4
2.6 (0.6)
3.1 (0.5)
30.2 (4.3)
5.7 (1.2)
3.6 (0.4)
57.5 (9.9)
10 mg dronabinol 1
2
20 mg dronabinol 1
2
160
Daily
THC ( g/L) 11-OH-THC
THCTHC ( g/L) 11-OH-THC THC-COOH
g/L)
COOH( g/L)
g/L)
g/L)
15.3 (8.5)
5.1 (2.3)
58.2 (17.5)
0.0 (0.0)
0.0 (0.0)
0.0 (0.0)
6.4 (2.7)
2.5 (0.8)
37.4 (9.9)
-
-
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Discussion
This study was designed to assess the effects of two doses dronabinol, i.e. 10 and 20 mg, on driving
performance in occasional and daily cannabis users. These doses are within the normal range prescribed for
patients suffering from anorexia in AIDS and other wasting deseases, emesis due to chemotherapy in cancer
patients and for chronic pain.
Results of this study showed that occasional cannabis users were impaired
on the highway driving test as well as on the car following test when driving under the influence of
dronabinol. Their SDLP increased significantly with on average 2.5 cm in the 10 mg dronabinol condition and
4.2 cm in the 20 mg dronabinol condition compared to placebo. This means that occasional users had
increased weaving and thus diminished automatic control over the car to an extent that is relevant for traffic
safety. In the car following test occasional users had increased TSA. This indicates that they reacted slower
to the changes in speed of the car in front of them when under the influence of 10 mg dronabinol compared
to placebo. Interestingly, performance under the influence of 20 mg dronabinol did not reach significance and
was better than that with 10 mg dronabinol. This is probably due to the large amount of missing data. Only 6
subjects had complete data and entered the analysis. If the other 6 would have been included, results could
have been different. The overall effect, when occasional and daily users are used as a between subjects
factor, also just missed significance, probably due to the same reason. Therefore, this result should be
interpreted with caution. However, the fact that occasional users in the 10 mg condition did reach
significance with this amount of subjects, points toward a strong effect.
In daily users, mean SDLP did not differ between treatments according to superiority tests. However,
equivalence test demonstrated that the 95% CI associated with change SDLP after both doses of dronabinol
included the criterion value equivalent to BAC of 0.5 mg/mL. A relatively wide range of these 95% CIs was
caused by large inter-individual variations in change SDLP observed after both doses. Inspection of
individual data indicated that approximately 25% of the heavy users demonstrated impairment in road
tracking performance that was equivalent or worse than that observed after BAC of 0.5 mg/mL. Together,
these data show that THC induced impairments of road tracking performance in heavy cannabis are less as
compared to those observed in occasional users. This reduction in sensitivity to the impairing effects of THC
in heavy users has been reported before and has been interpreted as a demonstration of behavioral
tolerance that develops after repeated cannabis use Ramaekers et al. (2009, 2010). This study however also
demonstrates that behavioral tolerance was not complete in every heavy cannabis user as indicated by large
inter-individual differences in driving impairments observed in these drivers.
Tolerance to the effects of dronabinol in chronic cannabis users has been shown in a recent
study as well (Bedi, et al., 2010). HIV positive patients who were also daily cannabis users were evaluated
on caloric intake as well as subjective and cognitive measures. Within 16 days tolerance developed to the
therapeutic effects, i.e. caloric intake and sleep quality, in these patients who received 4 times per day 10 mg
dronabinol. However, the mood-enhancing effects did not disappear over the 16 days test period. Thus,
tolerance developed for some specific effects and not for others.
We used the daily cannabis user group as a model for chronic dronabinol use. Chronic use of
dronabinol might be necessary in patient populations for which the drug has been proven useful, i.e. AIDS
patients, cancer patients undergoing chemotherapy and chronic pain patients. Considering the fast
development of tolerance in the study of Bedi et al. (2010) and the results of the current study, this model
161
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
seems valid. The consequence of these results is that chronic users might need higher doses to reach
therapeutic effects. Although we did not show a significant effect of dronabinol on driving performance in
daily cannabis users, some subjects in this group should be classified as impaired. Compared to Bedi et al.
(2010) our dose was low: 4 times per day 10 mg versus once 20 mg as our highest dose. Therefore, if
chronic users need even higher doses than used in that study, traffic safety might be comprised for these
individuals. It seems that even with the current dose in our study some subjects are not tolerant to the effects
on driving performance. Higher doses would probably increase the number of subjects showing impairment.
Subjective effects were also assessed in this study to determine tolerance to these effects as well in
daily users. We used two measures for subjective effects: ARCI-M and VAS. The ARCI-M showed significant
effects on all factors. In general the subjects experienced marijuana related effects when under the influence
of both doses dronabinol. Effects lasted up to 6 hours after drug intake. There were differences between
occasional and daily users. In general occasional users scored higher on the ARCI-M indicating that they
experienced more marijuana related effects, i.e. more items were answered positively, than the daily users.
The effects generally lasted longer for occasional users as well. Daily users experienced longer lasting
effects in the 20 mg than in the 10 mg dronabinol condition.
The VAS showed a similar effect: in general the positive drug effects, i.e. good drug effect, high,
stoned and stimulated, were increased under the influence of dronabinol. Occasional users reported these
effects for both doses, whereas daily users only for the 20 mg dronabinol condition. Again these effects were
experienced throughout a testing day especially in occasional users. The daily users generally reported the
effects up to 4.25 hours after drug intake. User group differed significantly for the negative effects, i.e.
sedation and anxiety. Daily users reported more sedation under the influence of dronabinol than occasional
users, which lasted longer in the 20 than in the 10 mg condition. Occasional users experienced more anxiety
than daily users, especially toward the end of a testing day, i.e. 4.25 and 6 hours after drug intake.
From these results it is clear that daily cannabis users were not tolerant to the subjective effects of
dronabinol. They experienced less effects though, that were less intense and shorter lasting than occasional
cannabis users. Other studies found the same in various population. The study with HIV positive patients
mentioned earlier found that these patients, who were daily cannabis users, experienced positive mood
effects and elevated ratings of drug high, good drug effect, drug liking and wanting to take the drug again up
to 16 days with 4 times daily 10 mg dronabinol compared to placebo (Bedi, et al., 2010). As in our study
these patients also experienced sedation, especially in the second testing week (day 9-16). The authors
concluded that the subjects were not tolerant to these subjective effects. Another study compared frequent
and infrequent users on subjective measures and found that frequent users reported higher ratings on feel
drug, high and ARCI-M in a low dose condition, i.e. 7.5 mg dronabinol, than infrequent users (Kirk & de Wit,
1999). In the high dose condition, i.e. 15 mg dronabinol, both user groups reported elevated scores on these
measures. The infrequent users also reported more negative effects, i.e. sedation, compared to frequent
users and lower ratings on stimulant-like effects and drug liking compared to placebo in the high dose
condition, which was not found in the frequent users. Based on this the authors conclude that the frequent
users might be tolerant to some of the subjective effects, especially the negative effects that were only found
in infrequent users. However, their results clearly point out that frequent users are not tolerant to the positive
effects as was found in the current study. The discrepancies between this study and ours, especially on
sedation which was more elevated in our daily users than our occasional users, is probably due to
162
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
differences in dose. We used 10 and 20 mg dronabinol, whereas they used 7.5 and 15 mg. Moreover, the
frequent users of this study used less cannabis than our daily users: 3.5 versus 13.4 joints per week.
The conclusion from our study is that it is unsafe to drive under the influence of a therapeutic dose of
dronabinol. Chronic users might become less sensitive to the impairing effects to some extent. However, not
all daily users had developed tolerance and some drove comparable or worse than subjects under the
influence of 0.5 mg/mL BAC, which is the legal limit in most European countries.
163
DRUID 6th framework programme
Deliverable 1.2.2
Acknowledgement
This work was conducted as part of the Driving under the influence of drugs, alcohol and medicines
(DRUID) research consortium funded by European Union grant TREN-05-FP6TR-S07.61320-518404DRUID.
This report reflects only the author's view. The European Community is not liable for any use that may
be made of the information contained therein.
References
Bedi, G., Foltin, R. W., Gunderson, E. W., Rabkin, J., Hart, C. L., Comer, S. D., et al. (2010). Efficacy
and tolerability of high-dose dronabinol maintenance in HIV-positive marijuana smokers: a controlled
laboratory study. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 212(4), 675-686.
Brookhuis, K. A., de Waard, D., & Mulder, B. (1994). Measuring driving performance by car-following
in traffic. Ergonomics, 37(3), 427-434.
Chait, L. D., Fischman, M. W., & Schuster, C. R. (1985). 'Hangover' effects the morning after
marijuana smoking. Drug Alcohol Depend, 15(3), 229-238.
Curran, H. V., Brignell, C., Fletcher, S., Middleton, P., & Henry, J. (2002). Cognitive and subjective
dose-response effects of acute oral Delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) in infrequent cannabis users.
Psychopharmacology (Berl), 164(1), 61-70.
European Monitoring Centre for Drugs and Drug Addiction (2008). EMCDDA Insights Series No 8:
Drug use, impaired driving and traffic accidents.: Luxembourg: Office for official publications of the
European Communities.
European Monitoring Centre for Drugs and Drug Addiction (2009). Annual report 2009: the state of the
drugs problem in Europe: Luxembourg: Publications Office of the European Union.
Fernandez-Serrano, M. J., Perez-Garcia, M., & Verdejo-Garcia, A. (2010). What are the specific vs.
generalized effects of drugs of abuse on neuropsychological performance? Neurosci Biobehav Rev.
Goodwin, R. S., Gustafson, R. A., Barnes, A., Nebro, W., Moolchan, E. T., & Huestis, M. A. (2006).
Delta(9)-tetrahydrocannabinol,
11-hydroxy-delta(9)-tetrahydrocannabinol
and
11-nor-9-carboxy-
delta(9)-tetrahydrocannabinol in human plasma after controlled oral administration of cannabinoids.
Ther Drug Monit, 28(4), 545-551.
Gray, K. M., Hart, C. L., Christie, D. K., & Upadhyaya, H. P. (2008). Tolerability and effects of oral
Delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol in older adolescents with marijuana use disorders. Pharmacol Biochem
Behav, 91(1), 67-70.
Grotenhermen, F., Leson, G., Berghaus, G., Drummer, O. H., Kruger, H. P., Longo, M., et al. (2007).
Developing limits for driving under cannabis. Addiction, 102(12), 1910-1917.
Kirk, J. M., & de Wit, H. (1999). Responses to oral delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol in frequent and
infrequent marijuana users. Pharmacol Biochem Behav, 63(1), 137-142.
Lamers, C. T., & Ramaekers, J. G. (2001). Visual search and urban driving under the influence of
marijuana and alcohol. Hum Psychopharmacol, 16(5), 393-401.
Louwerens, J., Gloerich, A., de Vries, G., Brookhuis, K., & O’Hanlon, J. (1987). The relationship
between drivers’blood alcohol concentration (BAC) and actual driving performance during high speed
travel. Alcohol Drugs Traffic Saf, 86, 183-186.
McGilveray, I. J. (2005). Pharmacokinetics of cannabinoids. Pain Res Manag, 10(A), 15A-22A.
164
Menetrey, A., Augsburger, M., Favrat, B., Pin, M. A., Rothuizen, L. E., Appenzeller, M., et al. (2005).
Assessment of driving capability through the use of clinical and psychomotor tests in relation to blood
cannabinoids levels following oral administration of 20 mg dronabinol or of a cannabis decoction made
with 20 or 60 mg Delta9-THC. J Anal Toxicol, 29(5), 327-338.
O'Hanlon, J. F. (1984). Driving performance under the influence of drugs: rationale for, and application
of, a new test. Br J Clin Pharmacol, 18 Suppl 1, 121S-129S.
Ramaekers, J. G., Berghaus, G., van Laar, M., & Drummer, O. H. (2004). Dose related risk of motor
vehicle crashes after cannabis use. Drug Alcohol Depend, 73(2), 109-119.
Ramaekers, J. G., Kauert, G., Theunissen, E., Toennes, S., & Moeller, M. (2009). Neurocognitive
performance during acute THC intoxication in heavy and occasional cannabis users. J
Psychopharmacol.
Ramaekers, J. G., Moeller, M. R., van Ruitenbeek, P., Theunissen, E. L., Schneider, E., & Kauert, G.
(2006). Cognition and motor control as a function of Delta9-THC concentration in serum and oral fluid:
limits of impairment. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 85(2), 114-122.
Ramaekers, J. G., Muntjewerff, N. D., & O'Hanlon, J. F. (1995). A comparative study of acute and
subchronic effects of dothiepin, fluoxetine and placebo on psychomotor and actual driving
performance. Br J Clin Pharmacol, 39(4), 397-404.
Ramaekers, J. G., Robbe, H. W., & O'Hanlon, J. F. (2000). Marijuana, alcohol and actual driving
performance. Hum Psychopharmacol, 15(7), 551-558.
Ramaekers, J. G., Theunissen, E. L., de Brouwer, M., Toennes, S. W., Moeller, M. R., & Kauert, G.
(2010). Tolerance and cross-tolerance to neurocognitive effects of THC and alcohol in heavy cannabis
users. Psychopharmacology (Berl).
Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (2009). Results from the 2008 National
Survey on Drug Use and Health: National Findings: Office of Applied Studies, NSDUH Series H-36,
HHS Publication No. SMA 09-4434). Rockville, MD.
165
Chapter 8 : Effects of analgetic medication on
actual driving
Markus Schumacher*, Anja Knoche*, Mark Vollrath‡, Frank Petzke°, Ricarda Jantos†, Eric Vuurman,̃
Jan Ramaekers˜
*
Federal Highway Research Institute (BASt)
Section U3 / Traffic Psychology, Traffic Medicine
Bruederstraße 53
D-51427 Bergisch Gladbach, Germany
‡
Technische Universität Braunschweig
Institut für Psychologie
Ingenieur- und Verkehrspsychologie
Gaussstr. 23
D-38106 Braunschweig, Germany
° Georg-August-Universität
Schmerz-Tagesklinik und -Ambulanz
Zentrum Anaesthesiologie, Rettungs- und Intensivmedizin
Robert-Koch-Str. 40
37075 Göttingen, Germany
†
Universität Heidelberg
Institut für Rechts- und Verkehrsmedizin
Voß-Straße 2
D- 69115 Heidelberg, Germany
˜Maastricht University
Dept of Neuropsychology & Psychopharmacology,
Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience
Universiteitssingel 40
6229 ER Maastricht, The Netherlands
166
Abstract
Nowadays morphine-like drugs, called opioids (e.g. fentanyl, oxycodone, buprenorphine, and
hydromorphone) are widely used to treat chronic non-cancer pain. Some studies have already
demonstrated that long term treatment with opioids for non-cancer pain does not impair driving related
skills. But there is still a lack of studies investigating actual driving performance. Twenty patients
suffering from chronic non-cancer pain, on long term treatment with opioid analgesics (fentanyl,
buprenorphine, oxycodone, hydromorphone or morphine), aged between 35 and 68 years,
participated in this study. Their performance in an on-the-road driving test was compared to the
performance of an age independent sample of healthy controls aged between 23 years to 58 years.
Performance of healthy controls with blood alcohol concentration (BAC) of 0.5‰ was used as
reference. The on-the-road driving tests consisted of a road-tracking test and a car-following test. The
primary outcome measure was standard deviation of lateral position (SDLP). Time to speed adaption
(TSA) and brake reaction time (BRT) was measured in the Car-following test. Whereas alcohol
increased SDLP in healthy controls by about 2.4 cm, no difference in SDLP between patients and
controls was found. Neither TSA nor BRT differed between patients and controls. Also no effect of
alcohol on these measures was found. The data indicates that patients with chronic non-cancer pain
are not necessarily unfit to drive but due to the individual variability of test results an individual
assessment is recommended.
Keywords
Chronic non-cancer pain, opioids, driving ability, on-the-road driving
Introduction
According to a recent review of publications the prevalence of chronic pain in the general population
ranges from 10% to over 40% (Nickel & Raspe, 2001). Pain is classified being chronic when it persists
for three month or longer (Mersky & Bogduk, 1994). The pharmacological management of chronic pain
has changed over the last decade. Based on an optimistic assessment of the beneficial effects of
morphine-like drugs, called opioids (e.g. fentanyl, oxycodone, buprenorphine, and hydromorphone) on
pain intensity and functional capabilities, various opioids were widely used to treat chronic non-cancer
pain conditions (Portenoy, 2000). More recently it became evident that the uncritical use of opioids
may also cause considerable problems (e.g. dose escalation due to lack of relevant effect,
development of tolerance with loss of efficacy). Therefore Guidelines like LONTS (Long-Term
Treatment with Opioids for Non-Cancer Pain by the German Association for the Study of Pain
(Schmidt, Lowenstein, Hesselbarth, Klinge, & Michels, 2010) have formulated a more cautious
approach to opioid therapy in patients with chronic non-cancer pain.
The availability and clinical evidence for efficacy of better tolerated neuromodulatory drugs like
gabapentin and pregabalin, as well as both traditional and newer antidepressant agents like
amitriptyline and duloxetine have broadened the pharmacological treatment spectrum. These agents
are particularly effective in conditions where neuropathic or mixed pain aetiology was found as well as
in musculoskeletal pain syndromes where a central hypersensitivity may be relevant. These
developments have led to combination therapy in many patients with chronic pain. Although there is
167
little scientific evidence to support this approach, it is widely established and accepted in clinical
practice. Hence today monotherapy with opioids is the exception rather than the rule.
It is well documented that cognitive functions like attention are impaired in chronic pain patients (e.g.
Dick & Rashiq, 2007; Eccleston, Crombez, Aldrich, & Stannard, 1997; Grisart & Plaghki, 1999)
whereas relief of pain might diminish these impairments (Dick et al., 2007; Eccleston et al., 1997;
Grisart, 2009). Attention deficits may lead to reduced fitness to drive. Lagarde, Chastang, Lafont,
Coeuret-Pellicer, & Chiron (2005) showed that pain alone or in combination with pain treatment was
associated with an increased risk of being involved in traffic accidents. However it was not possible to
distinguish in this study between the negative effects of pain itself and those of pain treatment. Opioids
are often prescribed in the treatment of chronic pain (McQuay, 1999) but are associated with side
effects in the gastrointestinal system as well as in the central nervous system. Drowsiness and
sleepiness are symptoms frequently reported (Kalso, Edwards, Moore, & McQuay, 2004; Moore &
McQuay, 2005). Several studies showed that driving under the influence of sedating medications or
drugs, especially of alcohol, increases accident risk (Drummer et al., 2004; Movig et al., 2004). The
prevalence of drivers tested positive for opioids in a recent study ranged from 1% to 5%. Overall, the
correlation between opioid use and culpability in a recent study was very low (Drummer et al., 2004).
Studies published earlier support the opinion that the prevalence of opioids use in traffic is very low
and is not associated with motor vehicle accidents or motor vehicle fatalities (Fishbain, Cutler,
Rosomoff, & Rosomoff, 2002; Fishbain, Cutler, Rosomoff, & Rosomoff, 2003; Lenné, Dietze,
Rumbold, Redman, & Triggs, 2000). Beside these epidemiological studies, a limited number of
experimental studies have been published aiming at measuring driving ability of chronic pain patients
(see Kress & Kraft, 2005 for a review and Veldhuijzen, Karscha, & van Wijck, 2009 for an overview).
No performance decrement, measured in a set of tests, was found in cancer pain patients treated with
stable doses of oral morphine (Vainio, Ollila, Matikainen, Rosenberg, & Kalso, 1995). Sabatowski et
al. (2003) used the computerized test set that is also used for testing traffic offenders in Germany.
They assessed driving ability of chronic non-cancer pain patients treated with transdermal fentanyl
and found that their performance was non-inferior to that of a control group. The results of two other
studies with the same experimental design and the same test set but with different opioid analgesics
were similar: No overall impairment was found for the long-term use of transdermal buprenorphine
(Dagtekin et al., 2007) and also not for the long-term use of controlled release oxycodone (Gaertner et
al., 2006). However, particular patients had poor results. The assumption, that no general impairment
occurs due to long-term treatment with opioids was also supported by Strumpf, Willweber-Strumpf,
Herberg, & Zenz (2005).
Gaertner et al. (2008) examined the effects of opioid drug dose changes on cognitive and
psychomotor performances relevant for driving. Seven days after dose change, no deterioration was
observed. Tassain et al. (2003) analyzed the performance of pain patients before the initiation of
opioid therapy with slow release morphine. This initial performance was compared to the performance
after 3, 6 and 12 month under stable medication. Results indicate that stable opioid medication did not
lead to a deterioration of cognitive functioning during long-term application.
Patients with chronic pain often report cognitive complaints (McCracken & Iverson, 2001) and it is well
known that an association exists between fatigue and pain (Fishbain et al., 2003). The performance
impairments might be caused by side effects of opioids but these side effects decline over time
168
(Dellemijn, van Duijn, & Vanneste, 1998; Tassain et al., 2003). In addition, those who use opioids
occasionally or habitually (treatment of chronic pain or opioid abusers) are much less likely to be
impaired (Zacny, 1995). Test indicators of cognitive ability and psychomotor function increase over
time (Jamison et al., 2003). Moreover, it was assumed that the impairing effect of pain is more
profound than the impairment caused by side effects of the opioids (Sjogren, Olsen, Thomsen, &
Dalberg, 2000). The assumption that pain itself is impairing is supported by the study of Veldhuijzen et
al. (2006). In this study an on-the-road driving test was used which was developed in the nineteen
eighties and was applied in numerous studies on the impairments caused by psychoactive drugs and
alcohol (Verster & Ramaekers, 2008). The standard deviation of lateral position (SDLP), measuring
the range of weaving, was used as dependant measure in this test. The difference in SDLP of pain
patients and healthy controls who were matched for age, education and driving experience,
corresponded to the difference in SDLP observed in healthy volunteers driving sober vs. those driving
with blood alcohol concentration (BAC) of 0.8‰ . Psychotropic drug use was not allowed in this study
and the difference between patients and controls remained significant even when patients who had
used paracetamol and/or NSAIDs during the study were excluded from the data analysis. In contrast
to opioid analgesics both substances are not psychoactive. So the study demonstrated that chronic
pain itself impaired driving performance. The results underline the need for effective pain treatment in
order to enhance traffic safety. No assumption could have been drawn from this study on the
impairments caused by psychoactive substances prescribed for pain treatment. Sabatowski, Kaiser, &
Gossrau (2010) stated that to this date no decision could be drawn explicitly from the available studies
whether slow release opioid formulations lead to impaired fitness to drive in pain patients or not. This
assumption was mainly based on studies in which driving related skills were assessed. Up to now no
study exists in which driving performance of pain patients under long term combination therapy was
assessed in an on-the-road driving test.
In order to better reflect the target group of pain patients treated with combination therapy, chronic
pain patients treated with a variable mixture of other compounds in addition to a level 3 opioid were
recruited for this study. The sample reflects the current standard of pharmacological treatment of
chronic pain. It is more helpful to study this combination therapy, with the opioid being the most potent
drug affecting the ability to drive, to gain a realistic and clinically relevant picture of driving ability of
patients suffering from chronic pain, than focusing on the specific effects of a given opioid.
Methods
This study compared the driving ability of patients receiving opioid analgesic of WHO level 3 for
chronic non-cancer pain treatment with a group of healthy controls. Performance of the controls
driving with BAC of 0.5‰ was used as a reference for the impairment. Driving ability was measured by
two standardized driving scenarios widely used in the field of drug research: Road tracking test
(O'Hanlon, 1984) and Car-following test (Brookhuis, de Waard, & Mulder, 1994).
Subjects
All subjects gave written informed consent, possessed a valid driver’
s license for passenger cars,
travelled at least 2000 km in the preceding 12 month and were driving on a regular basis (at least
once a week). This study aimed at comparing the performance of pain patients to a representative
169
cross-section of the driving population. Therefore controls were stratified for age (20-30, 30-40, 40-50,
50-65). Since prevalence of chronic pain increases with age and men are more often affected by
chronic pain than women (Nickel et al., 2001) the average age differs between both groups as can be
seen in table 2. All subjects were given financial compensation for participating in this study.
Patients
No study related change in prescribed medication was made. 13 male and 7 female patients (see
table 2) of the pain outpatient department of the University Hospital of Cologne aged between 35 and
68 (m= 54, sd = 8.91) suffering from non-cancer pain responsive to opioids were enrolled in the study.
They have been treated for at least four weeks with either transdermal fentanyl (
12
g/h),
transdermal buprenorphine (10 g/h), slow release oxycodone ( 10 mg/day), slow release oxycodone
combined with naloxone ( 10 mg/day), slow release hydromorphone ( 4 mg /day) or slow release
morphine ( 20 mg/day). The dosage of the analgesic remained unchanged for at least 14 days prior
to the assessment. All patients received co-medication on a stable dose at least during the past 14
days. Regular intake of benzodiazepines ( 4 times per week) and barbiturates (> 3 times per week)
was not allowed. In addition, the daily intake of antidepressants and anticonvulsant had to be below a
given dose limit (see table 1). Patients had to abstain from intake of immediate release opioids,
benzodiazepines, barbiturates and alcohol for at least two days prior to the assessment.
Table 1: Co-medication dose limits.
antidepressants
anticonvulsants
amitryptilin
carbamazepin
doxepin
75mg
75mg
oxcarbazepin
1200mg,
1800mg,
imipramin
75mg
gabapentin
2400mg,
trazodon
100mg
pregabalin
600mg
sertralin
50mg
fluoxetin
20mg
fluvoxamin 75mg
duloxetin
venlafaxin
120mg
225mg
citalopram 10mg
Controls
12 male and 7 female healthy controls participated in this study (see table 2). Their mean (sd) age
was 43 (11.07) ranging from 23 years to 58 years. Participants were selected from a pool of
volunteers that had given consent to participation in studies conducted by BASt in advance. Exclusion
criteria were history of alcohol addiction or abuse, intake of medicaments prohibiting the consumption
of alcohol or psychoactive substances with known impairing effect on driving ability. Women who were
170
pregnant or breast feeding or women of child-bearing potential who were not using a highly effective
contraception method with a pearl-index
1 were also excluded due to the alcohol administration in
the course of the study.
All male but only five of seven female drivers completed the driving test sober as well as under the
influence of alcohol. Two female controls terminated their participation due to personal reasons not
related to the study. The total number of controls of which data of both conditions (sober and under
alcohol influence) is available is 17 (89 %).
Table 2: Sample characteristics.
characteristic
patients
controls
(N = 20)
(N = 19)
age (years)
mean (sd)
54 (8.91)
43 (11.07)
gender
male N (%)
13 (65%)
12 (63%)
female N (%)
7 (35%)
7 (37%)
median km (range)
8500
12000
(1200 - 25000)
(2000 - 50000)
driving
experience
(km/last 12 month):
driving
license
median (range)
34 (17 - 50)
25 (5 - 40)
pain
median (range)
12.5 (5 - 27)
-
musculoskeletal
16
-
visceral pain
2
-
other
2
-
opioid analgesic N
fentanyl
5 (25 µg/h; 25 µg/h – 75 µg/h)
(median;
buprenorphine
1 (87.5 µg/h; --)
oxycodone
5 (140 mg/d; 30 mg/d – 150 mg/d)
hydromorphone
6 (26 mg/d; 16 mg/d – 56 mg/d)
morphinesulfate
3 (120 mg/d; 60 mg/d – 140 mg/)
(years)
duration
of
(years)
diagnosis (N)
range)
dose
Study design
This study was conducted according to a mixed within-between design. Treatment consisted of
alcohol administration before the driving test vs. no alcohol administration and was only done in the
control group. The medication of involved pain patients was not changed during the study.
Hence, patients did the driving test once, whereas controls did the driving test twice. To fulfil all safety
requirements according to the protocol, the first driving test of controls always had to be done sober.
Accordingly, the sequence of driving could not be balanced but it was adhered to a two weeks delay
between both driving sessions.
171
Procedure
General remarks
This study was conducted in collaboration with Maastricht University that was responsible for the
driving test. Therefore the use of two instrumented cars of the Federal Highway Research Institute
(BASt) was allowed to Maastricht University. All participants were Germans, medically screened at the
pain outpatients department of the University Hospital of Cologne. It was verified by breath test and by
urine drug screening that subjects did not use alcohol or other psychoactive substances besides those
prescribed for pain treatment before each driving test. All participants completed a computer-based
test of driving related skills there before taking part in the driving test.
At the day of the driving test participants were picked up at Cologne (see table 3 for the timeline),
brought to Maastricht and afterwards back home. All driving tests, as well as familiarization of drivers
with the car, were accompanied by a Dutch licensed driving instructor. This driving instructor met
participants at a rest area near the Dutch boarder. From there participants drove in the instrumented
car to Maastricht University which took about 30 minutes (30 km) and involved driving on a primary
highway. On their way to Maastricht participants received a training driving test aiming to learn test
procedures. At Maastricht University patients were served lunch. Alcohol was administered to
participants after arrival if the under-influence driving (alcohol condition) was on agenda. Driving tests
started and ended at the University building. Depending on traffic conditions it took about 15 minutes
to get from the University building to the starting point of the Road tracking test on the highway.
Alcohol administration
The amount of alcohol needed to obtain the intended BAC of 0.5‰ was calculated by the Watson
formula. This formula takes gender and weight of the individual into account as well as the time span
until the intended BAC should be reached (Watson, Watson, & Batt, 1981). The total amount of
alcohol was split into three parts, mixed with orange juice and administered -60, -40 and -20 minutes
before the driving test. BAC was checked -40 and -20 minutes before the driving test as well as during
the test. In case BAC did not rise as expected additional alcohol was administered. The last dose was
given 20 minutes before the start of the driving test because a 20 minutes drive was necessary to get
to the highway section on which the driving test was done.
Ethical issues
The study was conducted according to the code of ethics on human experimentation established by
the declaration of Helsinki (2008). The German Federal Institute for Drugs and Medical Devices and
the Ethics Committee of the University Hospital of Cologne gave approval to this investigation. It was
also approved by the Ethics Committee of Maastricht University.
172
Table 3: Timeline of the driving test for patients and controls.
Start
End
[hh:mm]
[hh:mm]
08:30
09:00
Patients
Taking of blood, saliva and urine
sample, check of inclusion criteria
Controls
---
09:00
10:30
Transfer to Dutch boarder
11:00
11:45
Meeting with driving instructor, training driving test
11:45
12:15
Lunch break at Maastricht
Alcohol administration at
University
Maastricht University
12:15
12:30
Participant drives to starting point of driving test
12:30
13:30
Road tracking test
13:30
13:45
Return to Maastricht University
13:45
14:00
Break
14:00
14:05
Participant drives to starting point of Car-following test
14:05
14:45
Car-following test
14:45
15:00
Return to Maastricht University
15:00
15:10
Break
15:10
16:45
Transfer to Cologne
Alcohol administration
Actual driving tests
The driving test was conducted in an instrumented car. It comprised two different driving tests: (1)
Road tracking test and (2) Car-following test. The Road tracking test was conducted on a primary
highway (A79) near Maastricht (see figure 1) between the exits Bunde and Heerlen-Centrum. The
distance between both exits was 17 km. In this test participants drove along that section six times (=
three rounds from/to Bunde) which equates a distance of 100 km. The Car-following test was done on
A2 between the exits Gronsveld and Moelingen. The distance between both exits was 7.5 km. For the
Car-following test participants drove along that section four times.
Figure 1: Map of the route Maastricht (Bunde) - Heerlen (A79) used for the Road tracking test.
(1) Road tracking test
For the Road tracking test (O'Hanlon, 1984) subjects drove 100 km in a specially instrumented car on
a primary highway. Their task was to maintain a constant speed (95 km/h) and a steady lateral
position on the right (slower) traffic lane. An optical measurement device mounted at the grill (linescan camera) continuously measured lateral distance between the vehicle and the left road marking.
Drivers were allowed to overtake slower vehicles and were advised to change back to the right traffic
lane after overtaking as soon as possible. Data on lateral position was stored together with data on
173
vehicle movements (e.g. speed, lateral and longitudinal acceleration) and inputs by the driver (e.g.
steering wheel angle, throttle position) on a computer in the car. The technical equipment also
provided video recordings of the drive.
In the off-line editing routine the signal which was digitized at a rate of 30 Hz was down sampled to
10 Hz. To eliminate noise the signal was then filtered by a digital low-pass filter (cut-off frequency:
2 Hz). Afterwards all data segments with signal loss as well as overtaking manoeuvres were
eliminated. Sections where the highway was left and entered again in the opposite direction were also
removed. The remaining data was then used to calculate means and standard deviation of lateral
position for every 5-km-segment of the track (equates three per section). The values of all of these
segments were then averaged to get an overall measure of standard deviation of lateral position
(SDLP) for the whole driving test. SDLP is a measure of weaving (see figure 2). It was used as the
primary outcome measure. In addition average speed and standard deviation of speed were computed
in the same way for the whole track. They were used as control measures. The test duration was 1
hour.
Figure 2: Description of SDLP (Verster, Volkerts, & Verbaten, 2002, S. 263).
(2) Car-following test
The Car-following test (Ramaekers, Muntjewerff, & O'Hanlon, 1995) involved the use of two vehicles.
The preceding vehicle was under an investigator's control. The subject was driving in the following
vehicle. The test began with the two vehicles driving behind each other at 95 km/h. Subjects were told
to follow the leading car at a distance of 50 m (time distance: 2 seconds) and to maintain that
headway constant. During the test, the speed of the leading car was controlled by a modified cruisecontrol system. At the beginning this system was set to maintain a constant speed. The investigator
started sinusoidal speed changes. The duration of one such cycle was 50 seconds: within 25 seconds
the leading car decelerates 10% of the actual driving speed and then accelerates to reach the initial
speed within 25 second again. Such speed change cycles were repeated 12 times at random sections
during the Car-following test. Between theses speed change manoeuvres, the investigator in the
leading car randomly activated the brake lights of his vehicle. The brake lights then light up for 3
seconds whereas the speed of the car remained constant. The subject was told to react to the onset of
brake lights by removing his/her foot from the gas pedal as fast as possible. This procedure was
repeated 12 times throughout the Car-following test.
174
Headway was continuously recorded by a radar distance sensor. This device was mounted in the grill
of the instrumented car (= the following vehicle). The velocity of the leading vehicle and the initiation
times of speed changes and brake lights were transmitted by Wi-Fi to the following instrumented
vehicle and were stored there on a computer along with the velocity of the following vehicle, distance
headway and response time to the onset of brake lights.
To analyze data of the speed changes a power spectral analysis of the speed signals of both cars was
done. Therefore the cycles were put in a direct sequence. The power spectral analysis revealed
phase-delay between the speed cycles of both vehicles (cycle frequency = 0.02 Hz). Phase delay was
converted to an indicator of time-to-speed adaptation (TSA, in seconds). Gain and coherence are
control measures and were also provided by power spectral analysis. Gain is the amplification factor
between both speed cycles. It indicates the amount of overshoot when participants adapt driving
speed to the speed changes of the leading car. Coherence is a measure to control for correspondence
between both speed signals.
Brake reaction time (BRT, in seconds) is as secondary outcome measure. It was calculated of the
delay between the onset of the brake lights of the leading car and release of the throttle by the subject
who was driving the following car. Single values for all events were averaged across the whole driving
test to get a measure of brake reaction time for the whole drive.
Test duration was 25 minutes.
Karolinska sleepiness scale
Subjective ratings of sleepiness were assessed by Karolinska sleepiness scale (Akerstedt & Gillberg,
1990) before and after the Road tracking test and the Car-following test as well as after each section
of the Road tracking test immediately before the highway was left at the exit to change direction.
Scores on the scales were ranging from 1 “extremely alert”to 9 “very sleepy, great effort to keep alert,
fighting against sleep”.
Rating Scale Mental Effort (RSME; Zijlstra, 1993)
With this scale participants have had to indicate the amount of effort they invested in the task by
flagging a 15cm long axis ranging from 0 to 150. Segments of the axis were tagged with verbal
categories indicating different levels of effort. Effort was assessed after the Road tracking test and
after the Car-following test.
Additional self rating scales
Visual analogue scales (10cm long) were used to assess self-ratings of pain intensity, quality of
driving and intensity of alcohol effects. The latter was only done for controls whereas pain intensity
was only assessed in patients. During the Road tracking test quality of driving was also assessed at
the end of each section while approaching the exit.
Collection of body fluids
For quantification of opioid concentrations in body fluids a blood sample (10 mL) was taken of every
patient at the University of Cologne before he/she was brought to Maastricht. Dried blood spots were
prepared as well as serum by centrifugation of half of the whole blood sample. Whole blood samples
175
and serum samples were frozen at -20°C and forwarded to the Heidelberg University where samples
were analysed using solid phase extraction and gas chromatography with mass spectrometric
detection. Dried blood spots were stored in a cool dark place. StatSure saliva sampler was used to
collect oral fluids for the quantitative analysis of analgesics concentrations by GC-MS. Until analysis
was done at Gent University these samples were also stored at -20°C.
Statistical analysis
SPSS 19 for Windows was used for all statistical analysis. The testing procedure consisted of three
steps.
1) Superiority testing by ANOVA for between group comparisons of patients and controls (sober). In
order to find out if differences between these groups are caused by opioid treatment, by the underlying
pain disorder or by age differences, age and pain intensity were used as covariates in the ANOVA.
2) Superiority testing by general linear model (GLM) repeated measures ANOVA with alcohol (two
levels: 0.5‰ vs. sober) as main within subject factor to compare performance of controls driving sober
and under influence of alcohol.
3) In case superiority was proven, equivalence testing based on difference scores was conducted in
addition (patients’SDLP – average SDLP of the sober controls). An alcohol criterion was calculated (=
averaged difference scores of SDLP of controls alcoholized – controls sober). Equivalence testing
assessed whether this alcohol criterion falls within the 95% CI for the difference scores of patients. If
that was the case the difference between groups was considered equivalent to a BAC of 0.5‰ and
therefore relevant for traffic safety. If the 95% CI was below the alcohol criterion value, the difference
between patients and healthy controls was not considered to be relevant.
Results
Between March 2010 and September 2010 20 pain patients and 19 healthy controls completed the
driving test. Because of the great expenses of the driving test only participants fulfilling the inclusion
criteria were included into the driving test.
Alcohol levels
Dräger Alcotest 6510 was used to measure BAC in the course of the driving test. On average the
participants received 47 mL alcohol (96 Vol.) before the Road tracking test. Depending on gender and
weight of the participants this amount differs between individuals (SD = 10.61; min = 32.5 mL; max =
62.9 mL). The intention was to raise drivers’BAC to 0.5‰ before the Road tracking test and to keep it
on that level for the duration of both driving tests. Therefore BAC was checked four times in the course
of the Road tracking test and also at the beginning and at the end of the Car-following test. As can be
seen from table 4, the intended BAC was reached before both driving tests. In case it was below
0.45‰ after the first round of the Road tracking test, additional alcohol was administered during a
break at the roadside. This was necessary for seven (41%) of the drivers. Additional alcohol had to be
administered to all drivers in the break between Road tracking test and Car-following test which was
done at the University building.
176
Table 4: BAC (‰ ) measured during testing (data of controls).
Time of measurement
Alcohol
administration
Road tracking
test
t
Average
BAC
SD BAC
N
[min.]
[‰ ]
st
-60
--
--
17
nd
-40
--
--
17
rd
3 dosed
-20
--
--
17
Start
0
0.49
0.11
17
after 1 round
23
0.49
0.11
17
nd
46
0.48
0.07
17
69
0.39
0.07
17
--
--
17
1 dose
2 dose
st
after 2 round
after
3
rd
round
(end)
Break (alcohol administration)
Car-following
Start
105
0.45
0.07
17
test
End
140
0.35
0.05
17
Toxicology
Toxicological analysis of the concentrations of the active agents in whole blood (B) and plasma (p)
revealed the following concentrations in the different specimen (see table 5).
Table 5: Average substance concentrations (range) in whole blood (B) and in plasma (p); N =
number of samples available.
active agent
B [ng/mL]
p [ng/mL]
N
buprenorphine
0.15 ( – )
0.30
1
fentanyl
0.48 (0.18 – 4.0)
0.61 (0.20 – 4.24)
5
hydromorphone
10.80 (2.31 – 20.17)
9.85 (2.41 – 19.71)
6
morphine
210.56 (97.77 – 215.65)
205.75 (91.78 – 206.70)
3
oxycodone
70.02 (47.17 – 122.55)
54.33 (30.62 – 89.84)
5
Traffic conditions
On-the-road driving tests are exposed to influences of traffic conditions. High traffic density during the
Road tracking test might reduce fatigue of the driver because driving in dense traffic is less
monotonous. The number of overtaking manoeuvres was used as an indicator of traffic density. As
could be seen from figure 3 the drivers had to overtake equally often across all conditions indicating
comparable traffic density.
Speed changes events in the Car-following test could only be initiated by the driver of the leading car
in case no slower vehicles were ahead and no faster cars were approaching. For both events both
cars have to drive at the same speed. The investigator in the leading car had the instruction to initiate
at least 8 speed cycles and at least 8 brake events. Figure 4 indicates that these requirements had
been fulfilled across all three conditions.
177
Figure 3: Average number of overtaking
Figure
maneuvers in the Road tracking test as an
successful speed cycles and brake events in
indicator
the
of
comparability
traffic
between
density
conditions
reveals
(P
=
patients, C- = controls sober, C+ = controls
4:
Mean
Car-following
(± SD)
test
of
over
number
of
conditions
-
indicates comparability (P = patients, C =
controls sober, C+ = controls 0.5‰ ).
0.5‰ ).
Premature termination of driving tests
Only one Road tracking test had to be terminated prematurely. This was done on request of the driver,
a healthy volunteer driving sober. He felt too sleepy to go on driving after 2/3 of the designated driving
time. None of the Road tracking tests had to be terminated prematurely by the driving instructor due to
security concerns, neither of patients nor in the alcohol condition.
All Car-following tests have been completed successfully.
Performance measures
Mean (SD) performances on the primary measure SDLP are shown in figure 5. Table 6 shows the
numbers and the results of superiority testing. Alcohol significantly impaired SDLP (F 1,16 = 18.39, p
.001). SDLP increased on average by 2.43 cm when controls drove under influence of alcohol (0.5‰ ).
Although SDLP has proven to be sensitive to impairments caused by alcohol, no significant difference
between patients and controls was found (F1,35 = 2.01, n.s). Neither age (F1,35 = 2.28, n.s) nor pain
intensity (F1,35 = 1.39, n.s) significantly affected SDLP.
Drivers have been instructed to keep the velocity of 95 km/h. Average speed was used as control
measure. Data analysis showed that drivers followed the instruction across all conditions (see table 6).
Standard deviation of speed did not differ between conditions, too (see table 6). Neither age of the
driver nor ratings of pain intensity affected both speed measures.
178
Figure 5: Mean (± SD) of lateral position
-
(SDLP in cm; P = patients, C = controls
+
sober, C = controls 0.5‰ ).
The results of the Car following test can be found in table 6. No impairing effects of alcohol were found
in the measures of the Car-following test. Therefore the measures have not proven to be sensitive to
the alcohol effect in the study at hand. Similarly no performance differences between patients and
controls were found.
Table 6: Summary of means (SD) and effects for the three conditions (P = patients, C - =
+
controls sober, C = controls 0.5‰ ) in the Road tracking test and the Car-following test.
groups (m (sd))
ANOVA (p
-
+
-
-
)
P
C
C
P vs. C
C vs. C
SDLP (cm)
20.53 (4.29)
17.96 (4.04)
20.74 (3.32)
.166
.001
speed (km/h)
95.24 (1.34)
94.56 (0.94)
94.42 (0.75)
.090
.781
2.36 (0.66)
1.91 (0.69)
2.23 (0.95)
.153
.173
3.17 (1.00)
3.26 (0.79)
3.57 (0.91)
.095
.090
gain
1.19 (0.06)
1.20 (0.15)
1.26 (0.10)
.888
.427
coherence
0.96 (0.03)
0.96 (0.02)
0.95 (0.01)
.241
.465
0.93 (0.29)
0.86 (0.15)
0.95 (0.16)
.320
.097
SD
speed
(km/h)
TSA
(seconds)
BRT
(seconds)
+
Subjective measures
All drivers made self-ratings on several aspects related to their performance in the driving test. Fatigue
and quality of driving were assessed after each section of the track. Unless otherwise stated, the
following results refer to the averaged ratings of all six sections in the Road tracking test and of all four
sections in the Car-following test. Other aspects like the extent of effort needed to perform the tests
successfully were assessed only once after the completion of the Road tracking test as well as of the
Car-following test. In the alcohol condition the degree of impairment caused by alcohol was assessed.
All patients assessed the intensity of pain by rating and informed on their opinion concerning the
sources of impairment.
179
Alcohol impairment
Before the Road tracking test and before the Car-following test all drivers were asked if they feel fit to
drive. 14 of 17 drivers (82%) felt unfit to drive, three were not sure (12%) and only one driver (6%)
rated himself being still fit to drive. Before the Car-following test 15 drivers (88%) felt unfit to drive.
Only two held the opinion that they were still able to drive a car safely (12%). Drivers’ratings on a 10
cm visual analogue scale ranging from zero (no effect of the alcohol at all) to 10 (strongest effect of
the alcohol) showed that the effect of alcohol was clearly noticeable during both parts of the driving
test (see figure 6). Accordingly they felt impaired in both parts of the driving test (see figure 7).
Figure 6: Mean (± SD) self-rated intensity of
Figure 7: Mean (± SD) self-rated impairment
the alcohol effect before and after both parts
by
of the test (0 = no effect at all / 10 = strongest
impairment
effect).
impairment).
alcohol
on
at
performance
all
/
10
=
(0
=
no
strongest
Performance
The rating scales used for assessment of performance showed a slight alcohol induced decline in
performance in the Road tracking test (F1,16 = 6.491, p
following test (F1,16 = 6.683, p
.022; see also table 7) and in the Car-
.020; see also table 8). There was no significant difference between
the ratings of patients and sober controls. Throughout all conditions drivers judged their performance
being good: all averaged ratings were above six on a 10 cm visual analogue scale ranging from zero
(very bad) to 10 (very well). Age and ratings of pain intensity did not affect these subjective ratings of
performance.
Pain intensity slightly increased during the Road tracking test as well as during the Car-following test
(see figure 8). Patients traced back the impairment equally on pain and on pain treatment (figure 9).
Overall, the intensity of pain as well as the extent of impairment caused by pain and by the side effects
of the analgesics seemed to be low.
180
Figure 8: Mean (± SD) self-rated intensity of
Figure 9: Mean (± SD) self-rated impairment
pain before and after Road tracking test and
by pain and by analgesic on performance in
Car-following test (0 = no pain at all / 10 =
the Road tracking test and in the Car-
strongest pain).
following test (0 = no impairment at all / 10 =
strongest impairment).
Sleepiness
Ratings on the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (ranging from 1 “extremely alert”to 9 “very sleepy, great
effort to keep alert, fighting against sleep”) differed significantly between conditions in the Road
tracking test (see table 7). Controls felt slightly sleepier when driving under influence of alcohol (F1,16 =
4.486, p
.050). Patients felt sleepier than controls in this part of the driving test (F1,35 = 5.905, p
.020). However all averaged ratings ranged between three and five across all conditions, indicating
ratings of “neither awake nor sleepy”up to “awake”. The covariates age and pain intensity did not
affect ratings of sleepiness.
Statistical analysis revealed no significant differences in ratings of sleepiness in the Car-following test.
Effort
By means of a 15cm long scale participants indicated the effort they needed to complete the driving
task. As depicted in table 7 more effort was needed to fulfil the requirements of the Road tracking test
under influence of alcohol (F1,16 = 12.690, p
24.586, p
.003). The same applies to the Car-following test (F 1,16 =
.000; see table 8). Ratings of patients and of controls did not differ. All in all low effort was
necessary across all conditions.
Table 7: Means (SD) and results of superiority test of subjective measures taken in the Road
-
+
tracking test (P = patients, C = controls sober, C = controls 0.5‰ ).
group
Performance
Sleepiness
(KSS)
Effort
ANOVA (p
P
C-
C+
(N = 20)
(N = 19)
(N=17)
7.45 (1.28)
7.16 (1.22)
3.19 (1.39)
3.99 (2.94)
)
P vs. C-
C- vs. C+
6.48 (1.56)
.345
.022
4.14 (1.08)
4.70 (1.69)
.020
.050
4.09 (2.19)
5.52 (2.15)
.214
.003
181
Table 8: Means (SD) and results of superiority test of subjective measures taken in the Carfollowing test (P = patients, C- = controls sober, C+ = controls 0.5‰ ).
group
Performance
Sleepiness
(KSS)
Effort
ANOVA (p
P
C-
C+
(N = 20)
(N = 19)
(N=17)
7.66 (1.27)
7.25 (1.22)
3.58 (1.79)
3.26 (2.49)
)
P vs. C-
C- vs. C+
6.57 (1.48)
.299
.020
3.51 (1.21)
4.04 (1.33)
.064
.192
2.64 (1.33)
4.31 (1.56)
.089
.000
Correlations to primary outcome measure SDLP
Correlation analysis revealed a positive relation between age and SDLP (see table 9). According to
the data the older driver is the more he is weaving. This indicates a performance decrement with age.
Neither gender nor driving experience or frequency of driving are significantly correlated to this
primary outcome measure of the Road tracking test.
Patients included into the present study were treated with different opioid analgesics at different dose
levels. These analgesics differ in strength of the analgesic effect as well as in strength of the expected
side effects. One way to compare different analgesics is to calculate the amount of morphine
necessary to get the same analgesia. These morphine equivalence dosages are not significantly
correlated to SDLP (see table 10). Correlation analysis also showed that performance of patients was
not related to the duration of pain and also not related to ratings of pain intensity.
Table 9: Correlation (Pearson) between SDLP and sample characteristics (data of patients and
controls driving sober).
r
p (two-sided) N
Characteristic
(Pearson)
Age
0.401
.011
39
Gender
0.234
.152
39
-0.215
.188
39
0.232
.155
39
Driving
experience
(km last 12 month)
Frequency
driving
of
182
Table 10: Correlation (Pearson) between SDLP, duration of pain, morphine equivalence dosage
and pain intensity (only data of patients).
r
Characteristic
Duration
of
(Pearson)
pain
(years)
morphine
equivalence dosage
pain intensity before
driving (cm)
p (two-sided) N
-0.042
.861
20
0.119
.618
20
-0.360
.119
20
Discussion
In line with published studies (e.g. Verster et al., 2008) the Road tracking test has proven to be
sensitive to the impairments caused by BAC of 0.5‰ . Alcohol deteriorated road-tracking performance
which was indicated by a significant increase in SDLP relative to sober driving. In contrast roadtracking performance of pain patients under long term treatment with opioid analgesics of WHO level 3
was not impaired. There was no statistical significant difference in SDLP between patients and an
age-independent group of healthy controls. Moreover none of the Road tracking tests of patients had
to be terminated prematurely although drowsiness and fatigue are well known side effects of opioid
treatment. In a recently published study the Road tracking test was used to study the effects of MDMA
on actual driving performance before and after sleep deprivation (Ramaekers, 2011). Approximately
20% of driving tests had to be terminated prematurely when participants drove after a night of sleep
deprivation. These subjects felt too sleepy to fulfil the monotonous driving task.
To date only one study is available in which driving performance of pain patients was assessed by the
Road tracking test (Veldhuijzen et al., 2006). Other than in the present study only some of the patients
had been treated with analgesics. None of them was treated with opioids but with paracetamol and/or
NSAIDs. Both agents are known to have only minor side effects on cognitive tasks (Bradley &
Nicholson, 1987; Wysenbeek, Klein, Nakar, & Mane, 1988) whereas opioid analgesics are associated
with cognitive side effects.
Overall, the mean SDLP of patients involved in the study of Veldhuijzen et al. (2006) was higher
compared to that of healthy controls. Hence the results indicated worse highway driving performance
of pain patients. The comparison of the absolute values from both studies showed, that SDLP was
higher in both groups in the study of Veldhuijzen et al. (2006) than in the present study (25.2 cm (SD =
4.6) vs. 20.7 cm (SD = 3.4), study at hand: 20.53 cm (SD = 4.29) vs. 17.96 cm (SD = 4.04)). The
deviations of absolute values in both studies might be traced back to the different characteristics of the
instrumented vehicles as well as to the different technical equipment used for the assessment of
lateral position. The study of Veldhuijzen et al. (2006) shows that the difference in SDLP of pain
patients and matched healthy controls equates the increase in SDLP caused by 0.8‰ alcohol in
healthy drivers. The study at hand has found no such difference which suggests that effective pain
treatment my improve fitness to drive. Pain itself is associated with fatigue (Fishbain et al., 2003)
183
which might cause an impairment as long as pain remains untreated. However, while drawing
conclusions from the actual study about road tracking performance of pain patients under stable
medication, one has to consider two aspects. First of all, it was very hard to find patients who were
willing to take part in the driving test. One can assume that only those who had no reasonable doubts
about their fitness to drive agreed to undergo the driving test. In addition, the standard deviation of
SDLP within the group of patients is rather high. This indicates performance differences between
individuals.
In the Car-following test the time needed to adapt driving speed to speed changes of a leading car
was measured. In addition, drivers had to react to the brake lights flash of the leading car by releasing
the accelerator. Although the BAC of 0.5‰ was reached before the Car-following test and drivers felt
impaired by the alcohol, both measures of reaction time showed no difference between sober driving
and driving under influence of alcohol. Therefore in the actual study those measures have not proven
to be sensitive to alcohol induced impairments. There was also no difference evident between patients
and healthy controls. Accordingly, the ability of pain patients to react was not impaired.
Several studies investigated driving ability of chronic pain patients receiving opioid analgesics by
measuring driving related skills by the aid of computer-based test systems (Dagtekin et al., 2007;
Gaertner et al., 2006; Sabatowski et al., 2003). Compared to healthy controls with BAC of 0.5‰ no
impairment was found in those studies. Therefore the results of computer-based tests of driving
related skills are in line with the results of this on-the-road test.
Future studies will need to examine a larger sample of chronic pain patients in order to find out if the
results could be generalized. In the present study patients treated with different analgesics at different
dosages were involved. Due to the low sample size no conclusions on impairments caused by a
certain active agent at a certain dosage could have been drawn. Therefore future studies must
examine a sample that is more comparable related to the psychoactive agents used for pain
treatment. Since age and increase in SDLP are positively correlated, more information is needed on
the relation between age and performance in the Road tracking test.
One major problem while conducting this study was to find a sufficient number of patients who were
willing to participate in the driving test. The obvious question is why so many patients refused to
participate. More information on their performance is needed. Moreover both the Road tracking test
and the Car-following test comprise tasks drivers have to cope with while driving on highways.
Therefore the influence of pain and pain treatment on skills drivers need to deal with tasks typical for
city traffic has to be subject of future studies.
All in all the study at hand suggests that patients with chronic pain on stable doses of opioid
analgesics are not necessarily unfit to drive. Nevertheless it is very important to take into consideration
that individual differences in cognitive and motor performance might exist.
184
References
Akerstedt, T. & Gillberg, M. (1990). Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. 27.
Int.J.Neurosci., 52, 29-37.
Bradley, C. M. & Nicholson, A. N. (1987). Studies on performance with aspirin and paracetamol and
with the centrally acting analgesics meptazinol and pentazocine. Eur.J.Clin.Pharmacol., 32, 135-139.
Brookhuis, K., de Waard, D., & Mulder, B. (1994). Measuring driving performance by car-following in
traffic. Ergonomics, 37, 427-434.
Dagtekin, O., Gerbershagen, H. J., Wagner, W., Petzke, F., Radbruch, L., & Sabatowski, R. (2007).
Assessing cognitive and psychomotor performance under long-term treatment with transdermal
buprenorphine in chronic noncancer pain patients. Anesth.Analg., 105, 1442-1448.
Dellemijn, P. L. I., van Duijn, H., & Vanneste, J. A. L. (1998). Prolonged treatment with transdermal
fentanyl in neuropathic pain. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management, 16, 220-229.
Dick, B. D. & Rashiq, S. (2007). Disruption of attention and working memory traces in individuals with
chronic pain. Anesth.Analg., 104, 1223-9, tables.
Drummer, O. H., Gerostamoulos, J., Batziris, H., Chu, M., Caplehorn, J., Robertson, M. D. et al.
(2004). The involvement of drugs in drivers of motor vehicles killed in Australian road traffic crashes.
Accid.Anal.Prev., 36, 239-248.
Eccleston, C., Crombez, G., Aldrich, S., & Stannard, C. (1997). Attention and somatic awareness in
chronic pain. Pain, 72, 209-215.
Fishbain, D. A., Cole, B., Cutler, R. B., Lewis, J., Rosomoff, H. L., & Fosomoff, R. S. (2003). Is pain
fatiguing? A structured evidence-based review. Pain Med., 4, 51-62.
Fishbain, D. A., Cutler, R. B., Rosomoff, H. L., & Rosomoff, R. S. (2002). Can patients taking opioids
drive safely? A structured evidence-based review. J.Pain Palliat.Care Pharmacother., 16, 9-28.
Fishbain, D. A., Cutler, R. B., Rosomoff, H. L., & Rosomoff, R. S. (2003). Are opioiddependent/tolerant patients impaired in driving-related skills? A structured evidence-based review.
J.Pain Symptom Manage., 25, 559-577.
Gaertner, J., Elsner, F., Radbruch, L., Kolibay, F., Theisohn, M., Berghaus, G. et al. (2008). Influence
of changes to daily dose of opioids on aspects of cognitive and psychomotor performance involved in
driving. Schmerz., 22, 433-441.
Gaertner, J., Radbruch, L., Giesecke, T., Gerbershagen, H., Petzke, F., Ostgathe, C. et al. (2006).
Assessing cognition and psychomotor function under long-term treatment with controlled release
oxycodone in non-cancer pain patients. Acta Anaesthesiol.Scand., 50, 664-672.
Grisart, J. (2009). [Cognitive effects of opioids]. Rev.Med.Suisse, 5, 1356-3.
Grisart, J. M. & Plaghki, L. H. (1999). Impaired selective attention in chronic pain patients. Eur.J.Pain,
3, 325-333.
Jamison, R. N., Schein, J. R., Vallow, S., Ascher, S., Vorsanger, G. J., & Katz, N. P. (2003).
Neuropsychological effects of long-term opioid use in chronic pain patients. J.Pain Symptom.Manage.,
26, 913-921.
Kalso, E., Edwards, J. E., Moore, R. A., & McQuay, H. J. (2004). Opioids in chronic non-cancer pain:
systematic review of efficacy and safety. Pain, 112, 372-380.
Kress, H. G. & Kraft, B. (2005). Opioid medication and driving ability. Eur.J.Pain, 9, 141-144.
185
Lagarde, E., Chastang, J. F., Lafont, S., Coeuret-Pellicer, M., & Chiron, M. (2005). Pain and pain
treatment were associated with traffic accident involvement in a cohort of middle-aged workers.
Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 58, 524-531.
Lenné, M., Dietze, P., Rumbold, G., Redman, J. R., & Triggs, T. J. (2000). Opioid dependence and
driving ability: a review in the context of proposed legislative change in Victoria
8. Drug and Alcohol Review, 19, 427-439.
McCracken, L. M. & Iverson, G. L. (2001). Predicting Complaints of Impaired Cognitive Functioning in
Patients with Chronic Pain. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management, 21, 392-396.
McQuay, H. (1999). Opioids in pain management. Lancet, 353, 2229-2232.
Mersky, H. & Bogduk, N. (1994). Classification of chronic pain: Descriptions of chronic pain
syndromes and definitions of pain terms. (2 ed.) Seattle: IASP Press.
Moore, R. A. & McQuay, H. J. (2005). Prevalence of opioid adverse events in chronic non-malignant
pain: systematic review of randomised trials of oral opioids. Arthritis Res.Ther., 7, 1046-1051.
Movig, K. L., Mathijssen, M. P., Nagel, P. H., van Egmond, T., DE GIER, J. J., Leufkens, H. G. et al.
(2004). Psychoactive substance use and the risk of motor vehicle accidents. Accid.Anal.Prev., 36,
631-636.
Nickel, R. & Raspe, H. H. (2001). Chronischer Schmerz: Epidemiologie und Inanspruchnahme.
Nervenarzt, 72, 897-906.
O'Hanlon, J. F. (1984). Driving performance under the influence of drugs: Rationale for, and
application of, a new test. Br J Clin Pharmacol, 18 (Suppl. 1), 121-129.
Portenoy, R. K. (2000). Current pharmacotherapy of chronic pain. J.Pain Symptom.Manage., 19, 1620.
Ramaekers, J. G. (2011). Effects of stimulant drugs on actual and simulated driving (Rep. No. 1.2.1
(under review)). Maastricht: Maastricht University.
Ramaekers, J. G., Muntjewerff, N. D., & O'Hanlon, J. F. (1995). A comparative study of acute and
subchronic effects of dothiepin, fluoxetine and placebo on psychomotor and actual driving
performance. Br J Clin Pharmacol., 39, 397-404.
Sabatowski, R., Kaiser, U., & Gossrau, G. (2010). Chronische Schmerzen: Fahrsicherheitsrelevante
Aspekte beim Einsatz von Opioiden [Opioids in the management of chronic pain and driving ability].
Anasthesiol.Intensivmed.Notfallmed.Schmerzther., 45, 384-385.
Sabatowski, R., Schwalen, S., Rettig, K., Herberg, K. W., Kasper, S. M., & Radbruch, L. (2003).
Driving ability under long-term treatment with transdermal fentanyl. J Pain Symptom Manage., 25, 3847.
Schmidt, H., Lowenstein, O., Hesselbarth, S., Klinge, V., & Michels, H. (2010). [S3 guidelines LONTS
(Long-term administration of opioids for non-tumor pain)]. Schmerz., 24, 71-72.
Sjogren, P., Olsen, A. K., Thomsen, A. B., & Dalberg, J. (2000). Neuropsychological performance in
cancer patients: the role of oral opioids, pain and performance status. Pain, 86, 237-245.
Strumpf, M., Willweber-Strumpf, A., Herberg, K. W., & Zenz, M. (2005). Sicherheitsrelevante
Leistungsfähigkeit von Schmerzpatienten unter Langzeitopioidtherapie. Der Schmerz, 19, 426-433.
Tassain, V., Attal, N., Fletcher, D., Brasseur, L., Degieux, P., Chauvin, M. et al. (2003). Long term
effects of oral sustained release morphine on neuropsychological performance in patients with chronic
non-cancer pain. Pain, 104, 389-400.
186
Vainio, A., Ollila, J., Matikainen, E., Rosenberg, P., & Kalso, E. (1995). Driving ability in cancer
patients receiving long-term morphine analgesia. The Lancet, 346, 667-670.
Veldhuijzen, D. S., Karscha, A. M., & van Wijck, A. J. M. (2009). Drugs, driving and traffic safety in
acute and chronic pain. In J.C.Verster, S. R. Pandi-Perumal, J. G. Ramaekers, & J. J. DE GIER
(Eds.), Drugs, driving and traffic safety (pp. 355-370). Basel: Birkhäuser.
Veldhuijzen, D. S., van Wijck, A. J., Wille, F., Verster, J. C., Kenemans, J. L., Kalkman, C. J. et al.
(2006). Effect of chronic nonmalignant pain on highway driving performance. Pain, 122, 28-35.
Verster, J. & Ramaekers, J. G. (2008). The on-the-road driving test. In J.C.Verster, S. R. PandiPerumal, J. G. Ramaekers, & J. J. DE GIER (Eds.), Drugs, driving and traffic safety (pp. 83-90). Basel:
Birkhäuser.
Verster, J. C., Volkerts, E. R., & Verbaten, M. N. (2002). Effects of Alprazolam on driving ability,
memory functioning and psychomotor performance: A randomized, placebo-controlled study.
Neuropsychopharmacology, 27, 260-269.
Watson, P. E., Watson, I. D., & Batt, R. D. (1981). Prediction of blood alcohol concentrations in human
subjects. Updating the Widmark Equation. J.Stud.Alcohol, 42, 547-556.
Wysenbeek, A. J., Klein, Z., Nakar, S., & Mane, R. (1988). Assessment of cognitive function in elderly
patients treated with naproxen. A prospective study. Clin.Exp.Rheumatol., 6, 399-400.
Zacny, J. P. (1995). A review of the effects of opioids on psychomotor and cognitive functioning in
humans. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 3, 432-466.
Zijlstra, F. R. H. (1993). Efficiency in work behavior. A design approach for modern tools. PhD thesis,
Delft University of Technology, Delft, The Netherlands.
187
Chapter 9: Effects of opioid analgesics on
driving related skills
Markus Schumacher*, Anja Knoche*, Frank Petzke‡, Ricarda Jantos†,
* Federal Highway Research Institute (BASt)
Section U3 / Traffic Psychology, Traffic Medicine
Bruederstraße 53
D-51427 Bergisch Gladbach, Germany
‡ Georg-August-Universität
Schmerz-Tagesklinik und -Ambulanz
Zentrum Anästhesiologie, Rettungs- und Intensivmedizin
Robert-Koch-Str. 40
37075 Göttingen, Germany
† Universität Heidelberg
Institut für Rechts- und Verkehrsmedizin
Voß-Straße 2
D- 69115 Heidelberg, Germany
188
Abstract
Nowadays morphine-like drugs, called opioids (e.g. fentanyl, oxycodone, buprenorphine, and
hydromorphone) were widely used to treat chronic non-cancer pain conditions. Some studies already
have demonstrated that long term treatment with opioids for non-cancer pain does not impair driving
related skills. Whereas today many pain patients are treated with drug combinations, these studies
have focused on specific opioids. Twenty-six patients suffering from chronic non-cancer pain on long
term treatment with opioid analgesics (fentanyl, buprenorphine, oxycodon, hydromorphone or
morphine) aged between 35 and 68 years participated in this study. Their performance in a computerbased test of driving related skills (Vienna Test System) was compared to the performance of an age
independent sample of healthy controls. Their mean (sd) age was 43 (10.68) years. Performance of
21 controls with blood alcohol concentration (BAC) of 0.5‰ was used as reference for the impairment.
The test battery assessed stress tolerance, visual orientation ability, concentration, attention and
reaction speed. These areas of performance are considered to be relevant for fitness to drive
according to the German Driving Licensing act (FeV). A combination score was calculated of the set of
tests as primary outcome measure. Findings Patients performed worse than controls with respect to
this primary outcome measure. No alcohol induced impairment could have been demonstrated.
Chronic pain patients under long term treatment with opioids show impairments in driving related
skills. Since there was no impact of alcohol on performance, no conclusion on the size of the
impairment can be drawn.
Keywords
Chronic non-cancer pain, opioids, driving ability, psychomotor performance, Vienna test system
189
Introduction
According to a recent review of publications the prevalence of chronic pain in the general population
ranges from 10% to over 40% (Nickel & Raspe, 2001). Pain is classified being chronic when it persists
for three month or longer (Mersky & Bogduk, 1994). The pharmacological management of chronic pain
has changed over the last decade. Based on an optimistic assessment of the beneficial effects of
morphine-like drugs, called opioids (e.g. fentanyl, oxycodone, buprenorphine, and hydromorphone )
on pain intensity and functional capabilities, various opioids were widely used to treat chronic noncancer pain conditions (Portenoy, 2000). More recently it became evident that the uncritical use of
opioids may also cause considerable problems (e.g. dose escalation due to lack of relevant effect,
development of tolerance with loss of efficacy). Therefore Guidelines like LONTS (Long-Term
Treatment with Opioids for Non-Cancer Pain by the German Association for the Study of Pain
(Schmidt, Lowenstein, Hesselbarth, Klinge, & Michels, 2010) have formulated a more cautious
approach to opioid therapy in patients with chronic non-cancer pain.
The availability and clinical evidence for efficacy of better tolerated neuromodulatory drugs like
gabapentin and pregabalin, as well as both traditional and newer antidepressant agents like
amitriptyline and duloxetine have broadened the pharmacological treatment spectrum. These agents
are specifically effective in conditions where neuropathic or mixed pain aetiology was found as well as
in musculoskeletal pain syndromes where a central hypersensitivity may be relevant. These
developments have led to combination therapy in many patients with chronic pain. Although there is
little scientific evidence to support this approach, it is widely established and accepted in clinical
practice. Hence today monotherapy with opioids is the exception rather than the rule.
It is well documented that cognitive functions like attention are impaired in chronic pain patients (e.g.
Dick & Rashiq, 2007; Eccleston, Crombez, Aldrich, & Stannard, 1997; Grisart & Plaghki, 1999)
whereas relief of pain might diminish these impairments (Dick et al., 2007; Eccleston et al., 1997;
Grisart, 2009). Deficits in attention may lead to reduced fitness to drive. Lagarde, Chastang, Lafont,
Coeuret-Pellicer, & Chiron (2005) showed that pain alone or in combination with pain treatment was
associated with an increased risk of being involved in traffic accidents. However it was not possible in
this study to distinguish between the negative effects of pain itself and those of pain treatment.
Opioids are often prescribed in the treatment of chronic pain (McQuay, 1999) but are associated with
side effects in the gastrointestinal system as well as in the central nervous system. Drowsiness and
sleepiness are frequently reported side effects (Kalso, Edwards, Moore, & McQuay, 2004; Moore &
McQuay, 2005). Several studies showed that driving under the influence of sedating medications or
drugs, especially of alcohol, leads to an increase in accident risk (Drummer et al., 2004; Movig et al.,
2004). The prevalence of drivers tested positive for opioids in a recent study ranged from 1% to 5%.
Overall the association between opioids and culpability was very low (Drummer et al., 2004). Reviews
on studies published earlier supported this notion that the prevalence of opioid use in traffic is very low
and is not associated with motor vehicle accidents or motor vehicle fatalities (Fishbain, Cutler,
Rosomoff, & Rosomoff, 2002; Fishbain, Cutler, Rosomoff, & Rosomoff, 2003; Lenné, Dietze,
Rumbold, Redman, & Triggs, 2000). Beside these epidemiological studies a limited number of
experimental studies have been published aiming at measuring driving ability of chronic pain patients
190
under medical treatment (see Kress & Kraft, 2005 for a review and Veldhuijzen, Karscha, & van Wijck,
2009 for an overview).
No performance decrement in skills related to driving was found in cancer pain patients treated with
stable doses of oral morphine (Vainio, Ollila, Matikainen, Rosenberg, & Kalso, 1995). Sabatowski et
al. (2003) used the Vienna Test System (VTS), a computerized test battery that is also used for the
assessment of fitness to drive of traffic offenders in Germany. They assessed driving ability of noncancer pain patients under long-term treatment with transdermal fentanyl. Performance of patients
was compared to the performance of matched healthy controls tested with blood alcohol concentration
(BAC) of 0.5‰ . The results showed that performance of patients was non-inferior to that of controls.
The results of two other studies which were done with the same experimental design and the same
test battery but with different opioids were similar. No overall impairment was found for the long-term
use of transdermal buprenorphine (Dagtekin et al., 2007) and also no impairment was found for the
long-term treatment with controlled release oxycodone (Gaertner et al., 2006). The authors conclude
from their studies that there is no general impairment due to long-term treatment with opioids. This
conclusion is in line with the conclusion of Strumpf, Willweber-Strumpf, Herberg, & Zenz (2005).
Gaertner et al. (2008) examined the effects of opioid drug dose changes on cognitive and
psychomotor performances relevant for driving. Seven days after dose change, no deterioration was
observed. Tassain et al. (2003) analyzed the performance of pain patients before the initiation of
opioid therapy with slow release morphine. This initial performance was compared to the performance
after 3, 6 and 12 month under stable medication. Stable opioid medication did not lead to a
deterioration of cognitive functioning during long-term application.
Patients with chronic pain often report cognitive complaints (McCracken & Iverson, 2001) and it is well
known that there exists an association between fatigue and pain (Fishbain et al., 2003). The
performance impairments might be caused by side effects of opioids. These side effects decline over
time (Dellemijn, van Duijn, & Vanneste, 1998; Tassain et al., 2003) or test scores on cognitive ability
and psychomotor function are even increasing over time (Jamison et al., 2003). In addition, those who
use opioids occasionally or habitually (treatment of chronic pain or opioid abusers) are much less
likely show signs of impairment (Zacny, 1995).
Moreover it was proposed that the impairing effect of pain is more profound than the impairment
caused by side effects of the opioids (Sjogren, Olsen, Thomsen, & Dalberg, 2000). This is further
supported by a study by Veldhuijzen et al. (2006).They found pain itself to significantly affect driving
performance. In their study an on-the-road driving test was used to measure fitness to drive. In
summary Sabatowski, Kaiser, & Gossrau (2010) state that to this date no decision can be drawn
explicitly from the available data whether slow release opioid formulations lead to impaired fitness to
drive or not.
Methods
In order to better reflect the collective of pain patients treated with combination therapy, in this study
chronic pain patients were enclosed treated with a variable mixture of other compounds in addition to
a level 3 opioid. For this reason the sample reflects the current standard of pharmacological
management of chronic pain. It is more helpful to study this combination therapy, with the opioid being
the most potent drug affecting the ability to drive, to gain a realistic and clinically relevant insight into
191
driving ability of patients suffering from chronic pain, than focusing on the specific effects of a specific
opioid. Performance on driving related skills of patients receiving opioid analgesic of WHO level 3 for
chronic non-cancer pain treatment is compared to performance of healthy controls. Performance of
the control group with BAC of 0.5‰ was used as a reference. Driving ability was measured by a
computerized test of driving related skills that fulfils the legal regulations in Germany for the
assessment of fitness to drive. Whereas patients had to do this computer-based test once, controls
had to do it twice: once sober and once under the influence of alcohol. Due to organisational
restrictions it was not possible to test half of the controls first sober and the others first under influence
of alcohol. In order to compensate for potential learning effects it was ensured that there was a time
span of at least two weeks between both test sessions.
Subjects
All subjects gave written informed consent, possessed a valid driver’
s license for passenger cars,
travelled at least 2000 km by car in the last preceding 12 month and were driving on a regular basis
(at least once a week). This study aimed at comparing the performance of pain patients to a
representative cross-section of the driving population. Therefore controls were matched to four age
groups (20-30, 30-40, 40-50, 50-65). The prevalence of chronic pain increases with age (Nickel et al.,
2001). Overall women are more often affected by most of the chronic pain syndroms but men are
more likely to receive high-dose opioids (Mailis-Gagnon et al., 2011). Accordingly the average age
differs between both groups as can be seen in table 2. All subjects were given financial compensation
for participating in this study to account for loss of earnings and travel costs.
Patients
No study related changes in prescribed medication were made. 15 male and 11 female patients (see
table 2) of the pain outpatient department of the University Hospital of Cologne aged between 35 and
68 (m= 54, sd = 8.28) suffering from non-cancer pain responsive to opioids were enrolled into the
study. They had been treated for at least four weeks with either transdermal fentanyl ( 12 g/h),
transdermal buprenorphine (10 g/h), slow release oxycodone ( 10 mg/day), slow-release oxycodone
combined with naloxone ( 10 mg/day), slow release hydromorphone ( 4 mg /day) or slow release
morphine ( 20 mg/day). Table 2 shows the actual frequencies of the analgesics and the duration of
opioid treatment. The dosage of the opioids remained unchanged for at least 14 days prior to the
assessment. To meet the inclusion criteria co-medication with NSAIDs, anticonvulsants and
antidepressants had to be on a stable dose during the past 14 days. In addition the daily intake of
antidepressants and anticonvulsant had to be below a given dose limit (see table 1) and patients had
to restrain from intake of immediate release opioids, benzodiazepines, barbiturates and alcohol at
least two days prior to the assessment.
192
Table 1: Dose limits for co-medication.
antidepressants
anticonvulsants
amitryptilin
carbamazepin
doxepin
75mg
75mg
oxcarbazepin
1200mg,
1800mg,
imipramin
75mg
gabapentin
2400mg,
trazodon
100mg
pregabalin
600mg
sertralin
50mg
fluoxetin
20mg
fluvoxamin 75mg
duloxetin
120mg
venlafaxin
225mg
citalopram 10mg
Controls
13 male and 8 female healthy controls participated in this study. Their mean (sd) age was 43 (10.68).
Participants were selected from a pool of volunteers that previously had given consent to participate in
studies conducted by BASt. Exclusion criteria were a history of alcohol addiction or abuse, intake of
medications prohibiting the consumption of alcohol or medicinal or recreational use of psychoactive
substances with known impairing effect on driving ability. Women who were pregnant or breast
feeding or women of child-bearing potential who were not using a highly effective contraception
method with a pearl-index
1 were also excluded due to the alcohol administration during the course
of the study.
12 male (92%) and 6 female (75%) participants completed the test in both conditions: sober as well as
under the influence of alcohol. The total number of controls of which data of both conditions is
available was 18 (86%).
Study design
This study was conducted according to a mixed within-between design. Treatment consisted of
alcohol administration before the test vs. no alcohol administration and was only done in the control
group. The amount of alcohol necessary to obtain the intended BAC of 0.5‰ was calculated by the
Watson formula. This formula takes into account gender and weight of the individual as well as the
time span until the intended BAC should be reached (Watson, Watson, & Batt, 1981). Alcohol was
mixed with orange juice before administration.
Pain patients were enclosed with their existing medication and no study related change in medication
was done. Hence patients did the computer-based test once, whereas controls did the test twice. The
first test was always done sober, the second test at least two weeks later under the influence of
alcohol.
193
Table 2: Sample characteristics.
characterisitic
Patients
Controls
(N = 26)
(N = 21)
age (years)
mean (sd)
54.00 (8.28)
43.10 (10.68)
gender
male N (%)
15 (58)
13 (62)
female N (%)
11 (42)
8 (38)
median km (range)
7500
12000
(1200 - 25000)
(2000 - 50000)
driving experience
(km/last 12 month):
driving
license
median (range)
34 (17 - 50)
25 (5 - 40)
pain
median (range)
12.50 (5 - 40)
-
opioid
median (range)
27.00 (2-192)
musculoskeletal
20
-
visceral pain
3
-
other
3
-
opioid analgesic N
fentanyl
5 (25µg/h; 25µg/h - 75µg/h)
(median; dose range)
buprenorphine
4 (43.75µg/h; 35µg/h - 88µg/h)
oxycodone
5 (140mg/d; 30mg/d – 150mg/d)
hydromorphone
8 (20mg/d; 8mg/d – 56mg/d)
morphinesulfate
4 (130mg/d; 60mg/d – 200mg/)
(years)
duration
of
(years)
duration
of
treatment (month)
diagnosis (N)
Procedure
The study was conducted according to the code of ethics on human experimentation established by
the declaration of Helsinki (2008). The German Federal Institute for Drugs and Medical Devices
(BfARM) and the ethics committee of the University Hospital of Cologne gave approval for this
investigation.
After the participants had been informed about the study and had given their written informed consent,
their demographic data, data on their driving experience and medical history was recorded and
fulfilment of the inclusion and exclusion criteria was checked. Prior to the computer based testing
blood, saliva, and urine samples were taken from all patients.
Urine of all participants was screened for psychoactive substances to control for intake of medications
not prescribed or recreational drug use. Also an alcohol breath test was done in all subjects. Pain
intensity was rated by patients before and after the testing.
For the controls in the alcohol condition the total amount of alcohol was split into two parts, mixed with
orange juice and administered 30 min and 15 min before the test. In case the actual BAC level was
too low at the testing time, additional alcohol was administered and BAC level was checked again
after an adequate time span.
Completion of all eight designated tests of the test battery took 90 minutes. In order to prevent
excessive exhaustion patients were allowed a 10 minutes break after they had completed the first five
194
tests (after 60 minutes approximately). Testing of healthy volunteers in the alcohol condition was also
interrupted at the same time. During this break BAC was checked and additional alcohol was
administered in case BAC was below 0.45‰ .
Vienna Test System (VTS)
Brief description
After withdrawal of a driver’
s license (e.g. due to drunk driving) in Germany an expert assessment of
the fitness to drive is needed before a driver’
s license can be reinstated. According to the German
Driving Licensing Act (FeV) five skills considered to be relevant for safe driving must be assessed by
using accredited computer-based testing devices. These skills are: stress tolerance, visual orientation
ability, concentration, attention and reaction speed (BASt, 2009; Schuhfried GmbH, 2009). The Vienna
Test System used in this study is such a device accredited to be eligible within the scope of the
assessment of fitness to drive. Five different performance tests are recommended by the
manufacturer to assess the skills mentioned above. Table 3 gives an overview of the respective skills
and the tests appropriate to assess them (Schubert, Schneider, Eisenmenger, & Stephan, 2005;
Schuhfried GmbH, 2009). In this study three additional tests were used to obtain a deeper insight into
potential performance decrements caused by sedating psychoactive substances. 2HAND test was
used to measure potential impairments in eye-hand and hand-hand coordination. VIGIL test was used
to measure potential attention decrements under monotonous tasks conditions. According to the latest
revision of FeV VIGIL can be done additionally if participants are prone to daytime sleepiness. As
sleepiness is one of the common side effects of opioids this test was included. WRBTV was done to
quantify the level of risk accepted in traffic situations.
Table 3: Overview of skills required by German Driving Licensing Act (FeV) and tests of the test
battery covering these skills (Schubert et al., 2005; Schuhfried GmbH, 2009). If a test assesses
several skills, the skills the tests focuses on are highlighted (Schuhfried GmbH, 2009). Not mandatory
tests according to FeV and the guidelines of the manufacturer are identified (last column); the tests
are sorted in the testing sequence; the 10 minute break was placed before the LVT.
stress
visual
tolerance
orientati
concentration
attention
reaction
mandat
speed
ory?
X
yes
on
DT
X
X
COG
TAVTMB
X
X
yes
X
X
X
yes
2HAND
-
-
-
-
-
no
VIGIL
-
-
-
-
-
no
LVT
X
X
RT
WRBTV
-
-
-
-
yes
X
yes
-
no
195
The Vienna Test System consists of a central processing unit, a display, a response panel and
response pedals (see figure 1). All tests were presented in a standardized way and always in the
same sequence by the computer system. Each test started with an explanation on the monitor. Data
collection began after a training phase had been completed successfully. In case the training goal was
not reached, the investigator was informed by the system and gave additional explanations. Testing
was only performed, if the training phase was completed successfully. It took about 90 minutes to
complete all tests.
Figure 1: Vienna Test System.
In the following section the different tests of the test battery are briefly described. All pictures are taken
from the website of the manufacturer (www.schuhfried.com). A more detailed description of all tests
can also be found there. The duration of the tests varies depending on the test form (some of them
adapt to the working speed of the participant) and the time needed for instruction and training phase.
Determination Test (DT)
Visual and acoustic stimuli are presented to the
participant. The participants’task is to press the
corresponding buttons on the response panel or
the pedals.
In the present study test form S2 was used.
Since the test is adaptive to the working speed of
the participant overall duration varied between
10 to 15 minutes.
Cognitrone (COG)
It is the respondents’task to compare abstract
figures and to indicate whether there are
corresponding figures or not by pressing a
button.
Test form S11 was used. The working speed
depends on performance of the participant.
Therefore duration was 5 to 8 minutes.
196
Adaptive Tachistoscopic Traffic Perception Test (TAVTMB)
In this test pictures of different traffic situations
are presented to the participants for very short
time periods. The participants then indicated
what they had seen on those pictures by
choosing the correct answers out of five options
presented to them.
Duration was 8 to 10 minutes.
Two-Hand Coordination Test (2HAND)
The participant has to move the red dot along the
paths by turning buttons with his left and right
hand. Turning one button moves the dot in
vertical direction whereas turning the other
button moves the button in horizontal direction.
Test form S3 was used. Duration was 5 to 10
minutes.
Vigilance Test (VIGIL)
In this monotonous monitoring task a white dot
moves along a circle of open dots. Sometimes
the white dot skips one dot of the circle. The
participant has to indicate these jumps by
pressing a button.
Test form S1 was used. Duration was 25
minutes.
Visual Pursuit Test (LVT)
In this test an array of lines is presented. The
participant has to indicate where the tagged line
ends by pressing the corresponding number on
the response panel as fast as possible.
Test form S3 was used. Test duration was 3 to 5
minutes.
197
Reaction Test (RT)
This test involves the presentation of visual and
acoustic stimuli. The respondent has to press a
button only in case the visual target stimuli and
the
acoustic
target
stimuli
are
presented
together.
Test form S3 was used and duration was 4 to 7
minutes.
Vienna Risk Taking Test Traffic (WRBTV)
Videos of 24 traffic situations are presented on
the screen. Each traffic situation is presented
twice. At the second presentation the participant
has to indicate when the situation becomes too
dangerous by stopping the video.
Duration was 15 to 20 minutes.
Testscores and calculation of sum score (primary outcome measure)
For the different tests, specific main variables, secondary variables and additional results were used to
describe the participants’performance. These variables have been defined by the manufacturer and
are specific for the test and the test version. The performance rating is based on the main and
secondary variables hence only these variables are described in table 4 and were used in the further
analysis. A more detailed description of all variables and the available additional results can be found
in the manuals referred to in that table.
Besides raw values, percentile ranks for the individual’
s performance with respect to an ageindependent reference group are provided for all main and secondary variables by the test system.
According to FeV absence of fitness to drive was assumed if the results of an individual were below
the 16th-percentile in any of the five tests (DT, COG, TAVTMB, LVT, RT) in any main or secondary
variable. In line with that rationale all tests were classified as passed if the main and, if available, the
secondary variables were above the 16th-percentile.
Comparable to published studies on driving ability of pain patients in which the same testing device
was used (e.g. Sabatowski et al., 2003), a sum score was defined as a primary endpoint measure in
advance. In the present study this score was calculated of all main and secondary variables of the
tests after z-transformation. Since only DT, COG, TAVTMB, LVT and RT are obligatory according to
FeV only those tests were included into this sum score. Hence the score differs from the one used in
the study referred to above but fulfils the recommendations of the German Driving Licensing Act (FeV)
as close as possible. In case there were several main and/or secondary variables available for a
specific test, an average score was calculated of these variables after z-transformation and then this
averaged scores was included into the sum score instead of the main and secondary variables. The
198
overall sum score of all tests as well as the scores for specific tests were calculated in the way that
higher values represent a better performance.
2HAND, VIGIL and WRBTV are not mandatory according to FeV. Therefore these tests were not
included into the overall sum score.
Rating scales
Karolinska sleepiness scale
Subjective ratings of sleepiness were assessed by the Karolinska sleepiness scale (Akerstedt &
Gillberg, 1990) before and after completion of the computerized test. Scores on the scales are ranging
from 1 “extremely alert”to 9 “very sleepy, great effort to keep alert, fighting against sleep”.
Brief pain inventory (BPI)
The German version of the Brief Pain Inventory (Radbruch et al., 1999) is a comprehensive instrument
for the assessment of pain intensity and impairments caused by pain.
Rating Scale Mental Effort (RSME; Zijlstra, 1993)
This scale asks participants to indicate the amount of effort they had invested into a task by flagging a
15cm long axis ranging from 0 to 150. Segments of the axis are tagged with verbal categories
indicating different levels of effort.
Additional self rating scales
Visual analogue scales (10cm long) were used to assess self-ratings of pain intensity, quality of
performance and intensity of alcohol effects. The latter was only done for controls whereas pain
intensity was only assessed in patients.
Collection of body fluids
For quantification of analgesics concentrations in body fluids a blood sample (10 mL) was taken of
every patient at the day of testing. Dried blood spots were prepared as well as serum by centrifugation
of half of the whole blood sample. Whole blood and serum samples were frozen at -20°C until analysis
using solid phase extraction and gas chromatography with mass spectrometric (GC-MS) detection.
Dried blood spots were stored in a cool dark place. The StatSure™ saliva sampler was used to collect
oral fluids for the quantitative analysis of opioid analgesic concentrations by GC-MS. Until analysis
was done these samples were also stored at -20°C.
199
Table 4: Brief description of main and secondary variables of all tests and abbreviations referred to in
the following sections (*= inverted for integration into sum score).
Test
DT
COG
Main
Secondary
Score (s)
Abbreviation
Reference
variable
variable
number of correct reactions
DTMV
(Neuwirth
X
number of false reactions*
DTSV1
Benesch, 2007)
X
number of missings*
DTSV2
average reaction time*
COMV
X
X
&
(Wagner
&
Karner, 2008)
TAVTMB
X
number of traffic situations
TAMV
without errors
X
(Sommer
&
Neurwirth,
number of correct answers
TASV1
2007)
across all traffic situations
X
number of errors across all
TASV2
traffic situations*
2HAND
X
average time needed to
2HMV1
(Puhr, 2008a)
pass the track
X
average % of time outside
2HMV2
track
VIGIL
X
ratio 2HMV1 to 2HMV2
2HMV3
X
total number of
correct
VIMV1
of
false
VIMV2
reaction
time
VIMV3
number of correct answers
LVMV
(Biehl, 2008)
RTMV1
(Prieler, 2008)
(Puhr, 2008b)
reactions
X
total
number
reactions
X
average
(correct reactions)
LVT
X
within limited time frame
RT
X
average
reaction
time*
(time span between signal
and release of rest button)
X
Average motor time* (time
RTMV2
span between release from
rest
button
and
button
press)
WRBTV
X
variation reaction time*
RTSV1
X
Variation motor time*
RTSV2
average
WRMV
time
distance
(Hergovich,
(sec) to critical situation
Arendasy,
when videos were stopped
Sommer,
by participant
Bognar, 2007)
&
200
Statistical analysis
SPSS 19 for Windows was used for all statistical analysis. The testing procedure consisted of three
steps.
1) Superiority testing by ANOVA for between group comparisons of patients and controls (sober).
2) Superiority testing by general linear model (GLM) repeated measures ANOVA with alcohol (two
levels: 0.5‰ vs. sober) as main within subject factor to compare performance of controls driving sober
as well as under influence of alcohol.
3) Only for main measures, in case superiority of controls over patients was proven, equivalence
testing based on difference scores was conducted in addition (patients’scores – average score of the
sober controls). In addition an alcohol criterion was calculated (= averaged difference scores controls
alcoholized – controls sober). Equivalence testing assessed whether this alcohol criterion fell within
the 95% CI for the difference scores of patients. If that was the case the difference between groups
was considered equivalent to a BAC of 0.5‰ and therefore relevant for traffic safety. If the 95% CI
was below the alcohol criterion value, the difference between patients and healthy controls was not
considered to be relevant.
Results
Between December 2009 and July 2010 a total number of 26 pain patients completed the test. The
alcohol calibration proceeded from January 2010 to September 2010. Only data of patients and
controls fulfilling the inclusion criteria was included into data analysis.
Alcohol levels
For the alcohol calibration the intended BAC of controls was 0.5‰ . The mean levels measured by
breath analysis (Dräger Alcotest 6510) before and after the two parts of the test battery are shown in
table 5. At the beginning of both parts of the computer based test the intended BAC-level was
reached. As expected, BAC decreased during the time of testing. Therefore additional alcohol had to
be administered to 11 (62%) controls between the first and second part of the test.
Table 5: BAC (‰ ) measured during testing (data of controls).
Time of measurement
Average BAC
SD BAC
N
Start part 1
0.52
0.04
18
End part 1
0.36
0.09
18
Start part 2
0.51
0.12
18
End part 2
0.41
0.06
18
201
Toxicology
Toxicological analysis of the concentrations of different active agents in whole blood (B) and plasma
(p) revealed the following concentrations (see table 6).
Table 6: Average substance concentrations (range) in whole blood (B) and in plasma (p); N = number
of samples available.
active agent
B
p
N
Buprenorphine
0.36 (0.21 – 0.48)
0.34 (0.18 -0.47)
4
Fentanyl
39.04 (20.39 – 110.55)
27.38 (13.66 – 82.31)
5
Hydromorphone
5.56 (2.31 – 12.11)
5.64 (2.41 – 9.89)
8
Morphine
298.10 (82.36 – 479.10)
285.00 (85.74 – 468.85)
4
Oxycodone
39.04 (20.39 – 110.55)
27.38 (13.66 – 82.31)
5
Subjective measures
Self-ratings on several aspects related to performance in the computer based test were taken. Since
the tests were not done at once but in two parts with a break in between, self-ratings were assessed
for both parts separately. Unless otherwise stated, the following results refer to the averaged ratings of
these two parts.
Alcohol impairment
Before starting the test in the alcohol condition, controls were asked if they feel fit to drive. All of them
felt unfit to drive. In addition they had to indicate the intensity of the alcohol effect before and after both
parts of the test on a 10 cm visual analogue scale ranging from zero (no effect at all) to 10 (strongest
effect). Furthermore self ratings of the amount of impairment caused by alcohol had been assessed
for both parts of the test.
As can be seen in figure 2 during the whole time of testing participants’ratings are near the middle of
the scale with a slight decrease in reported intensity at the end of part two. As shown in figure 3 a
medium intensity of impairment was reported for both parts of the test.
Figure 2: Mean (± SD) self-rated intensity of
Figure 3: Mean (± SD) self-rated impairment by
alcohol effect before and after both parts of the
alcohol on performance (0 = no impairment at all
test (0 = no effect at all / 10 = strongest effect).
/ 10 = strongest impairment).
202
Pain and impairments related to pain and pain treatment
Patients reported a moderate intensity of pain during the course of the computer-based test (see
figure 4). Pain intensity did not change over time (F3, 75 = 0.625, p
.601). They rated the impact of
pain and of their analgesic medication as being low (see figure 5).
Figure 4: Mean (± SD) self-rated intensity of pain
Figure 5: Mean (± SD) self-rated impairment
before and after both parts of the test (0 = no
caused by pain and by analgesic (0 = no
pain at all / 10 = strongest pain).
impairment at all / 10 = strongest impairment).
Effort
By means of a 15cm long scale (RSM) participants indicated the effort they needed to complete the
task. More effort was needed by controls to perform the test battery under influence of alcohol (F1, 17
= 5.760, p
.028; see also table 7). No statistical significant differences in ratings were found between
patients and controls.
Sleepiness
Scores on the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale are ranging from 1 “extremely alert”to 9 “very sleepy,
great effort to keep alert, fighting against sleep”. As can be seen in table 7 on average participants
rated themselves being neither alert nor sleepy in all conditions.
Performance
A 10 cm visual analogue scale ranging from zero (very bad) to 10 (very well) was used to assess selfratings of quality of performance (see table 7). Ratings of patients did not differ from those of healthy
controls. But controls rated their performance worse under influence of alcohol (F1, 17 = 22.564, p
.000).
Table 7: Means (SD) and results of superiority test of subjective measures (P = patients, C- = controls
sober, C+ = controls 0.5‰ ).
groups (m (sd)
ANOVA (p
P
C-
C+
(N = 26)
(N = 21)
(N=18)
P vs. C-
)
C- vs. C+
Effort
5.47 (2.49)
4.34 (1.35)
5.62 (2.57)
.067
.028
Sleepiness
4.73 (1.74)
4.76 (1.32)
5.56 (1.98)
.936
.150
6.08 (1.31)
6.07 (1.61)
4.42 (1.29)
.990
.000
(KSS)
Performance
203
Passed tests
Table 8 shows the percentage of participants having passed all five tests. The percentage differs
between groups. Only 2 out of 26 patients (8%) passed all five tests (DT, COG, TAVTMB, LVT, RT)
and therefore fulfilled the prerequisites of FeV for being fit to drive. Even though the percentage of
controls having passed all tests in the sober condition is higher (33%), even 14 controls (67%) failed to
pass all tests. Under influence of 0.5‰ alcohol the percentage of controls having passed all tests is
lower. Only 4 out of 18 controls passed all five tests (22%).
Table 8: Percentage of participants having passed / failed all five tests according to FeV between
groups.
Passed?
N
Patients
26
Controls
Controls
(sober)
(0.05‰ )
21
18
Yes
8%
33%
22%
No
92%
67%
78%
The percentage of patients and controls scoring above the 16th percentile for all main and secondary
variables is shown in table 9 for all tests. It becomes immediately obvious that the percentage of
participants who meet the criteria to pass the Determination test is very low in all conditions. Overall,
the percentage of patients having passed the test was below that of the reference group. Alcohol
seems to improve performance of healthy subjects since more of them passed LVT under influence of
alcohol. In the WRBTV more patients met the 16th-percentile criteria indicating that they accept lower
levels of risk in critical traffic situations.
Table 9: Percentage of participants having passed a test. For description of main and secondary
variables see table 4; passed means all main and secondary variables of a test above the 16th
percentile.
test name
patients
controls
controls
(sober)
(0.05‰ )
N
26
21
18
DT
46%
48%
39%
COG
96%
100%
100%
TAVTMB
69%
95%
100%
LVT
88%
100%
100%
RT
42%
71%
61%
2HAND
75%
76%
83%
VIGIL
58%
71%
61%
WRBTV
92%
71%
56%
When taking into account only those five mandatory tests, patients passed three (m = 3.42, sd = 1.03)
tests on average, controls passed four tests sober (m = 4.14, sd =.793) as well as under influence of
alcohol (m = 4.00, sd =.686).
204
Participants have to score above the 16th percentile on 12 main and secondary variables in total (six
main variables and six secondary variables; see table 4) to meet the criteria of FeV for being fit to
drive. To pass all eight tests of the test battery, participants have to score above the 16th percentile on
19 main and secondary variables in total.
On average, patients scored above the 16th percentile on nine of 12 variables of the mandatory tests
(see table 10). They performed worse than controls tested sober (F1, 45 = 7.637, p
.008).
Performance of patients was also worse than performance of controls under influence of alcohol (F1,
42 = 4.591, p
.038). Alcohol did not affect the number of passed variables in the control group (F1,
17 = 0.680, n.s.). Patients’performance is also below the performance level of controls when taking
into account all variables of all tests of the test battery (F1, 45 = 6.327, p
.016).
Sum score (primary endpoint)
A sum score was defined as primary endpoint in advance. This score comprises the z-transformed
raw values of all main and secondary variables of all five tests mandatory according to FeV. Higher
values of this score indicate higher performance levels. As can be seen from figure 6 and table 10,
performance of controls is nearly equal whether they are under influence of 0.5‰ BAC or not. Patients
perform worse than controls (F1, 45 = 14.983, p
.000).
An additional equivalence test was done based on difference scores from control group (sober).
Basically, this procedure assesses whether the alcohol criterion value (average difference sum score
between controls sober and under influence of alcohol) falls within the 95%CI of difference scores
between patients and controls. Figure 7 indicates that the performance difference between patients
and controls is not equal to a performance change caused by alcohol.
Figure 6: Mean (± SD) sum score of all
Figure 7: Average difference scores for patients
mandatory tests in all groups (P = patients, C- =
(95% CI) to sober controls. The alcohol criterion
controls sober, C+ = controls 0.5‰ ). Higher
(horizontal line = average difference scores
values indicating better performance.
between controls sober and under 0.5‰ BAC) is
not included in the 95% CI.
205
Table 10: Summary of means (SD) and effects in the computer based test (P = patients, C- = controls
sober, C+ = controls 0.5‰ ).
groups (m (sd))
Number
of
variables
>
16th
ANOVA (p
)
P
C-
C+
P vs. C-
P vs. C+
C- vs. C+
9.42 (2.02)
10.81 (1.21)
10.56 (1.15)
.008
.038
.421
15.23 (2.89)
17.00 (1.58)
16.22 (1.86)
.016
.207
.076
-1.82 (2.75)
1.11 (2.35)
1.33 (2.28)
.000
.000
.0197
percentile
(FeV only)
Number
of
variables
>
16th
percentile
(all tests)
Sum
Score
(z
transformed)
There was a negative correlation between age and the sum score of all tests as can be seen in table
11 only for controls. This indicates that performance decreases with age. Neither gender nor driving
experience or frequency of driving was significantly correlated to this primary measure in both groups.
Patients included in the present study were treated with different opioid analgesics and with different
dosage. Moreover, they differed in duration of pain. The strength of the analgesic effect of different
opioids can be compared with reference to morphine. Therefore morphine equivalent dosages were
calculated. Neither morphine equivalent dosage nor duration of pain was correlated to the
performance in the test battery (see table 12).
Table 11: Correlation (Pearson) between sum score and sample characteristics of patients and of
controls.
Patients
Controls
Characteristic
r (Pearson) p (two-sided)
N
r (Pearson)
p (two-sided)
N
Age
-.158
.441
26
-.672
.001
21
Gender
-.204
.318
26
-.095
.681
21
.085
.681
26
-.211
.358
21
.202
.323
26
-.331
.142
21
Driving
experience
(km last 12 month)
Frequency
driving
of
206
Table 12: Correlation (Pearson) between sum score duration of pain and morphine equivalent dose
(only patient group).
p (two-sided) N
r
Characteristic
(Pearson)
duration of pain
.105
.609
26
morphine equivalent .244
.229
26
dose
Performance profiles
In the following section the performance of patients and sober controls is compared. This section
starts with the mandatory tests. Since superiority of the control group was already proven for the sum
scores, testing can be regarded as closed procedure. Therefore no adjustment was made for multiple
comparisons. T-test for independent samples was used to compare performance in both groups.
Determination Test (DT)
DT measures stress tolerance and reaction speed. For the test, the respondent has to distinguish
different stimuli (colours, sounds) and press the corresponding response buttons. As can be seen from
table 13 the number of correct reactions was lower in the group of patients. This indicates that their
stress tolerance and reaction speed was diminished compared to healthy controls. The number of
false or missed reactions did not differ between both groups.
Table 13: Main and secondary variables of DT.
Variable
Patients
Controls
m (sd)
m (sd)
388.81
494.43
(103.92)
(90.07)
30.15
29.81
reactions
(22.18)
(21.96)
Number of missings
32.42
28.95
(16.81)
(15.63)
Number
of
correct
reactions
Number
of
false
T
df
p
(one
sided)
3.673
45
.000
-.053
45
.479
-.726
45
.236
Cognitrone (COG)
Cognitrone measures attention and concentration. The respondent’
s task is to compare figures and
indicate if they correspond or not. No significant difference was found between patients and controls
for the average time needed for correct reactions (see table 14).
207
Table 14: Main variable of COG.
Variable
Average reaction time
Patients
Controls
m (sd)
m (sd)
2.94
2.66
(0.69)
(0.54)
T
df
p
(one
sided)
-1.515
45
.068
Adaptive Tachistoscopic Traffic Perception Test (TAVTMB)
By briefly presenting pictures of traffic situations TAVTMB measures the observational ability of the
respondent. After the short presentation, the respondent has to indicate which information had been
given on these pictures (multiple choice). Patients performed worse in all three variables used to
describe performance in this test (see table 15). On average the number of traffic situations for which
the patients indicated all relevant information correctly was lower for patients than for controls.
Moreover they ticked less correct answers and made more errors in the test. Hence the patients’
ability to gain an overview of traffic situations is diminished compared to healthy controls.
Table 15: Main and secondary variables of TAVTMB.
Variable
Number
Patients
Controls
m (sd)
m (sd)
traffic
10.23
12.76
without
(2.82)
(2.30)
correct
45.81
48.52
(4.53)
(2.94)
of
situations
T
df
p
(one
sided)
3.317
45
.001
2.372
45
.011
-2.002
45
.026
errors
Number
of
answers
across
all
traffic situations
Number
of
errors
3.73
2.10
across
all
traffic
(3.47)
(1.55)
situations
Visual Pursuit Test (LVT)
LVT assesses visual orientation ability. The respondent has to track simple visual elements in a
complex environment. On average, the number of correct responses of patients is lower than the
number of correct responses of controls. Hence visual orientation ability of patients is lower (see table
16) because they are worse than healthy controls in obtaining an overview in a complex environment
quickly and accurately.
208
Table 16: Main variable of LVT.
Variable
Patients
Controls
m (sd)
m (sd)
correct
10.65
13.67
answers within limited
(4.76)
(3.51)
Number
of
T
df
p
(one
sided)
2.417
45
.010
time frame
Reaction Test (RT)
In order to measure reaction time, visual and acoustic stimuli are presented to the respondent in this
test. A reaction is only required in case both stimuli are presented together. This test distinguishes
between reaction time and motor time. Table 17 shows the results. The time span between stimulus
and release of the rest button, referred to as reaction time, is longer for patients than for controls. Also
the time span between release from rest button and button press, referred to as motor time, is longer
for patients than for controls. Moreover, the variation of both measures is higher within the group of
patients. To sum up, patients react slower to the onset of stimuli than healthy controls.
Table 17: Main and secondary variables of RT.
Variable
Average reaction time
Average motor time
Variation reaction time
Variation motor time
Patients
Controls
m (sd)
m (sd)
481.15
444.24
(70.51)
(70.55)
193.15
149.29
(59.29)
(51.79)
78.96
63.76
(20.41)
(18.55)
29.65
22.48
(12.15)
(12.09)
T
df
p
(one
sided)
-1.784
45
.041
-2.666
45
.005
-2.642
45
.006
-2.017
45
.025
Two-Hand Coordination Test (2HAND)
Due to motor impairments in the hands, two patients were not able to do the Two-Hand Coordination
Test. For further analysis, their missing scores were replaced by mean values of their group. The
mean time for passing the track was longer in the patient-group than in the control group. Patients
moved the dot longer outside the track than controls (table 18). Compared to healthy controls patients
are impaired in motor-coordination. They are not as good as controls in transferring visual information
about deviations into motor reactions for compensation.
209
Table 18: Main and secondary variables of 2HAND.
Variable
Patients
Controls
m (sd)
m (sd)
Average time needed
49.66
37.66
to pass the track (1)
(23.99)
(17.91)
Average
2.80
1.65
outside track (2)
(3.57)
(1.19)
Ratio (1) to (2)
5.46
5.47
(5.08)
(4.99)
%
of
time
T
df
p
(one
sided)
-1.879
43
.034
-1.412
43
.083
.003
43
.499
Vigilance Test (VIGIL)
Vigilance Test revealed no differences between patients and controls (see table 19) with respect to the
difference between their ability to sustain attention under monotonous task conditions.
Table 19: Main and secondary variables of VIGIL.
Variable
Patients
Controls
m (sd)
m (sd)
Total number of correct
97.65
97.43
reactions
(3.01)
(3.67)
Total number of false
3.92
2.33
reactions
(4.64)
(1.83)
Average reaction time
0.47
0.47
(correct reactions)
(0.09)
(0.07)
T
df
p
(one
sided)
-.232
45
.409
-1.478
45
.073
-.251
45
.401
Vienna Risk Taking Test Traffic (WRBTV)
Willingness to take risk is measured by the time distance to the critical situation in a video at which the
video file is stopped by the respondent. The average time distance to the critical traffic situations at
which the video files were stopped by the patients is lower than by the controls (see table 20). This
indicates that patients accepted a higher level of risk in traffic situations.
Table 20: Main variable of WRBTV.
Variable
Patients
Controls
m (sd)
m (sd)
Average time distance
6.68
8.02
(sec)
(1.52)
(1.75)
T
df
p
(one
sided)
2.805
45
.004
210
Discussion
The present study demonstrated that patients with chronic pain treated with stable doses of opioid
analgesics show some impairment in driving related skills compared to healthy controls. Driving ability
was measured by a computerized test that fulfils the German law regulations for the assessment of
fitness to drive (Vienna Test System). To fulfil the criteria of being fit to drive according to the German
Driving Licensing Act (FeV), respondents have to score above the 16th percentile of an ageindependent reference group on five standardized tests assessing five areas of performance: stress
tolerance, visual orientation ability, concentration, attention and reaction speed (BASt, 2009;
Schuhfried GmbH, 2009). The impairment becomes obvious when looking at the tests with respect to
this criteria, as well as when looking at the whole set of mandatory test. In addition a sum score was
defined in advance as primary outcome measure. This score included all raw values of the
performance measures specified by the manufacturer for the tests fulfilling German regulations for the
assessment of fitness to drive. With respect to this sum score patients perform worse than a group of
age-independent healthy controls.
When looking at the individual tests of the test battery it becomes obvious that stress tolerance and
reaction speed was diminished compared to healthy controls as well as observational ability, visual
orientation ability and motor-coordination. Therefore the results of this study are not in line with
findings of studies on driving ability of pain patients treated with opioid analgesics published before
(Dagtekin et al., 2007; Gaertner et al., 2006; Sabatowski et al., 2003). The authors of those studies
conclude from their results that driving ability of patients treated with transdermal Fentanyl
(Sabatowski et al., 2003), transdermal Buprenorphine (Dagtekin et al., 2007) or controlled release
Oxycodone (Gaertner et al., 2006) is not impaired compared to healthy controls. Even though the
same computer-based tests were used, there are some aspects in which these studies are different
from the present study. These are: primary outcome measure, composition of the patient group and
composition of the control group.
Primary outcome measures
Overall it becomes immediately obvious that in the study at hand the percentage of patients as well as
the percentage of controls that fulfil the strict requirements of being fit to drive is low: Among healthy
controls only 70% passed the test measuring reaction speed which is one of the areas of performance
considered being relevant for being fit to drive according to the German law. Only half of them passed
the test measuring stress tolerance. Table 20 compares the percentage of patients who have passed
the test in this study to the results of the published studies referred to before. Overall the percentage
of patients having passed a test in the intent-to-treat group in these studies is higher.
211
Table 20: Percentage of patients having passed a test in this study and in the other published studies
(xx = not assessed / no results available; results of intent-to-treat-group).
test name
this study
Dagtekin et al.,
2007
5
Gaertner et al.,
Sabatowski
2006
al., 2003
N
26
30
30
30
DT
46%
80%
66.7%
95%
COG
96%
85%
72.4%
80%
TAVTMB
69%
85%
82.1%
82%
LVT
88%
xx
xx
xx
RT
42%
xx
xx
xx
2HAND
75%
85%
82.8%
xx
VIGIL
58%
75%
79.3%
xx
WRBTV
92%
xx
xx
xx
et
6
However in this study the percentage of patients who passed the tests is lower than the percentage of
controls. Due to the high failure rate in some of the tests of the test battery, the percentage of patients
and controls who meet the performance criteria for all tests was low, too.
Therefore it seemed reasonable to assume, that this narrow interpretation of the German law
regulations is too strict. Law regulations also allow that a bad performance in one test can be
counterbalanced by a good performance in another test. This takes into account that most of the
mandatory tests assess more than one area of performance. The main methodological shortcoming of
this approach is that then a case by-case decision by experts would be necessary.
As in the studies mentioned before a sum score was defined in advance as primary outcome
measure. This score integrates all main and secondary variables recommended by the test system
manufacturer. Herewith this score is very close to law regulations. Performance of patients is also
below that of controls with reference to this sum score. The way this sum score is calculated in the
present study differs from the way this is done in the studies mentioned before. Dagtekin et al., 2007;
Gaertner et al., 2006; Sabatowski et al., 2003 integrated only three tests (DT, COG, TAVT) into this
score whereas in this study the results of DT, COG, TAVT, LVT and RT are integrated in order to fulfil
the recommendations of the manufacturer of the computer based testing device and the
recommendations of FeV. Moreover Dagtekin et al., 2007; Gaertner et al., 2006; Sabatowski et al.,
2003 did not use all main and secondary variables as recommended by the manufacturer but used
two variables of each test representing performance speed and quality of performance (Stachwitz,
2006).
Composition of the patient group
Whereas Dagtekin et al., 2007; Gaertner et al., 2006; Sabatowski et al., 2003 included only patients
treated with one specific opioid analgesic in one study, the sample in this study is very heterogeneous.
Overall five opioid analgesics at different dose levels have been included. Due to the low sample size
no results concerning the different opioids have been possible. So in the present study several
5
6
numbers estimated from figure.
numbers estimated from figure.
212
different opioid analgesics at different dose levels were included. Moreover all patients were treated
with additional medications. But correlation analysis in the present study revealed that morphine
equivalence dosage and test performance were not related.
Composition of the control group and type of study
The performance of patients here was compared to the performance of a reference group stratified for
age. Whereas the performance of patients was compared to the performance of matched controls in
the published studies(Dagtekin et al., 2007; Gaertner et al., 2006; Sabatowski et al., 2003), both
groups in this study differ in mean age (patients m = 54.00; sd = 8.28 / controls m = 43.10; sd =
10.68). Correlation analysis of the data revealed that performance decreased with age so age and
performance were related which might account for the different results. In addition the studies which
have been published on the same topic were designed as non-inferiority studies. In those studies
performance of patients was compared to historic performance data of healthy volunteers performing
under influence of 0.5‰ alcohol. Instead of testing the controls under influence of alcohol virtual
values that are equivalent to test performance under influence of 0.5‰ alcohol had been calculated.
This calculation was based on the effect size from a vigilance test controls did under influence of 0.5‰
alcohol.
This study was designed to compare the amount of impairment to that caused by 0.5‰ alcohol in
healthy volunteers. But it was not possible to show an alcohol induced performance decrement,
although an impairment could have been expected according to a review on the effects of low doses
of alcohol on skills related to driving (Moskowitz & Dary, 2000). But due to organizational restrictions it
was not possible to counterbalance the sequence of sober and alcoholized testing so all controls first
performed the test sober. Although the intended BAC of 0.5‰ was successfully reached in the alcohol
condition and maintained stable during testing, no performance decrement could be found. According
to their self-ratings controls felt impaired by the alcohol and rated their performance being worse than
in the sober condition. So it seems that the alcohol effect was not strong enough to outweigh the
training effect, although there was a delay of at least two weeks between both test sessions and there
was a training phase before data was acquired right at the beginning of each test.
The results of this study demonstrated that patients suffering from chronic non-cancer pain, treated
with stable doses of opioid analgesics, are impaired in driving related skills. But it could not be
concluded if the impairment is comparable to the impairment caused by alcohol in healthy people.
Therefore further research is needed and no general statement can be given about patients’fitness to
drive under opioid treatment. It is necessary to make a by-case decision taking into account several
aspects.
213
References
Akerstedt, T. & Gillberg, M. (1990). Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual
27. Int.J.Neurosci., 52, 29-37.
BASt (2009). Begutachtungsleitlinien zur Kraftfahrereignung (Berichte der Bundesanstalt für
Straßenwesen (BASt), Mensch und Sicherheit, Heft M 115). Bremerhaven: Wirtschaftsverlag NW.
Biehl, B. (2008). LVT Linienverfolgungstest. Mödling: Schuhfried.
Dagtekin, O., Gerbershagen, H. J., Wagner, W., Petzke, F., Radbruch, L., & Sabatowski, R. (2007).
Assessing cognitive and psychomotor performance under long-term treatment with transdermal
buprenorphine in chronic noncancer pain patients. Anesth.Analg., 105, 1442-1448.
Dellemijn, P. L. I., van Duijn, H., & Vanneste, J. A. L. (1998). Prolonged treatment with transdermal
fentanyl in neuropathic pain. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management, 16, 220-229.
Dick, B. D. & Rashiq, S. (2007). Disruption of attention and working memory traces in individuals with
chronic pain. Anesth.Analg., 104, 1223-9, tables.
Drummer, O. H., Gerostamoulos, J., Batziris, H., Chu, M., Caplehorn, J., Robertson, M. D. et al.
(2004). The involvement of drugs in drivers of motor vehicles killed in Australian road traffic crashes.
Accid.Anal.Prev., 36, 239-248.
Eccleston, C., Crombez, G., Aldrich, S., & Stannard, C. (1997). Attention and somatic awareness in
chronic pain. Pain, 72, 209-215.
Fishbain, D. A., Cole, B., Cutler, R. B., Lewis, J., Rosomoff, H. L., & Fosomoff, R. S. (2003). Is pain
fatiguing? A structured evidence-based review. Pain Med., 4, 51-62.
Fishbain, D. A., Cutler, R. B., Rosomoff, H. L., & Rosomoff, R. S. (2002). Can patients taking opioids
drive safely? A structured evidence-based review. J.Pain Palliat.Care Pharmacother., 16, 9-28.
Fishbain, D. A., Cutler, R. B., Rosomoff, H. L., & Rosomoff, R. S. (2003). Are opioiddependent/tolerant patients impaired in driving-related skills? A structured evidence-based review.
J.Pain Symptom Manage., 25, 559-577.
Gaertner, J., Elsner, F., Radbruch, L., Kolibay, F., Theisohn, M., Berghaus, G. et al. (2008). Influence
of changes to daily dose of opioids on aspects of cognitive and psychomotor performance involved in
driving. Schmerz., 22, 433-441.
Gaertner, J., Radbruch, L., Giesecke, T., Gerbershagen, H., Petzke, F., Ostgathe, C. et al. (2006).
Assessing cognition and psychomotor function under long-term treatment with controlled release
oxycodone in non-cancer pain patients. Acta Anaesthesiol.Scand., 50, 664-672.
Grisart, J. (2009). [Cognitive effects of opioids]. Rev.Med.Suisse, 5, 1356-3.
Grisart, J. M. & Plaghki, L. H. (1999). Impaired selective attention in chronic pain patients. Eur.J.Pain,
3, 325-333.
Hergovich,
A.,
Arendasy,
M.,
Sommer,
M.,
&
Bognar,
B.
(2007).
WRBTV
Wiener
Risikobereitschaftstest Verkehr. Mödling: Schuhfried.
Jamison, R. N., Schein, J. R., Vallow, S., Ascher, S., Vorsanger, G. J., & Katz, N. P. (2003).
Neuropsychological effects of long-term opioid use in chronic pain patients. J.Pain Symptom.Manage.,
26, 913-921.
Kalso, E., Edwards, J. E., Moore, R. A., & McQuay, H. J. (2004). Opioids in chronic non-cancer pain:
systematic review of efficacy and safety. Pain, 112, 372-380.
Kress, H. G. & Kraft, B. (2005). Opioid medication and driving ability. Eur.J.Pain, 9, 141-144.
214
Lagarde, E., Chastang, J. F., Lafont, S., Coeuret-Pellicer, M., & Chiron, M. (2005). Pain and pain
treatment were associated with traffic accident involvement in a cohort of middle-aged workers.
Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 58, 524-531.
Lenné, M., Dietze, P., Rumbold, G., Redman, J. R., & Triggs, T. J. (2000). Opioid dependence and
driving ability: a review in the context of proposed legislative change in Victoria
8. Drug and Alcohol Review, 19, 427-439.
Mailis-Gagnon, A., Lakha, S. F., Ou, T., Louffat, A., Yegneswaran, B., Umana, M. et al. (2011).
Chronic noncancer pain: Characteristics of patients prescribed opioids by community physicians and
referred to a tertiary pain clinic. Can.Fam.Physician, 57, e97-e105.
McCracken, L. M. & Iverson, G. L. (2001). Predicting Complaints of Impaired Cognitive Functioning in
Patients with Chronic Pain. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management, 21, 392-396.
McQuay, H. (1999). Opioids in pain management. Lancet, 353, 2229-2232.
Mersky, H. & Bogduk, N. (1994). Classification of chronic pain: Descriptions of chronic pain
syndromes and definitions of pain terms. (2 ed.) Seattle: IASP Press.
Moore, R. A. & McQuay, H. J. (2005). Prevalence of opioid adverse events in chronic non-malignant
pain: systematic review of randomised trials of oral opioids. Arthritis Res.Ther., 7, 1046-1051.
Moskowitz, H. & Dary, F. (2000). A Review of the literature on the effects of low doses of alcohol on
driving-related skills. Washington: U.S. Department of Transportation, National Highway Traffic Safety
Administration.
Movig, K. L., Mathijssen, M. P., Nagel, P. H., van Egmond, T., DE GIER, J. J., Leufkens, H. G. et al.
(2004). Psychoactive substance use and the risk of motor vehicle accidents. Accid.Anal.Prev., 36,
631-636.
Neuwirth, W. & Benesch, M. (2007). DT Determinationstest. Mödling: Schuhfried.
Nickel, R. & Raspe, H. H. (2001). Chronischer Schmerz: Epidemiologie und Inanspruchnahme.
Nervenarzt, 72, 897-906.
Portenoy, R. K. (2000). Current pharmacotherapy of chronic pain. J.Pain Symptom.Manage., 19, 1620.
Prieler, J. (2008). RT Reaktionstest. Mödling: Schuhfried.
Puhr, U. (2008a). 2HAND Zweihand Koordination. Mödling: Schuhfried.
Puhr, U. (2008b). VIGIL Vigilanz. Mödling: Schuhfried.
Radbruch, L., Loick, G., Kiencke, P., Lindena, G., Sabatowski, R., Grond, S. et al. (1999). Validation of
the German version of the Brief Pain Inventory. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management, 18, 180187.
Sabatowski, R., Kaiser, U., & Gossrau, G. (2010). Chronische Schmerzen: Fahrsicherheitsrelevante
Aspekte beim Einsatz von Opioiden [Opioids in the management of chronic pain and driving ability].
Anasthesiol.Intensivmed.Notfallmed.Schmerzther., 45, 384-385.
Sabatowski, R., Schwalen, S., Rettig, K., Herberg, K. W., Kasper, S. M., & Radbruch, L. (2003).
Driving ability under long-term treatment with transdermal fentanyl. J Pain Symptom Manage., 25, 3847.
Schmidt, H., Lowenstein, O., Hesselbarth, S., Klinge, V., & Michels, H. (2010). [S3 guidelines LONTS
(Long-term administration of opioids for non-tumor pain)]. Schmerz., 24, 71-72.
215
Schubert, W., Schneider, W., Eisenmenger, W., & Stephan, E. (2005). Begutachtungsleitlinien zur
Kraftfahrereignung. Kommentar. (2 nd. ed.) Bonn: Kirschbaum.
Schuhfried GmbH (2009). Verkehrspsychologie: Testbatterie
nach FeV Anlage 5 Nr. 2
(Bedienungsanleitung). Mödling: Schuhfried.
Sjogren, P., Olsen, A. K., Thomsen, A. B., & Dalberg, J. (2000). Neuropsychological performance in
cancer patients: the role of oral opioids, pain and performance status. Pain, 86, 237-245.
Sommer, M. & Neurwirth, W. (2007). TAVTMB Tachistoskopischer Verkehrsauffassungstest. Mödling:
Schuhfried.
Stachwitz, P. (2006). Fahrsicherheit unter analgetischer Medikation mit retardiertem Oxycodon bei
Patienten mit chronischen nicht-tumorbedingten Schmerzen. Medizinsche Fakultät der Universität
Köln.
Strumpf, M., Willweber-Strumpf, A., Herberg, K. W., & Zenz, M. (2005). Sicherheitsrelevante
Leistungsfähigkeit von Schmerzpatienten unter Langzeitopioidtherapie. Der Schmerz, 19, 426-433.
Tassain, V., Attal, N., Fletcher, D., Brasseur, L., Degieux, P., Chauvin, M. et al. (2003). Long term
effects of oral sustained release morphine on neuropsychological performance in patients with chronic
non-cancer pain. Pain, 104, 389-400.
Vainio, A., Ollila, J., Matikainen, E., Rosenberg, P., & Kalso, E. (1995). Driving ability in cancer
patients receiving long-term morphine analgesia. The Lancet, 346, 667-670.
Veldhuijzen, D. S., Karscha, A. M., & van Wijck, A. J. M. (2009). Drugs, driving and traffic safety in
acute and chronic pain. In J.C.Verster, S. R. Pandi-Perumal, J. G. Ramaekers, & J. J. DE GIER
(Eds.), Drugs, driving and traffic safety (pp. 355-370). Basel: Birkhäuser.
Veldhuijzen, D. S., van Wijck, A. J., Wille, F., Verster, J. C., Kenemans, J. L., Kalkman, C. J. et al.
(2006). Effect of chronic nonmalignant pain on highway driving performance. Pain, 122, 28-35.
Wagner, M. & Karner, T. (2008). COG Cognitrone. Mödling: Schuhfried.
Watson, P. E., Watson, I. D., & Batt, R. D. (1981). Prediction of blood alcohol concentrations in human
subjects. Updating the Widmark Equation. J.Stud.Alcohol, 42, 547-556.
Zacny, J. P. (1995). A review of the effects of opioids on psychomotor and cognitive functioning in
humans. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 3, 432-466.
Zijlstra, F. R. H. (1993). Efficiency in work behavior. A design approach for modern tools. PhD thesis,
Delft University of Technology, Delft, The Netherlands.
216
Chapter 10: Risperidone effects on real driving
performance compared with the effects of
alcohol
P.A. Sardi*, G.M. Sardi*, C. Signoretti*, G. Skopp†, R.P.J. Freeman‡
* SIPSiVi
Via Matteotti, 2
12100 Cuneo
Italy
† Institute of Legal Medicine
University Hospital
Heidelberg, Germany
‡ Institute of Education
University of London
20 Bedford Way,
London, United Kingdom
217
Abstract
People with psychotic disorders can be treated using antispsychotic medication. Unfortunately, such
medication has side effects that can include impairment in driving performance. The present study
was designed to assess the effects of one particular antipsychotic medication – Risperidone – on
performance in road tracking and reaction to a sudden event on a closed-circuit test track. Fifteen
patients taking Risperidone (3-4 mg/die) and fifteen matched volunteers participated, with the
volunteers driving under the double-blind administration of the legal Blood Alcohol Concentration
(BAC) of 0.5 g/l and a placebo. The mean lateral position relative to the white-line at the edge of the
road was significantly worse for those drivers under the influence of alcohol, but not for Risperidone.
However, drivers under the influence of Risperidone showed greater Standard Deviation of Lateral
Position when monitoring their speed and on straight sections of the track compared to drivers
receiving the placebo or the legal level of alcohol. In the sudden event task, both drivers under the
influence of a legal level of alcohol and Risperidone were significantly slower in reacting compared to
the placebo, but there was no difference between these two conditions. It is concluded that the effects
of alcohol and risperidone in general did not effect most driving measures. However, those measures
that were sensitive to alcohol and risperidone indicated that the effects of risperidone were
comparable or worse than effects assiociated with a BAC of 0.5 mg/ml.
Key words
Driving under the influence of drugs, DUID, antipsychotics, risperidone
218
Introduction
There is a wide consensus on considering all antipsychotic medicines to cause a serious
impairment of driving capacity. A notable exception is Lithium, which is considered to have the least
impact on driving performance, but is unfortunately unable to reduce all kinds of psychiatric symptoms.
As confirmed by the DRUID deliverable 4.1.1 “Review of existing classification efforts”, issued by the
DRUID WP4 on the 7th of February 2008, there is not yet an internationally accepted classification of
these driving impairments and the existing classifications are largely based on expert opinions rather
than on experimental evidence.
In addition to the prescription of antipsychotic medicines, modern antipsychotic therapies
include a focus on social integration as a basic approach. A particular example is employment, which
often requires driving a car, especially in countries where the public transport network is not well
developed. For this reason, psychiatric teams trying to socially integrate their patients through
working, and hence driving, are particularly interested in prescribing antipsychotic medicines that
cause the least impairment in driving performance.
Attempting to study the driving performance of psychotic patients with experimental methods
produces unique challenges. For example, the more emotional aspects of this disease, that are very
important in determining the degree of impairment and consequential risks, are not sufficiently
activated by the aseptic environment of the laboratory. As a result, the use of a driving simulator has
poor validity. However, studying driving in real traffic situations raises serious ethical issues and legal
responsibilities on the psychiatric teams. An intermediate approach can be found by using a closed
circuit where similar tasks to real driving can be studied while protecting all the participants. Such an
approach facilitates the study of the main difficulties of a real road environment, but under controlled
conditions in order to allow mistakes that will not result in road accidents. This is also an environment
where insurance companies will accept, explicitly and without reservation, to provide cover for the
risks incurred by these patients when driving.
Such a study proved possible with the participation of the Administration of the Health Unit
Rome 7 and its Psychiatric Department (who are particularly engaged in patients’socialization through
the choice of suitable jobs). Second, the Automobile Club of Italy (a public body) was interested in
studying road risks, and owns a closed circuit in Rome Vallelunga, which is located in the catchment
area of the above mentioned psychiatric department. Third, the Engineering Faculty of Parma, and its
spin-off Vislab were also available to participate. The Vislab system enables the accurate
measurement of various factors in driving: manoeuvres (including braking, steering, speed etc), the
physical relationship between the car and its environment (including distances to obstacles, road
layout, white lanes etc.). All these data can be recorded every tenth of a second, stored in an in-car
computer and made available for later analysis. Fourth, suitable psychotic patients were available and
willing to participate in such a study. Such patients face two risks - to lose their driving license and to
lose their insurance cover – and these concerns usually prevent them from participating. For this
reason, previously the effects of antipsychotic medicines on driving have been seldom tested, and
predominantly on healthy volunteers. An example is the IMMORTAL project, where antidepressant
medicines were tested on persons mourning as a reaction to a real loss, not under a psychotic
depression. But the European Commission insisted on the need to have a scientific assessment on
the degree of impairment caused by essential medicines, in order to overcome a situation where the
pharmaceutical industries are able to declare “possible”impairing effects on driving capacity so as not
219
to be prosecuted for indirectly causing road accidents, legal bodies legislate generically against driving
under the influence of these medicines, but leave responsibility for any exceptions to psychiatrists and
patients finding a doctor authorizing such exceptions, without a sound scientific basis to such
decisions. The DRUID project was initially titled “ENABLE”to highlight in the same word the aims of
formal authorization and the effective ability to drive.
The pharmaceutical industries have already tried to address these problems by proposing a
second generation of antipsychotics. Soyka, et al. (2005) notes that there “is some but limited
evidence that patients under novel atypical neuroleptics show less impairment compared to
conventional neuroleptics. More clinical and experimental studies are warranted”. Earlier, a review by
Meltzer and Gurk (1999) noted “clear improvements against the cognitive dysfunction in
schizophrenia, especially on verbal and work memory, attention, executive function, motor
coordination”in Risperidone, one of these new antipsychotics, which performs better than the other
new generation antipsychotics. Nevertheless, there is not yet consensus about the impairment
produced by Risperidone on driving ability. In the above mentioned “Review of existing classification
efforts”, it appears that Risperidone (N05AX08) is not classified under Wolschrijn, et al. (1991),
classified II (Likely to produce minor or moderate adverse affects) by ICADTS (International Council
on Alcohol, Drugs and Traffic Safety) and classified also II (nearly the same meaning as ICADTS) in
Belgium, Spain I, Spain II, Portugal and France I. However, France II classifies it as III (Attention,
danger: do not drive). This final classification, introduced by a decree published in the official bulletin
on the 2nd of August 2005, is legally binding and expressed by a label on its boxes. Greece II
classifies it “+”, that means “affecting driving ability”, and Norway “w”, which means “drug with warning
in Norwegian Catalogue”. Other classifications ignore it. In practice, only France II prohibits to drive
under Risperidone.
The producers of Risperidone state that it “May affect your driving ability; therefore, do not
drive or operate machinery before talking to your healthcare professional”. Therefore, the psychiatric
team of Rome 7 asked their patients to participate in the experiment while being treated with various
types of antipsychotic medicines, not just those treated with Risperidone. The fact that the availability
of patients treated with Risperidone was sufficiently high that 15 took part in our experiment, might be
evidence for an increase in their cognitive capacity, allowing them to understand that they could trust
that their results would be confidential and that there was no risk of losing their licence and hence their
employment as a result. The psychiatric team enabled the participation of two psychiatrists and four
nurses during all the experimental sessions.
Method
Subjects
Fourteen males and one female composed the experimental group (N = 15). Their average
age was 39 years. The patients were recruited through the psychiatric team of the Health Department
of Rome-F whose catchment area includes the closed circuit of ACI-Vallelunga.
The inclusion criteria were: patients treated with the same antipsychotic medicine
(Risperidone/Paliperidone), alone or with mood stabilizing medicines; regular, fixed doses for at least
three months (3-4 mg/die); their diagnoses include schizophrenia, paranoia, bipolar psychosis. Other
inclusion criteria were the possession of a normal driving license, driving a car at least once a week,
220
absence of other severe health problems, and unimpaired liver function enabling normal
metabolization of their medicine. The regular consumption and metabolization was also tested by the
assessment of the metabolite in their samples, sent to the Heidelberg laboratory who analyzed them.
A group of these patients also agreed to undergo the Vienna driving test. The psychiatric team
provided detailed diagnosis and symptoms for each subject.
The exclusion criteria were: acute cases; any other physical and/or psychological impairment;
other medicines affecting driving; other substance consumption (illicit drugs, alcohol abuse); clinical,
legal or social contraindications.
The experimental group received a form containing a description of the experiment trials and
of the overall aims of the DRUID project. Then they were asked to sign a consent form to agree to
participate in the experiment. For reasons of privacy, the experimental subjects were allocated codes
and in no cases did the researchers know the names of the patients, that were coded and stored by
the health personnel using their usual confidential procedure. This experimental study received the
approval of the Ethical Committee of the Health Department “USL RM/F”. The ethical proposal was
drafted in accordance with the DRUID ethical manual approved by the DRUID Consortium. At the end
of each experimental session the subjects received reimbursement in petrol vouchers.
A matched sample of 15 healthy volunteers were recruited as reference group and to enable a
comparison with the effects of alcohol. The average age of the reference group was 38 years. The
reference group was matched with the experimental group based on: gender, age, weight and overall
distance driven per year. The healthy volunteer subjects were tested for illicit drug consumption (six
substances) before each driving session. The subjects were recruited by leaflets advertising the study
circulated in the ACI Vallelunga Circuit, the Health Department Rome-F and by emails to SIPSiVi
networks. The inclusion criteria were that they matched the characteristics of the experimental
subjects.
Study design
The study was conducted using a double-blind, matched design. The experimental group was
taking a regular dose of Risperidone (inclusion criteria: 3-4 mg/die). The subjects in the reference
group were given 98% pure alcohol mixed with 200ml of bitter orange juice according with the
weight/concentration formula in order to reach a Blood Alcohol Concentration (BAC) of 0.5 g/l. Their
placebo consisted of bitter orange juice with the order of presentation of the placebo and alcohol
balanced over the subjects in the reference group.
Experimental group (Risperidone) procedure
The experimental group (Risperidone) subjects were taken to the Vallelunga circuit by the
health personnel in groups with a maximum of five subjects per session (08:00 meeting time).
Subjects received a briefing and a description of the project and then signed a consent form. The
health personnel team was two psychiatrists and four nurses. The nurses collected the blood samples
(serum, whole blood and blood dry spot) before the driving sessions. The blood sample were coded by
the health department for privacy reasons, centrifuged and transported in a cooled container box (20C) to University Hospital, Heidelberg in Germany for analyses (for details see pharmacokinetic
assessment in Appendix 1). Each subject was instructed on the driving task and practised to enable
221
learning saturation before each trial. All sessions were carried out in the morning and ended before
14:00 with each trial lasting about 45 minutes.
Reference group (alcohol calibration) procedure
The healthy volunteers for alcohol calibration were asked to arrive at Vallelunga Circuit by
08:00 with a maximum of five subjects per session. Subjects were tested for drug consumption (urine)
and only subjects who tested negative were enrolled in the study. Subjects were asked to be present
at the circuit for two sessions on two different dates at least one week apart. In each session subjects
were given 200 ml of bitter orange juice 20-30 minutes before the driving session. In one session it
was a placebo (only orange juice) and in the other session the orange juice was combined with 98%
pure alcohol according with weight of the subject so they would reach 0.5 BAC. Half of the subjects
received the alcohol in the first session and the other half received the alcohol in the second session with the other half receiving the placebo. All subjects were monitored with a breathalyzer before and
after the driving session. All the subjects that consumed alcohol beverage were required to have 0.5
BAC at the beginning and at the end of the session for their data to be included in the analysis. Only in
one case was a subject below 0.5 BAC at the end of the driving session; their data for that session
were excluded and the session repeated on another date. Each subject was instructed on the driving
task and practised to enable learning saturation before each trial. All sessions were carried out in the
morning and ended before 14:00 with each trial lasting about 45 minutes.
Driving tests
Road Tracking
Louwerens, Gloerich, de Vries, Brookhuis, & O’
Hanlon (1987) constructed the road tracking
test with the following characteristics: having the subject driving at a constant speed of 95 km/h as
straight as possible on the right lane of a highway (left-hand drive) for a one-hour test drive, with the
car equipped in order to take measures of several parameters: the primary dependent measure of this
test is the Standard Deviation from Lateral Position (SDLP), while speed and standard deviation of
speed are recorded as secondary control measures. This test enabled calibration of the effects of any
sedative drug in relation to the BAC required to achieve the equivalent level of driving impairment. The
alcohol calibration curve demonstrates that the intoxicated drivers’mean SDLP rises exponentially
with BAC. Results from the alcohol calibration study can be used for describing drug effects on SDLP
in terms of respective BAC equivalencies. The change in SDLP at BAC of 0.5 mg/ml has been used
as a criterion level to quantify drug effects. The SDLP changes of the alcohol calibration study have
been used as reference intervals (0.0 and 0.5 BAC values) to assess the equivalent effect of
Risperidone in the driving tasks of experimental subjects.
In the present study a variation of the Road Tracking test was used, taking into account the
different scenario used. We considered driving performance separately on sharp curves (that are not
usually so sharp on a highway) and on straight sections. Due to the sharp curves on the circuit, the
fixed speed we used was only 30 km/h and there were two conditions: with and without a
speedometer. In the absence of real traffic, it was possible to require the driver to keep a fixed
distance of 30 cm from the white line on the right hand side. This distance was chosen to emulate the
222
distance to be kept from the line by the driver in order not to hit pedestrians or bicycles sheltered by
that white lane, when restricted into a narrowed space.
The car used was equipped with a Driver Performance Monitor (DPM) a specific device
designed for data collection, developed by the VisLab research group (www.vislab.it). This system
includes a colour camera installed behind the vehicle's front windscreen, an embedded computer
placed in the car trunk and a touch screen monitor on the passenger's side which allows the selection
of different tests and the evaluation of the current test. The DPM permits data acquisition with no
scenario restrictions: a feature increasing the validity of the performance analysis. Moreover, powered
by advanced algorithms, it is able to manage five different tests in order to extract a complete profile of
the driver's performance. In this study the main data that have been collected and used are the
vehicle's position relative to the white line, the vehicle’s speed and the driver's reaction time.
Sudden event
The driver reaches the speed of 40 km/h, and passes besides a frame from which a “bobby
car”suddenly appears in a randomised set of trials. The bobby car is released when the front of the
driver’
s car crosses the path of a photo-cell, but the Vislab tool starts measuring the reaction time only
when the Bobby car becomes visible from the driver’
s car (as happens in real driving) and stops when
the driver starts braking. The tool also records the length of the track after braking and the movements
of the steering wheel (angle and speed of rotation).
Statistical analyses
All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS 18.0. Statistical analyses consisted of two
steps: 1) Assessment for overall treatment effects by means of superiority testing; 2) Equivalence or
non-inferiority testing of Risperidone effects based on the difference of scores compared to the
matched sample when under the influence of alcohol.
In step 1, the data were entered into the General Linear Model (GLM) repeated measures
ANOVA for the 15 matched participants (i.e. 15 controls and 15 Risperidone users) with the control
group measured both without and with alcohol. If the sphericity assumption was violated or not
applicable, the Greenhouse-Geisser correction was used.
Results
Dropouts and missing data
One of the Risperidone participants admitted to his psychiatrist after his testing session that he
had discontinued his Risperidone for three days before his test session, because he was afraid of his
driving licence being withdrawn or suspended due to his driving performance. Subsequently, blood
analyses confirmed the absence of Risperidone in his blood for that session. Approximately six
months later, the same participant took part in another test session while under treatment with
Risperidone (and the presence of Risperidone was confirmed by blood analysis).
Driving tests
For the four straight line measures (mean speed, standard deviation of the speed, mean
lateral position and standard deviation of lateral position - SDLP), no differences were significant, but
223
SDLP approached significance, (F1.5, 20.4 = 3.60, p = .058), with a trend for greater variation in the
distance from the line for Risperidone.
For the speed monitoring task, there was a significant effect for lateral position (F2, 28 = 3.79, p =
.035), with a significant difference between the alcohol and Risperidone conditions (t14 = 2.17, p =
.048). Those drivers under the influence of alcohol drove closer to the line maintaining a mean
difference of 22 cm compared with placebo 29 cm and Risperidone 32 cm as can be seen in Figure 1.
The 95% CI (-0. 046 – 0.101) associated with the difference in lateral position between the risperidone
and placebo did not include the mean difference induced by alcohol (i.e. -0.069). LP in the risperidone
group thus was equivalent to placebo controls.
Figure 1. Mean Lateral Position from white line (m) for the three conditions of the speed monitoring
task with the target distance of 30 cm marked in red.
The SDLP in the speed monitoring task also showed a significant effect, (F2, 28 = 3.65, p = .039) with
a significant difference between the alcohol and Risperidone conditions (t14 = 2.65, p = .019) with
greater variation in the distance from the line for Risperidone.
Considering only the straight sections, the mean lateral position approached significance (F1.43,
20.04 = 3.36, p = .069) with again those under the influence of alcohol driving closer (25 cm) to the
white line than either in the Placebo (28 cm) or Risperidone (33 cm) conditions. SDLP was significant
(F2, 28 = 6.23, p = .006) with a significant difference between the alcohol and Risperidone conditions
(t14 = 3.04, p = .009). Once again those using Risperidone showed greater variation (.12) compared
to Placebo (.10) and alcohol (.10).
For the curved sections only, the standard deviation of speed was significant (F1.34, 18.78 =
8.29, p = .006) with a significant difference between the alcohol and Risperidone conditions (t14 =
2.83, p = .013). Those using Risperidone (1.9) showed greater variation in speed than either Placebo
(1.2) or alcohol (1.2). In addition, SDLP approached significance (F1.28, 17.92 = 3.53, p = .068) with
again a trend to greater variation for Risperidone (.25) compared to Placebo (.22) and alcohol (.20). All
other measures were not significant.
224
Sudden event
For the sudden event, the reaction time to begin braking was significant (F2, 28 = 3.52, p =
.043), but there was not a significant difference between the alcohol and Risperidone conditions (t14 =
.66, p = .518). Both alcohol (340 ms) and Risperidone (300 ms) mean reaction times were slower than
the Placebo (210 ms) as can be seen in Figure 2. The 95% CI associated with the difference between
risperidone and placebo (i.e. -0.00560 thru 0.18560) included the mean difference between alcohol
and placebo (i.e. = 0.131). This implies that the increment in RT in the risperidone group was of
clinical relevance and comparable/bigger than that produced by a BAC=0.5 mg/ml.
Figure 2. Reaction time (ms) to the sudden event for the three conditions.
Vienna test
Psychologists based in Rome administered the Vienna test to eleven patients (three patients
were not available to undergo that check of their driving ability, mainly because of their fear of losing
their licence). Nine out of these eleven produced results indicating they were able to drive safely. One
of the two who did not had already been prevented from driving by relatives who were worried
because of frequent road accidents. The correlation between the Vienna test and the performances on
the circuit was rather strong (0.67) if these two patients are included, but much weaker when the
patient who performed worst on both tests was removed.
Discussion
The Results present a broadly consistent picture. The basic results for the Road Tracking task
that required drivers to maintain a fixed distance of 30 cm from the right-hand white line showed that
drivers under the influence of alcohol were closer to the line whereas drivers on risperidone and those
not under the influence of alcohol were able to maintain the appropriate distance. In addition, drivers
under risperidone drove with more ‘
weaving’than drivers under the influence of alcohol or the placebo.
225
Both drivers under the influence of alcohol and Risperidone showed impairment in reaction
times in the sudden event task compared to the placebo condition. However, the absence of a
difference between alcohol and Risperidone suggests that the impairing effect of Risperidone is
broadly equivalent to that of a legal level of alcohol (i.e. BAC 0.5).
Taken together these results provide empirical data that enable us to provide a judgement of
the effect of risperidone on driving performance. Drivers using risperidone drove with a lateral position
that was comparable to placebo. However the standard deviation of lateral position and reaction time
to sudden events were signifanctly increased and comparable or bigger than those observed after a
blood alcohol concentration of 0.5 mg/ml. The present data thus seems to indicate that drivers under
the influence of risperidone do demonstrate impairments that should be considered of clinical
relevance.
Acknowledgments
This study was conducted within the DRUID research consortium, WP 1.2, granted by the European
Commission, DG-TREN (TREN-05-FP6TR-S07.61320-518404 – DRUID)
References
Louwerens J.W., Gloerich, A.B.M., de Vries, G., Brookhuis, K.A. and O’
Hanlon, J.F. (1987). The
relationship between drivers’blood alcohol concentration (BAC) and actual driving performance during
high speed travel. In P.C. Noordzij and R. Roszbach (Eds.), Proceedings of the 10th International
Conference on Alcohol, Drugs and Traffic Safety (pp. 183-192). Amsterdam: Excerpta Medica.
Meltzer, H.Y. and McGurk, S.R. (1999). The effect of clozapine, risperidone, and olanzapine on
cognitive function in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 25, 233 - 255.
Ramaekers, J. G. (2003). Antidepressants and driver impairment: empirical evidence from a standard
on-the-road-test. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 64(1), 20-29.
Soyka, M., Kagerer, S., Brunnauer, A., Laux, G. and Moller, H.J. (2005). Driving ability in
schizophrenic patients: effects of neuroleptics. International Journal of Psychiatry Clinical Practice,
9(3), 168-174.
Wolschrijn, H., De Gier, J. J. and De Smet, P. A. G. M. (1991). Drugs and Driving: A New
Categorization System for Drugs Affecting Psychomotor Performance. Technical Report. Limburg, The
Netherlands: Institute for Drugs, Safety and Behavior, University of Limburg.
226
Chapter 11: Blood to serum ratios of hypnotics,
opioid and non-opioid analgesics as well as
antipsychotics and amphetamine-like drugs
and their analysis in dried blood spots
Ricarda Jantos, Gisela Skopp
Institute of Legal Medicine and Forensic Medicine, University Hospital, Voss-Str. 2, 69115 Heidelberg,
Germany
Telephone
++49 6221 568920
Fax
++49 6221 561300
E-mail:
[email protected]
227
Abstract
Blood is prevalently used for a toxicology investigation including DUID cases whereas analysis on
pharmacokinetic samples is performed on separated serum or plasma, as a rule. Most drugs,
however, do not equally distribute between blood and plasma. Dried blood spots (DBS) are
increasingly used in drug analysis due to their ease of collection, shipping and storage as well as the
reduced risk of infection. Therefore, the following objectives were pursued: a. determination of drug
concentration in whole blood and corresponding plasma samples to estimate ex vivo blood to plasma
(b/p) ratios, and b. comparison of drug levels in whole blood and corresponding DBS.
Analytes were hydromorphone, morphine, fentanyl and norfentanyl, oxycodone and noroxycodone,
alprazolam, zopiclone, temazepam, THC, 11-OH-THC and THC-COOH, MDMA and MDA, damphetamine as well as risperidone and 9-OH-risperidone. Analysis was performed on whole blood,
plasma and DBS by LC/MS/MS except THC and metabolites, where GC-MS was used. All analytical
methods were validated according to international guidelines for each respective matrix. B/p ratios
were derived from corresponding blood and plasma samples. Bland Altman analysis was used to test
agreement
of
concentrations
determined
from
whole
blood
and
corresponding
DBS.
The mean obtained b/p ratios were: hydromorphone: 1.04, morphine: 1.03, fentanyl: 0.87, norfentanyl:
1.19, oxycodone: 1.48, noroxycodone: 1.73, alprazolam: 0.81, zopiclone: 0.89, temazepam: 0.71,
MDMA: 1.19, MDA: 1.72, d-amphetamine: 0.89, risperidone: 0.65, 9-OH-risperidone: 0.73. Ratios
were close to reliably established ratios published in the current literature as far as available, except
for MDA. For all analytes except zopiclone, the mean ratio of the DBS and blood concentrations and
their relative standard deviations indicated that DBS analysis is as reliable as analysis from whole
blood. Bland-Altman difference plots for the several substances supported this thesis. Zopiclone is
expected to undergo degradation even in DBS.
Dividing the concentrations of the analytes blood by the obtained b/p ratios may give a reasonably
good estimate of the coexisting concentration in plasma, except for MDA. For law enforcement
purposes, it is recommended to consider inherent biological variations in the b/p relationship and to
take the range of individual b/p ratios as a basis. The MDA b/p ratio is recommended to be analyzed in
a separate study after administration of MDA itself. There is sound evidence that the DBS assay has
potential as a precise and inexpensive option for the determination of the investigated analytes in
small blood samples. However, stability of zopiclone in DBS has to be tested and compared to the
stability in whole blood specimens using a stability indicating method.
228
Introduction
Distribution of drugs between whole blood and serum or plasma
Drug analysis in forensic and postmortem toxicology including DUID (driving under the influence of
drugs) cases is usually performed on whole blood whereas serum or plasma is preferably used in
clinical facilities and pharmacological studies. Blood is a complex biological fluid consisting of a
buffered clear fluid containing proteins, fats, solids and suspended cells. The major constituents – the
red cells – can be separated from the clear fluid by centrifugation. If blood is allowed to stand without
the addition of an anti coagulating agent, then red cells will clot and the resultant fluid can be
decanted. If anticoagulants are added, plasma can subsequently be prepared. Serum is in most
respects similar to plasma except that it does not contain soluble factors that lead to blood clotting (1).
The proportion of blood volume that is occupied by red cells is referred to as the hematocrit value (%).
Normally, it averages 48% for men and 38% for women, and ranges from 35–54% in blood from
healthy adults (2). Most drugs are not equally distributed between the sub compartments of blood;
hence, the concentration in serum or plasma may differ from that in whole blood. Blood to plasma
(b/p) concentration ratios may not only vary between different compounds with the same core
structure, but also between the parent drug and corresponding metabolites (3) or depend on the
hematocrit value (4, 5). To know the distribution of drugs into the major sub compartments of blood is
mandatory in order to reliably compare whole blood to plasma or serum levels that have been derived
from controlled pharmacokinetic studies.
Generally, distribution of compounds between whole blood and plasma is determined using in vitro or,
to a much lesser extent, ex vivo procedures (5-7). In the conventional in vitro method, the drug is
incubated with a whole blood specimen at a known hematocrit value – which does not apply to DUID
samples. Following equilibration, an aliquot of the whole blood specimen is put on the side while
plasma is prepared from the remaining sample. It is not known how far off from the true values the in
vitro determined ones are likely to be. In the ex vivo method, the blood sample taken from a drug user
for analysis is portioned, and plasma is prepared from an aliquot of the original specimen. In both
methods, drug concentrations in whole blood and plasma will be measured separately using separate
standards for whole blood and plasma, respectively.
This investigation compares b/p distribution ratios of opioid and non-opioid analgesics, hypnotics and
antipsychotics as well as commonly used illegal drugs using authentic samples from healthy
volunteers conducting DUID experiments. Partners were instructed to collect 5-10 mL blood from the
median cubital vein or the anterior forearm by venipuncture into a vacuum tube or into a syringe and
needle with subsequent transfer to a VacutainerTM or a MonovetteTM containing potassium
oxalate/sodium fluoride as an anticoagulant/preservative (e.g. grey top VacutainerTM, DIN ISO 6710).
Plasma should be prepared by centrifuging the sample at 2000-3000 g for 10-15 minutes at 20-22°C
or at 4°C (labile compounds). Samples should be stored frozen and shipped on dry ice.
229
Dried blood spots
Dried blood spots (DBS) have routinely been used in neonatal metabolic screening for over two
decades, and have recently established themselves as a valuable tool in therapeutic drug monitoring
(8-13). Despite a limited sample size of 10-100 µL blood, analysis of DBS specimens has become
feasible with the advent of increasingly sensitive MS technologies (14). DBS can be stored at room
temperature and shipped by regular mail, in contrast to whole blood or plasma specimens. Use of
DBS is an appropriate method to reduce virus infection risk to a minimum which is a major concern
handling samples of drug users (15, 16). Being readily accessible also in subjects with limited venous
access, such as e.g. injecting drug users, it represents a valuable and less invasive alternative to
taking of a blood sample. The simple sampling can also be performed by non-medical personnel. In
addition, the use of DBS makes labile compounds such as ester type drugs less susceptible to
degradation (15). A blood spot card was designed for collection of DBS in the present investigation.
The face and the back of the card are shown in Figure 1. The 903 specimen collection paper (GE
Healthcare, Dassel, Germany) used for the custom made card is an FDA listed class II medical
device. It is manufactured from 100% pure cotton linters with no wet-strength additives. Both, the
manufacturing and post-printing quality of the paper were checked.
Fig. 1: Blood spot card, face and back of the card
230
Methods
Figure 2 gives an overview on the study design:
Whole blood
Plasma
Blood spot
Fig. 2: Plasma and blood spot specimens are obtained from the corresponding whole blood specimen
for measurement of blood/plasma ratios and to evaluate measurement from DBS
Measurement of drug concentration in whole blood and corresponding plasma samples enables an
estimation of ex vivo b/p ratios. The knowledge of these ratios and of their range is mandatory to
valuably compare drug concentrations measured either in whole blood or plasma.
A comparison of drug levels in whole blood and corresponding DBS allows demonstrating whether
drug measurement from DBS is as accurate as that from the whole blood specimen. DBS analysis will
make handling of toxicological samples much easier.
Extraction and determination of the analytes in plasma, whole blood and DBS
Extraction and determination:
Plasma, blood and DBS were extracted to determine the concentrations of the following analytes:
hydromorphone, morphine, fentanyl, norfentanyl, oxycodone, noroxycodone, alprazolam, temazepam,
zopiclone,
tetrahydrocannabinol
(THC),
11-hydroxy-tetrahydrocannabinol
(11-OH-THC),
tetrahydrocannabinol carboxylic acid (TCH-COOH), 3,4-methylenedioxy-methamphetamine (MDMA),
3,4-methylenedioxyamphetamine,
d-amphetamine,
risperidone
and
9-hydroxy-risperidone
(9-OH-risperidone). DBS were prepared by spotting a 100 µL aliquot of whole blood the custom made
DRUID card (GE Healthcare, Dassel, Germany), which were dried at room temperature for at least
3 h. Then, each sample was packed in a plastic bag together with a desiccant pack and stored at
ambient temperature (20-24°C) whereas whole blood and plasma samples were kept frozen (-20°C)
until analyzed. Before extraction, DBS were cut out completely and transferred into plastic tubes.
Analysis was performed by liquid chromatography/tandem mass spectrometry (LC/MS/MS) following
liquid/liquid extraction except cannabinoids which were determined by GC-MS. Only hydromorphone
was isolated by solid phase extraction. Each sample was extracted twice. Table1 gives an overview
on the extraction procedures and mass spectrometry conditions developed for the different analytes.
231
For quantitation, spiked plasma, whole blood or DBS samples were prepared and analyzed in the
same way, except for the dronabinol samples. For the determination of THC, 11-OH-THC and
THC-COOH 1000 µL plasma were extracted.
Calibration lines were constructed with linear least squares regression using the ratio of the target
analyte peak area to the corresponding internal standard peak area.
Validation: Imprecision, extraction efficiency and bench top stability (24 h) were investigated according
to the FDA Guidance for Industry (17). Carryover was checked as described by Bansal and DeStefano
(18). Ion suppression or enhancement was determined according to Matuszewski et al. (19). The
lower limit of detection (LLOD) and quantitation (LLOQ) was estimated from the calibration curves
according to DIN 32465 at a probability of 95% (20).
Data analysis was done using Microsoft Excel®. Agreement of the whole blood and DBS methods was
further assessed by Bland-Altman plots (21). Therefore, mean difference between the blood and DBS
concentrations was calculated. 95% of the values are required to lie between the limits of agreement,
which were calculated as the mean difference ±1.96xSD (standard deviation).
Materials and instrumentation: Zopiclone was purchased from Rhône Poulenc Rorer (Cologne,
Germany). Risperidone, 9-OH-risperidone and didehydromethylrisperidone were supplied by JanssenCilag
(Neuss,
Germany).
Hydromorphone,
morphine,
fentanyl,
norfentanyl,
oxycodone,
noroxycodone, alprazolam, temazepam, THC, 11-OH-THC, THC-COOH, MDMA, MDA, and
amphetamine as well as their deuterated standards and lorazepam-d4 were obtained from LGC.
Wesel, Germany. High-pressure liquid chromatography (HPLC)-grade acetonitrile and methanol as
well as ethyl acetate ( 99.5 %), toluene ( 99.5 %), isopropanol ( 99.5 %), solid NaOH ( 99 %),
ammonium acetate ( 98 %), acetic acid (100 %) were from Roth (Karlsruhe, Germany).
Isoamylalcohol ( 99 %), dichloromethane ( 99.8 %), ammonium hydroxide (25 %), hydrochloric acid
(25 %), sodium carbonate ( 99.5 %), sodium hydrogen carbonate ( 99.5 %), potassium chloride
99.5 %) und boric acid ( 99.8 %) were supplied by Merck (Darmstadt, Germany). Double distilled
water was obtained from Braun (Melsungen, Germany). Drug-free whole blood and plasma for
preparation of calibration lines and validation experiments were purchased from the local blood bank
of the University Hospital of Heidelberg.
For solid phase extraction of hydromorphone, Bond-Elut C8 1 mL columns were used (Varian,
Darmstadt, Germany).
LC-MS/MS analysis was performed on an API 4000 tandem MS with a TurboIon ionization source
operated in the positive-ion mode (AB Sciex, Darmstadt, Germany). It was interfaced to an HPLC
pump equipped with an autosampler 1100 series, Agilent, Waldbronn, Germany). GC-MS
determination was carried out using a 5973N mass selective detector interfaced with a 6890N GC
system
equipped
with
a
7683
series
injector
(Agilent,
Waldbronn,
Germany).
232
Table 1: Analysis of hydromorphone, morphine, fentanyl, norfentanyl, oxycodone, noroxycodone, alprazolam, zopiclone, temazepam, MDMA, MDA, d-amphetamine,
risperidone and 9-OH-risperidone: extraction, chromatography and mass spectrometry conditions
analyte
adjustment
of
the pH-value for
internal
standard
extracting agent [vol%]
(IS)
extrac-tion
mobile
flow
retention
phase
[µL/min]
time
column
transition
used
transition
for quantitation
IS
286à185
289à185
286à152
289à152
337à188
342à188
233à84
238à84
316à298
322à304
1.11
302à284
305à287
50
5.5-6.5
386, 371,303
389
mL/min
7.2-7.6
371, 474, 459
374
7.65-8.2
371, 473, 488
374
[min]
A:B:CI
[v:v:v]
hydromorphone1
carbonate
hydromorphone-d3
buffer pH 9.0
dichloromethane/
50:10:40
300
1.18
#
isopropanol/
conc. NH3 80:20:2
morphine2
borate
buffer
morphine-d3
ethyl acetate
50:10:40
220
2.00
fentanyl-d5
ethyl acetate
40:12:48
250
1.69
pH 8.5
fentanyl
5 %NH3
norfentanyl
oxycodone
norfentanyl-d5
5 %NH3
oxycodone-d6
noroxycodone
0.25 M
11-OH-THC
acid
acetic
THC-COOH
alprazolam
n-hexane/ethyl
11-OH-THC-d3
9:1
50:10:40
300
acetate
helium
THC-COOH-d3
borate
buffer
alprazolam-d5
borate
borate
toluene/isoamyl alcohol
1.20
#
45:11:44
250
3.70
#
309à205
314à210
40:12:48
300
1.15
#
389à245
325à307
40:12:48
300
3.02
#
301à255
306à260
95:5
buffer
lorazepam-d4
pH 8.5
temazepam
ethyl acetate
THC-d3
pH 8.5
zopiclone
1.36
norxycodone-d6
THC3
#
toluene/isoamyl alcohol
95:5
buffer
temazepam-d5
toluene/isoamyl alcohol
233
pH 8.5
MDMA4
95:5
0.01 M NaOH
MDMA-d5
ethyl acetate
60:8:32
220
2,15
194à163
199à165
MDA4^
MDA-d5
2,08
180à163
180à135
d-amphetamine
amphetamine-d5
1.71
136à91
141à124
411à191
421à201
risperidone
borate
9-OH-
pH 8.5
buffer
didehydromethyl-
ethyl acetate
risperidone
50:10:40
250
2.00
1.90
#
427à207
risperidone
I unless stated otherwise: A: 4 mM ammonium acetate buffer pH 3,2; B:
at three characteristic fragments within their respective retention time windows.
methanol; C: acetonitrile
THC and its metabolites were separated by a temperature gradient.
# Phenomenex Luna C18 2,0 mm x 150 mm, particle size 5 µm, Phenomenex,
4: the organic phase was acidified with 50 µL of methanol HCl (49:1, v:v) prior
Aschaffenburg, Germany
to evaporation to dryness
Agilent Zorbax Eclipse XDB-C8 2,1 mm x 150 mm. particle size 5 µm,
Agilent, Waldbronn, Germany
CP-Sil5 CB 0.2 mm x 12.5 m, film thickness 0.4 µm, Varian. Darmstadt,
Germany
1 solid phase extraction
2 ultrasonication (5 min) was applied following addition of borate buffer and IS
3: analysis was performed using GC-MS in the selected ion monitoring mode.
Identification of the analytes was performed in the single ion monitoring mode
234
Results and discussion
Evaluation
Table 2 gives an overview on the validation results determined in plasma. Additionally, matrix effect,
extraction efficiency and 24 h bench top stability were checked; all values were in an acceptable range
(data not shown). Carryover could not be observed for any analyte. There were no significant
differences between the validation results in plasma, blood and DBS; all parameters were in the same
range as presented in table 2 or better. It could be observed that matrix effects in DBS are of a lesser
extent than in the other media.
Table 2: validation results for the most important substances determined in plasma
analyte
hydromorphone
LLOD
LLOQ
between-run
[ng/mL]
[ng/mL]
precision [%]
0.9
3.1
4 ng/mL:
5.4
morphine
6.2
22.0
within-run precision [%]
linearity
4 ng/mL:
2-20 ng/mL
3.5
r=0.9998
12 ng/mL:
4.1
12 ng/mL:
4.1
50 ng/mL:
8.3
50 ng/mL:
9.0
50-
250 ng/mL:
5.6
250 ng/mL:
5.6
500 ng/mL
r=0.9999
fentanyl
norfentanyl
0.04
0.01
0.16
0.03
0.25 ng/mL:
4.7
0.25 ng/mL:
4.7
0.1-10 ng/mL
6.5 ng/mL:
4.7
6.5 ng/mL:
2.9
r=1.0000
0.25 ng/mL:
3.5
0.25 ng/mL:
2.9
0.1-
2.0 ng/mL:
3.7
2.0 ng/mL:
3.7
4.0 ng/mL
r=1.0000
oxycodone
noroxycodone
zopiclone
1.9
1.5
1.9
6.4
5.7
6.7
10 ng/mL:
6.8
10 ng/mL:
6.4
5-100 ng/mL
45 ng/mL:
4.2
45 ng/mL:
3.0
r=0.9997
10 ng/mL:
3.7
10 ng/mL:
3.7
5-100 ng/mL
45 ng/mL:
2.2
45 ng/mL:
1.9
r=0.9989
20 ng/mL:
5.5
20 ng/mL:
0.4
10-50 ng/mL
r=0.9999
temazepam
12.7
44.1
100 ng/mL:
4.9
100 ng/mL:
4.1
50-
500 ng/mL:
2.5
500 ng/mL:
2.0
500 ng/mL
r=0.9997
alprazolam
0.8
3.0
10.0 ng/mL:
10.0 ng/mL:
5.1
5.7 %
MDMA
0.9
3.1
2.5-50 ng/mL
r=0.9999
50 ng/mL:
3.3
50 ng/mL:
2.5
A:
50-
250 ng/mL:
4.1
250 ng/mL:
2.8
400 ng/mL
r=0.9997
B:
5235
40 ng/mL
r=0.9999
MDA
0.25
0.93
50 ng/mL:
2.4
50 ng/mL:
1.9
A:
5-
150 ng/mL:
6.4
150 ng/mL:
5.4
40 ng/mL
r=0.9992
B:
0.5-
4 ng/mL
r=0.9992
d-amphetamine
0.6
2.3
50 ng/mL:
5.1
50 ng/mL:
2.2
5-40 ng/mL
r=0.9998
risperidone
9-OH-
0.7
1.2
2.5
4.4
risperidone
6.7 ng/mL:
6.3
6.7 ng/mL:
2.9
5-40 ng/mL
19.7 ng/mL:
6.0
19.7 ng/mL:
6.0
r=0.9995
20.4 ng/mL:
6.2
20.4 ng/mL:
4.8
5-60 ng/mL
64.0 ng/mL:
10.4
64.0 ng/mL:
4.4
r=0.9999
Concentrations of the analytes in blood, plasma and DBS
Table 8 in the appendix provides complete information on the results of the analysis of all drugs.
Drugs where the sample size with concentrations >LLOQ did not exceed n=7 were not considered in
this part of the document, but results are also shown in table 8 in the appendix. For the dronabinol
samples, analysis of the blood specimens is not yet completed. Due to a sample volume of 100 µL for
DBS instead of 1000 µL for plasma and blood, the extraction method presented in table 1 turned out to
be not applicable to DBS samples without modifications. Method development for DBS extraction is
planned to be completed in May 2011, the results of the validation procedures and the determination
of the assays of THC, 11-OH-THC and THC-COOH in the dronabinol samples might be expected in
August 2011.
Blood/plasma ratios
Table 3 gives an overview on the b/p ratios and their relative standard deviations (RSD) determined in
the investigations for the DRUID project and reference values (if available).
Table 3: b/p ratios obtained in the presented studies and published values; n.a.: not available, -: no
reference
analyte
b/p ratio; range
b/p ratio; mean
reported values
reference
± RSD [%]
hydromorphone
0.91-1.22
1.04 ± 8.11%
1.35
(22)
morphine
0.96-1.07
1.03 ± 3.59%
1.02
(23)
fentanyl
0.62-1.02
0.87 ± 13.9%
1.0
(24)
norfentanyl
1.03-1.29
1.19± 6.81%
n.a.
-
oxycodone
1.29-1.76
1.48 ± 8.19%
n.a.
-
noroxycodone
1.28-2.09
1.73 ± 13.5%
n.a.
236
alprazolam
0.73-0.90
0.81 ± 5.84%
0.80
(25)
zopiclone
0.66-1.29
0.89 ± 16.1%
1.0
(26)
temazepam
0.58-0.87
0.71± 12.0%
0.53
(27)
MDMA
0.98-1.46
1.19 ± 8.03%
1.16
(28)
MDA
1.00-3.02
1.72 ± 31.1%
1.27
(28)
amphetamine
0.65-1.14
0.89 ± 10.94 %
0.91
(29)
risperidone
0.56-0.73
0.65 ± 7.52%
0.67
(30)
9-OH-risperidone
0.61-0.91
0.73 ± 12.3%
n.a.
-
Distribution ratios are generally derived from in vitro partition experiments where plasma water,
plasma proteins and red blood cells are pooled. Some caution is advisable using these data. When
spiked blood is diluted with autologous plasma water, erythrocytes may discharge the compound over
proportionally compared to plasma proteins. Concentration ratios between blood and plasma may vary
from 0.5 to 2.0 such as e.g. for phenytoin and maprotiline, respectively.
In table 3, b/p ratios reported in the bold typed references are not comparable to those obtained from
the studies performed in the DRUID project.
For hydromorphone, the b/p ratio obtained in our study differs significantly from the reference value.
Parab et al. give neither an information on the determination of the b/p ratio nor provide a reference for
the published factor (22).
Also, the b/p ratio of fentanyl significantly differs from the published value. Bower and Hull (24)
investigated the distribution of fentanyl between plasma and packed erythrocytes. The obtained ratio
was later cited as a b/p ratio. Unlike packed erythrocytes, whole blood also contains plasma proteins,
which has not been considered referring the erythrocyte/plasma ratio as a b/p ratio. Thus, the b/p ratio
obtained in our study cannot be compared to the value reported by Bower and Hull.
Bramness et al. published a b/p ratio of 1.0 for zopiclone in the context of a fatal zopiclone intoxication
(26). It is not apparent if the ratio has been determined in post-mortem blood; also, no particular
reference is provided. Whenever the b/p ratio has been determined from post-mortem blood it is
impossible to compare this value to those obtained in the present study. It is well known that
significant changes occur in post-mortem blood, due to autolysis and hemolysis. Especially hemolysis
may cause differences of the b/p ratio compared to blood samples collected from living subjects.
Hence, a comparison of the b/p ratios obtained in the DRUID study with the reference values is not
considered to be reasonable.
Osselton et al. investigated the b/p ratio of temazepam on blood samples of volunteers receiving drug
therapy (27). The authors give no information on the number of samples investigated. In addition, all
participants suffered from drug addiction which leads to hepatic or renal insufficiency affecting the
plasma or erythrocyte fraction of the blood or the amount and quality of plasma proteins.
The b/p ratios of morphine, alprazolam, MDMA and amphetamine and risperidone were in accordance
with the reported values and showed acceptable RSD. For 9-OH-risperidone, the slightly higher b/p
ratio in contrast to risperidone appears to be in line with its increased hydrophilic properties due to the
hydroxyl group and its decreased plasma protein binding compared to risperidone. Despite of the
smaller concentration range, the amphetamine b/p results are in line with those determined in the VTI
study from Sweden. Table 4 shows the analytical data of both studies:
237
Table 4: comparison of TNO and VTI amphetamine studies carried out during the DRUID project
Study of TNO,
Study of VTI,
The Netherlands
Sweden
positive findings in blood
n=29 (100%)
n=37 (62%)
positive findings in plasma
n=29 (100%)
n=37 (62%)
Concentration range, blood
10.75-40.65
0-123.5
concentration range, plasma
12.20-41.13
0-112.0
mean +/- SD, blood
20.61 +/- 7.08
21.78 +/- 29.60
mean +/- SD, plasma
23.21 +/- 7.60
22.92 +/- 29.40
0.64-1.14
0.65-1.10
0.89 +/- 11.24 %
0.91 +/- 13.49 %
b/p, range
b/p mean +/- RSD
MDA b/p ratio from samples of the study from RugPsy differed significantly from the value determined
in a previous study performed in Maastricht. Results from both studies are summarized in table 5:
Table 5: comparison of the b/p ratios of MDA determined in two MDMA studies carried out during
DRUID project
Study of Maastricht
Study of RugPsy, Groningen
positive findings in blood
n=15 (24%)
n=29 (78%)
positive findings in plasma
n=27 (43%)
n=30 (79%)
concentration range, blood
5.0-10.4
1.1-24.4
concentration range, plasma
5.7-16.3
1.0-13.5
mean +/- SD, blood
7.5 +/- 1.4
9.4 +/- 6.4
mean +/- SD, plasma
8.1 +/- 2.4
5.1 +/- 3.3
b/p, range
1.01-1.77
1.00-3.02
1.27 +/- 15.8 %
1.72 +/- 31.1 %
b/p mean +/- RSD
More positive findings were obtained from samples collected during the RugPsy study, and the
particular concentration range was considerably wider compared to samples obtained from the
Maastricht study. The low concentrations of MDA probably affect b/p ratios leading to relatively high
RSD. The low concentrations are likely to explain the differences which could not be observed for
MDMA. In both studies, MDA was determined as a metabolite of MDMA, which can be detected in a
concentration of 5-10 % of the MDMA concentration (31). MDA itself is used as an amphetamine-like
drug and is expected to lead to higher blood levels than after administration of MDMA. The b/p ratio
should be investigated after administration of MDA in dosages near to those of recreational use.
In general it has to be noted that the obtained b/p ratios cannot be applied without restriction to overtherapeutic or toxic blood concentrations since all participants of the particular studies received
therapeutic dosages or, in case of drug consumption, only moderate amounts.
238
Agreement of the determination in blood and DBS
With respect to the advantages mentioned in the introduction, DBS may be a suitable method for
roadside blood sampling in case of suspicion of DUID. Before use of DBS as a reliable method can be
realized, it must be shown that DBS analysis is able to provide results that are as reliable as those
using whole blood samples. Therefore, blood and DBS results of the determination of the drugs
investigated in the presented studies were compared using the DBS/blood ratio (DBS/b) and BlandAltman analysis. Accordingly, the respective mean of the corresponding results determined with the
two different methods were plotted on the x-axis and their difference on the y-axis. In contrast to a
scatter plot of blood and corresponding DBS concentrations, the Bland-Altman difference plot shows
the distribution of the differences over the complete concentration range. The mean of the differences
indicates over- or underestimation by one of the two methods. Besides the mean of the differences,
the 95 % limits of agreement were calculated as the mean difference ±1.96xSD. These values define
the range within which most differences between measurements by the two methods will lie. In case of
normal distribution, 95 % of the differences are expected to lie within these limits (21).
Ideally, the mean DBS/b ratio should be equal to 1.00, which means that results from whole blood and
DBS analysis do not differ. All analytes and their corresponding DBS/b ratios including respective RSD
are summarized in table 6:
Table 6: summary of the DBS/b ratios
analyte
n
DBS/b
DBS/b
range
mean
RSD [%]
hydromorphone
15
0.86-1.08
0.99
6.36
morphine
7
0.95-1.02
0.99
2.22
fentanyl
13
0.90-1.09
1.00
8.56
norfentanyl
13
0.84-1.05
0.97
5.94
oxycodone
12
0.98-1.10
1.02
3.52
noroxycodone
12
0.89-1.06
1.00
4.32
alprazolam
22
0.92-1.25
1.02
6.71
zopiclone
45
0.63-1.22
0.86
15.86
temazepam
9
0.85-1.11
0.97
7.68
MDMA
35
0.97-1.09
1.01
2.46
MDA
30
0.85-1.09
1.02
5.11
d-amphetamine
29
0.94-1.15
1.05
5.23
risperidone
10
0.86-0.97
0.93
3.51
9-OH-risperidone
14
0.91-1.03
0.97
4.56
For all samples containing opioid type drugs the estimated DBS/b ratios were in an acceptable range
and showed a small range of variation. Also, ratios and RSD for the benzodiazepine-type drugs
alprazolam and temazepam as well as for the amphetamine-like drugs MDMA, MDA and
d-amphetamine account for equality of analysis between the two media.
239
For zopiclone, a DBS/b ratio of 0.86 indicates an overestimation of the results from blood compared to
DBS. This overestimation is supported by the mean difference calculated for the Bland-Altman
difference plot of zopiclone (figure 3).
zopiclone: Bland-Altman difference plot
15,0
difference (blood-DBS) [ng/mL]
13,0
11,0
9,0
7,0
5,0
3,0
1,0
-1,0
-3,0
-5,0
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
50
mean [(blood+DBS)/2] [ng/mL]
Figure 3: Bland-Altman difference plot for zopiclone. The solid line illustrates the mean difference of
3.99 ng/mL, the dotted lines indicate the limits of agreement set to 1.96xSD (-3.62 and 11.59 ng/mL).
Whole blood samples were stored at -20°C until analysis, whereas DBS were kept at ambient
temperature. With respect to the different temperatures of storage, degradation of zopiclone to
2-amino-5-chloropyridine might have occurred which has recently been published for whole blood
samples by Nilsson et al (32). Currently, stability investigations are carried out to compare the
degradation in whole blood and DBS and to draw a conclusion concerning the better sample matrix
and the best conditions for storage. Final results can be expected in September 2011. First
investigations from residual samples of the P35 study indicate zopiclone degradation in whole blood
specimen of 21.8 % after 30 days of storage at -20 %. In DBS stored under the same conditions the
initial zopiclone concentration was decreased by only 10.0 %.
The mean DBS/b ratio of risperidone was not as close to 1.00 as the respective ratios of the other
analytes (except zopiclone). The risperidone Bland-Altman difference plot (figure 4) may, however, be
useful to evaluate whether determination in DBS is equivalent to that in whole blood:
240
Figure 4: Bland-Altman difference plot for risperidone. The solid line illustrates the mean difference of
0.83 ng/mL, the dotted lines indicate the limits of agreement set to 1.96xSD (-0.67 and 2.32 ng/mL).
risperidone: Bland-Altman difference plot
difference (blood-DBS) [ng/mL]
2,50
2,00
1,50
1,00
0,50
0,00
-0,50
-1,00
0
5
10
15
20
25
mean [(blood+DBS)/2] [ng/mL]
The Bland-Altman difference plot of risperidone clearly indicates that the methods of determination of
risperidone in either blood of DBS are comparable. None of the measured quantities was outside the
limits of agreement. A mean difference of 0.83 ng/mL is quite low, and only accounts for 7.4 % in
relation to a mean blood concentration of risperidone of 11.1 ng/mL. Overall, both methods can be
regarded as equivalent.
MDMA could be quantified in DBS as reliable as in whole blood specimens which was already evident
from the deliverable of July 2010 (29). The results obtained in the RugPsy study confirmed again
equality of both methods. Moreover, equivalence of the methods could be proven for MDA: The DBS/b
ratio of 1.02 is very close to 1.00; a very low coefficient of variation of 5.11 % and a very small mean
difference between both methods could be estimated from the results. Figure 5 shows the BlandAltman difference plot of MDA:
241
Figure 5: Bland-Altman difference plot for MDA. The solid line illustrates the mean difference of
0.02 ng/mL, the dotted lines indicate the limits of agreement set to 1.96xSD (-1.36 and 1.40 ng/mL).
MDA: Bland-Altman difference plot
3,5
3,0
2,5
difference (blood-DBS) [ng/mL]
2,0
1,5
1,0
0,5
0,0
-0,5
-1,0
-1,5
-2,0
0,0
5,0
10,0
15,0
20,0
25,0
mean [(blood+DBS)/2] [ng/mL]
All values except for a single one are within the limits of agreement. No trend of the differences
between the results obtained from either blood or DBS values over the whole concentration range
could be observed. Especially the very small mean difference leaves no doubt that analysis of MDA
from DBS is as reliable as from whole blood.
In addition to the DBS/b ratios presented in table 6, table 7 gives the results of the Bland-Altman
analyses for all analytes:
242
Table 7: Summary of results of the Bland-Altman difference plots
analyte
mean
difference
blood-DBS
mean-1.96xSD
mean +1.96xSD
mean
[ng/mL]
[ng/mL]
difference/mean
[ng/mL]
blood
concentration
[%]
hydromorphone
0.14
-0.90
1.17
1.53
morphine
2.12
-8.30
12.53
0.88
fentanyl
-0.03
-0.20
0.14
-2.16
norfentanyl
-0.006
-0.068
-0.055
-1.34
oxycodone
-1.24
-4.46
1.98
-2.07
noroxycodone
0.27
-3.26
3.80
0.63
alprazolam
0.07
-1.76
1.90
1.08
zopiclone
3.99
-3.62
11.60
15.30
temazepam
2.23
-16.19
20.66
2.71
MDMA
-3.55
-14.34
7.25
-1.94
MDA
0.02
-1.36
1.40
0.17
d-amphetamine
-1.03
-3.32
1.25
-5.01
risperidone
0.83
-0.67
2.32
7.44
9-OH-risperidone
0.64
-1.13
2.40
4.15
Obviously, for no analyte except zopiclone the mean difference exceeded ±10 % of the mean blood
concentration. Therefore, the DBS and whole blood methods for the analytes investigated during the
DRUID project can be regarded to be equivalent.
Conclusion
The b/p ratios of morphine, alprazolam, MDMA, amphetamine and risperidone obtained in the present
studies closely fit to reported values. For hydromorphone, fentanyl, zopiclone and temazepam the
calculated b/p ratios cannot be compared to published data, since some publications either provide no
detailed information on the investigation or on the samples size. Some of the values were cited as b/p
ratios but have been determined from other media such as packed erythrocytes. In the cases of
oxycodone, noroxycodone and norfentanyl no reference data are currently available. The b/p ratio of
MDA has assessed once again. The small concentrations resulting after administration of MDMA were
close to the limit of quantification resulting in relatively high coefficients of variation.
The DBS assay has potential as a precise and inexpensive option for the determination of several
analytes in small blood samples. The DBS/b ratios were very close to 1.00, and the relative standard
deviations
8.56 %. Measures of under-/overestimation were provided by Bland-Altman difference
plots, and 95 % of all differences between the concentrations determined from either whole blood or
DBS were within the limits of agreement. Also, differences were uniformly distributed across the
concentration range. Except zopiclone, which is very sensitive to degradation, all substances
243
investigated in the presented studies could be determined in DBS as reliable as in whole blood
specimens. A zopiclone stability study in blood and DBS samples is running to compare the extent of
degradation in both media: then, conclusions can be drawn concerning the optimum sample material
and optimum temperature for storage.
244
Appendix: Table 8: Summary of plasma, blood and DBS concentrations of all analytes including major metabolites and their b/p and DBS/b ratios
analyte
n
range
mean
media
range
mean
media
range
mean
media
range
mean
range
mean
remark
plasma
plasm
n
blood
blood
n
DBS
DBS
n DBS
b/p
b/p
DBS/b
DBS/
s
[ng/mL]
a
plasm
[ng/mL]
[ng/mL
blood
[ng/mL]
ng/mL]
[ng/m
[ng/mL
a
]
[ng/m
]
[ng/m
b
L]
L]
L]
hydromorphone
16
2.4-19.7
8.4
6.3
2.3-20.2
8.8
7.0
2.4-19.3
8.7
7.6
0.91-
1.04
1.22
morphine
7
85.7-468.9
232.7
206.7
81.4-
240.2
215.7
479.1
fentanyl
13
0.20-8.67
1.53
0.71
0.18-6.10
82.2-
238.1
209.2
472.6
1.24
0.52
0.18-6.27
0.96-
0.49
0.62-
1.03
13
0.07-1.82
0.40
0.17
0.08-2.21
0.46
0.21
0.07-2.28
0.47
0.21
1.03-
0.87
12
11.6-89.8
41.6
33.4
17.8-
59.9
53.7
122.6
noroxycodone
12
11.0-71.1
26.4
19.5
20.6-91.2
19.3-
61.2
57.1
123.9
42.9
33.6
20.4-86.5
1.29-
1.19
33.8
1.28-
1.48
6
0.30-0.84
0.53
0.45
0.09-0.47
0.26
0.21
0.17-0.48
0.31
0.24
0.50-
1.73
6
0.58-2.35
1.17
0.91
0.24-2.41
1.01
0.82
0.26-2.39
1.02
0.82
0.95-
0.55
2
75.4-322.8
204.1
-
93.4382.6
211.0
-
90.9389.8
240.4
-
1.151.24
0.84-
0.97
2
0.98-
1.02
0.89-
1.00
3
0.99-
1.07
1
1.02
1, 4
1.13
0.98
1.03
tramadol
1.00
1.06
0.63
norbuprenorphine
0.90-
1.10
2.09
buprenorphine
0.99
1.05
1.76
42.1
0.95-
1.19
1.29
oxycodone
1
1.02
1.02
norfentanyl
0.99
1.08
1.07
1.26
0.86-
0.981.09
1.19
0.97-
1.00
1.02
245
O-
2
11.9-69.9
40.9
-
16.7-65.5
41.1
-
16.0-67.3
41.7
-
desmethyltramadol
N-
0.94-
1.17
1.40
2
desmethyltramadol
148.1-
161.6
-
175.1
190.7-
201.4
-
212.1
195.2-
201.1
-
206.9
1.21-
0.96-
0.99
5
1.00
5
1.03
1.25
1.29
0.921.09
tilidine
1
-
2.16
-
-
2.38
-
-
2.21
-
-
1.10
-
0.93
nortilidine
1
-
64.5
-
-
70.3
-
-
70.6
-
-
1.09
-
1.00
6
bisnortilidine
1
-
133.8
-
-
204.1
-
-
205.4
-
-
1.53
-
1.01
6
THC
31
0.8-92.0
6.4
3.2
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
7
0.5-25.0
3.6
2.5
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
7
3.0-175.0
38.5
29.0
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
7
3.11-51.3
6.7
8.6
3.7-20.7
6.6
5.8
4.2-20.1
6.9
6.0
0.73-
0.81
0.92-
1.02
8
0.86
9
10
2
11-OH-THC
31
2
THC-COOH
31
2
alprazolam
52
0.90
zopiclone P35
45
11.9-50.4
29.8
29.6
10.1-49.1
26.1
24.9
4.7-39.8
22.1
21.5
0.66-
1.25
0.89
1.29
0.631.22
zopiclone P31
8
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
diazepam
2
215.9-
270.7
-
200.6-
232.2
-
216.0-
240.6
-
0.81-
0.87
1.00-
1.04
325.5
nordiazepam
2
184.5-
263.7
218.3
-
252.2
temazepam
10
114.6-
16
4.6-430.0
200.5
-
215.6
198.6
209.8
271.2
oxazepam
185.4-
265.2
82.5-
44.6
12.1-
233.2
-
288.8
141.3
154.2
191.7
103.0
177.5-
0.93
70.2-
28.3
7.4-356.8
0.93
1.00
139.0
155.5
184.6
79.0
0.86-
1.08
0.58-
20.8
0.40-
1.15
11
0.98
11
0.92
11, 12
1.34
0.71
0.87
79.7
0.96-
0.851.11
0.93
0.61-
246
355.7
midazolam
6
13.8-46.4
25.1
24.3
2.65
10.3-27.6
16.9
15.7
7.3-35.1
18.8
17.7
0.60-
1.16
0.70
0.89
0.71-
1.06
1.35
clonazepam
1
-
18.8
-
-
14.4
-
-
15.4
-
-
0.77
-
1.07
desalkylflurazepam
3
48.8-60.3
53.0
-
25.2-30.0
28.1
-
29.8-42.1
36.8
-
0.42-
0.54
1.18-
1.30
13, 14
13, 15
0.60
hydroxyethylflurazep
1.45
2
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
4
2.2-7.7
4.6
4.2
1.0-4.5
2.6
2.5
1.6-4.2
2.5
2.1
0.46-
0.55
0.71-
1.10
am
lormetazepam
0.62
nitrazepam
2
57.5-58.0
57.8
-
45.3-50.4
47.9
-
41.9-54.4
48.2
-
0.79-
1.56
0.83
0.87
zolpidem
4
15.6-54.1
33.4
31.8
19.5-54.7
33.2
29.4
23.9-75.6
43.8
37.8
0.78-
39
5.5-388.6
157.8
145.7
7.0-444.3
180.5
167.2
7.0-444.5
186.4
173.7
0.98-
1.09
39
0.97-13.5
5.1
5.2
1.12-24.4
9.4
9.6
0.54-24.1
8.4
7.9
1.00-
1.19
29
12.20-
23.21
21.60
41.13
risperidone
16
1.7-36.8
10.75-
20.61
19.60
40.65
16.0
9.8
10.90-
21.65
20.35
43.90
4.3-22.7
11.1
6.6
1.2-20.8
0.65-
1.72
6.2
0.56-
0.89
16
4.9-46.5
21.3
22.2
4.1-29.8
15.3
15.5
4.2-30.8
14.7
14.7
0.610.91
1:
one sample < LLOQ
1.01
16
0.85-
1.02
17
0.94-
1.05
1.15
0.65
0.73
9-OH-risperidone
0.97-
1.09
1.14
9.5
1.28
1.09
3.02
d-amphetamine
1.141.38
1.46
MDA
1.00
10.8
1.41
MDMA
0.92-
0.86-
0.93
18
0.97
19
0.97
0.73
0.911.03
2:
as a metabolite of fentanyl
4:
as a metabolite of buprenorphine
3:
as a metabolite of oxycodone
5:
as a metabolite of tramadol
247
6:
as a metabolite of tilidine
17:
7:
DBS analysis still in progress; plasma
plasma:
as an active metabolite of
37
samples,
29>LLOQ;
MDMA;
blood:
38
samples>LLOQ: THC: 178, 11-OH-THC: 193,
samples, 30>LLOQ; DBS: 38 samples, 34>LLOQ
THC-COOH: 200
18:
8:
blood: 10 samples >LLOQ
plasma: 42>LLOQ; blood: 33 samples,
19:
24>LLOQ; DBS: 33 samples, 22>LLOQ
plasma and DBS: 11 samples >LLOQ,
active metabolite of risperidone; in each
media 14 samples >LLOQ
-:
9:
not
applicable
suspected degradation of zopiclone in
DBS; stability study to compare the degradation
in whole blood and DBS is still running
10:
concentrations in the P31 study were too
low to provide reliable results
11:
in 2 samples as a metabolite of diazepam
12:
in
8
samples
as
a
metabolite
of
temazepam; wide range of concentrations, but
not
normally
distributed
(compare
great
discrepancies between the mean and the median
concentrations in the three different media)
13:
active metabolite of flurazepam
14:
in one samples accidental detection
although not the planned medication in the study
15:
only detectable in traces
16:
plasma:
37
samples,
34>LLOQ;
blood+DBS: 38 samples, 35>LLOQ
248
References
1.
J. Chamberlain. The analysis of drugs in biological fluids. CRC Press, Boca Raton, Florida,
USA, (1995).
2.
C. Geigy. Geigy Scientific Tables. Ciba Geigy Limited, Basle, Switzerland, (1997).
3.
K. P. Maguire, G. D. Burrows, T. R. Norman and B. A. Scoggins. Blood/plasma distribution
ratios of psychotropic drugs. Clin Chem 26: 1624-1625 (1980).
4.
G. Skopp, L. Pötsch, B. Ganßmann, R. Aderjan and R. Mattern. A preliminary study on the
distribution of morphine and its glucuronides in the subcompartments of blood. J. Anal. Toxicol. 22:
261-264 (1998).
5.
G. Skopp, K. Klinder, L. Poetsch, G. Zimmer, R. Lutz, R. Aderjan and R. Mattern. Postmortem
distribution of dihydrocodeine and metabolites in a fatal case of dihydrocodeine intoxication. Forensic
Sci. Int. 95: 99-107 (1998).
6.
A. W. Jones and H. Larsson. Distribution of diazepam and nordiazepam between plasma and
whole blood and the influence of hematocrit. Ther. Drug Monit. 26: 380-385 (2004).
7.
C. Giroud, A. Ménétrey, M. Augsburger, T. Buclin, P. Sanchez-Mazas and P. Mangin.
9-
THC, 11-OH- 9-THC and 9-THCCOOH plasma or serum to whole blood concentrations distribution
ratios in blood samples taken from living and dead people. Forensic Sci. Int. 123: 159-164 (2001).
8.
D. H. Chace, T. A. Kalas and E. W. Naylor. Use of tandem mass spectrometry for multianalyte
screening of dried blood spot specimens from newborns. Clin. Chem. 49: 1797-1817 (2003).
9.
P. M. Edelbroek, J. v. d. Heijden and L. M. L. Stolk. Dried blood spot methods in therapeutic
drug monitoring: methods, assays, and pitfalls. Ther. Drug Monit. 31: 327-336 (2009).
10.
R. Guthrie and A. Susi. A simple phenylalanine method for detecting phenylketonuria in large
populations of human infants. Pediatrics 32: 338-343 (1963).
11.
J. V. Mei, J. R. Alexander, B. W. Adam and W. H. Hannon. Use of filter paper for the collection
and analysis of human whole blood specimens. J. Nutr. 131: 1631S-1636S (2001).
12.
S. P. Parker and W. D. Cubitt. The use of dried blood spot sample in epidemiological studies.
J. Clin. Pathol. 52: 633-639 (1999).
13.
N. Spooner, R. Las and M. Barfield. Dried blood spots as a sample collection technique for the
determination of pharmacokinetics in clinical studies: considerations for the validation of a quantitative
bioanalytical method. Anal. Chem. 81: 1557-1563 (2009).
14.
W. Li and F. L. S. Tse. Dried blood spot sampling in combination with LC-MS/MS for
quantitative analysis of small molecules. Biomed. Chromatogr. 24: 49-65 (2010).
15.
G. Skopp and L. Poetsch. Nachweis von Cocain in Blutspots. Ach. Kriminol. 207: 81-88
(2001).
16.
G. Skopp. Preanalytic aspects in postmortem toxicology. Forensic Sci. Int. 142: 75-100 (2004).
17.
FDA Guidance for Industry: Bioanalytical Method Validation. U.S. Department of Health and
Human Services. Center for Drug Evaluation and Research. Food and Drug Administration, 2001.
18.
S. Bansal and A. DeStefano. Key elements of bioanalytical method validation for small
molecules. The AAPS Journal 9: E109-E114 (2007).
249
19.
B. K. Matuszewski, M. L. Constanzer and C. M. Chavez-Eng. Strategies for the assessment of
matrix effect in quantitative bioanalytical methods based on HPLC-MS/MS. Anal. Chem. 75: 30193030 (2003).
20.
Deutsches Institut für Normung. DIN 32645. Chemische Analytik - Nachweis- Erfassungs- und
Bestimmungsgrenze unter Wiederholbedingungen - Begriffe, Verfahren, Auswertung. Beuth, Berlin,
2008.
21.
J. M. Bland and D. G. Altman. Measuring agreement in method comparison studies. Stat.
Methods Med. Res. 8: 135-160 (1999).
22.
P. V. Parab, W. A. Ritschel, D. E. Coyle, R. V. Gregg and D. D. Denson. Pharmacokinetics of
hydromorphone after intravenous, peroral and rectal administration to human subjects. Biopharm.
Drug Dispos. 9: 187-199 (1988).
23.
C. W. Hand and R. A. Moore. Comparison of whole blood and plasma morphine. J. Anal.
Toxicol. 12: 234-235 (1988).
24.
S. Bower and C. J. Hull. Comparative pharmacokinetics of fentanyl and alfentanyl. Br. J.
Anaesth. 54: 871-877 (1982).
25.
D. J. Crouch, D. E. Rollins, D. V. Canfield, D. M. Andrenyak and J. E. Schultis. Quantitation of
alprazolam and
-hydroxyalprazolam in human plasma using liquid chromatography electrospray
ionization MS-MS. J. Anal. Toxicol. 23: 479-485 (1999).
26.
J. G. Bramness, M. Arnestad, R. Karinen and T. Hilberg. Fatal overdose of zopiclone in an
elderly woman with bronchogenic carcinoma. J. Forensic Sci. 46: 1247-1249 (2001).
27.
M. D. Osselton, M. D. Hammond and A. C. Moffat. Distribution of drugs and toxic chemicals in
blood. J. Forens. Sci. Soc. 20: 187-193 (1980).
28.
R. Garcia Boy, J. Henseler, J. G. Ramaekers, R. Mattern and G. Skopp. A comparison
between experimental and authentic blood/serum ratios of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine and
3,4-methylenedioxyamphetamine. J. Anal. Toxicol. 33: 283-286 (2009).
29.
G. Skopp and R. Jantos. Blood to plasma ratios of amphetamine and MDMA and analysis of
dried blood spots. DRUID project deliverable. (2010).
30.
G. Mannens, W. Meuldermans, E. Snoeck and J. Heykants. Plasma protein binding of
risperidone and its distribution in blood. Psychopharmacology 114: 566-572 (1994).
31.
H. J. Hemlin, K. Bracher, D. Bourquin, D. Vonlanthen and R. Brenneisen. Analysis of 3,4-
methylendioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) and its metabolites in plasma and urine by HPLC-DAD and
GC-MS. J. Anal. Toxicol. 20: 432-440 (1996).
32.
G. H. Nilsson, F. C. Kugelberg, R. Kronstrand and J. Ahlner. Stability tests of zopiclone in
whole blood. Forensic Sci. Int. 200: 130-135 (2010).
250