Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/51158374 The Relationship Between Adult Attachment Style and Therapeutic Alliance in Individual Psychotherapy: A Meta-Analytic Review Article in Psychotherapy Theory Research Practice Training · May 2011 DOI: 10.1037/a0022425 · Source: PubMed CITATIONS READS 157 4,540 2 authors: Marc Diener Joel Monroe Long Island University FirstHealth Moore Regional Hospital 23 PUBLICATIONS 703 CITATIONS 6 PUBLICATIONS 191 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects: Psychotherapy Process Research View project All content following this page was uploaded by Joel Monroe on 01 April 2016. The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. SEE PROFILE Psychotherapy 2011, Vol. 48, No. 3, 237–248 © 2011 American Psychological Association 0033-3204/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0022425 The Relationship Between Adult Attachment Style and Therapeutic Alliance in Individual Psychotherapy: A Meta-Analytic Review Marc J. Diener and Joel M. Monroe Argosy University The present study examined the relationship between adult attachment style and therapeutic alliance in individual psychotherapy. Search procedures yielded 17 independent samples (total N ⫽ 886, average n ⫽ 52, standard deviation ⫽ 24) for inclusion in the meta-analysis. Results indicated that greater attachment security was associated with stronger therapeutic alliances, whereas greater attachment insecurity was associated with weaker therapeutic alliances, with an overall weighted effect size of r ⫽ .17, p ⬍ .001 (95% confidence interval ⫽ .10 –.23). Publication bias analyses did not indicate any cause for concern regarding the results. The data were not demonstrably heterogeneous (Q ⫽ 6.10, df ⫽ 16, p ⫽ .99), and all between-study moderator analyses were nonsignificant (p values ⬎ .10) with the exception of the source of alliance ratings; results indicated that patient-rated alliance demonstrated a significantly larger relationship with attachment compared with therapist-rated alliance (Qbetween ⫽ 3.95, df ⫽ 1, p ⫽ .047). Implications for clinical practice and future research are discussed. Keywords: meta-analysis, attachment, alliance, psychotherapy, psychodynamic theory Supplemental materials: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0022425.supp and alliance into three categories: (a) security and alliance, (b) avoidance and alliance, and (c) anxiety and alliance. Regarding the relationship between self-reported adult attachment security and alliance, Smith et al. (2010) noted that results supported a mediumsized relationship in which greater security predicted greater alliance. Smith et al. (2010) maintained that results for the relationship between self-reported adult attachment avoidance and alliance were inconsistent, although overall findings did not support such a relationship. They also argued that research on the relationship between self-reported adult attachment anxiety and alliance was inconsistent, with overall results failing to support a significant relationship. The review by Smith et al. (2010), however, suffers from at least four important limitations which significantly restrict the validity of their conclusions. First, although Smith et al. (2010) report effect sizes from several of the studies they reviewed, no attempt was made to meta-analytically synthesize the results across studies. Smith et al. (2010) maintained that methodological differences between the studies precluded direct comparisons between the findings. However, this issue is an empirical rather than a theoretical one; yet, Smith et al. (2010) do not present sufficient empirical data to justify their assertion. Second, and related to the first issue, no moderator analyses were presented to examine the relationship between methodological or substantive features of individual studies and their associated effect sizes. Although Smith et al. (2010) did consider the potential impact of several variables (e.g., who rated the alliance), their analysis proceeded in a primarily narrative manner without comparison of the various effect sizes using formal statistical analyses such as categorical subgroup analyses or metaregression. Third, Smith et al. (2010) included several studies in their review which detail results of only primary regression analyses, despite the fact that such multivariate analyses render the findings incompatible with other studies. Fourth, the review by Smith et al. (2010) neglected to include at least four The connection between individual differences in adults’ relatively enduring patterns of interaction in close, personal relationships (Hazan & Shaver, 2007) and the quality of the patient– therapist relationship in psychotherapy has received considerable attention in the last several decades of theoretical exposition, clinical practice, and research (e.g., Ackerman et al., 2001; Bowlby, 1988; Meyer & Pilkonis, 2001, 2002; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007; Smith, Msefti, & Golding, 2010). Nevertheless, the Division 39 Task Force on Empirically Supported Therapy Relationships concluded that insufficient evidence existed to clearly support the utility of tailoring the therapy relationship to patient attachment style (Ackerman et al., 2001). In a more recent systematic review, Smith et al. (2010) divided research findings that examined the relationship between self-reported adult attachment This article was published Online First May 23, 2011. Marc J. Diener and Joel M. Monroe, American School of Professional Psychology, Argosy University. Earlier versions of this study were presented at the annual meeting of the Division of Psychoanalysis (39) of the American Psychological Association, New York, April 2008, and the International Association for Relationship Research, Herzliya, Israel, July, 2010. We thank Dr. Joel Weinberger for his helpful suggestions on earlier versions of this article. We are grateful to Dr. Rosemarie Vala Stewart for her assistance in reviewing the literature for relevant studies and to Dr. Michael Borenstein for his guidance on a number of statistical issues. Finally, we thank Drs. Diane Arnkoff, Gillian Hardy, Dennis Kivlighan, Brent Mallinckrodt, Margaret Parish, and Eric Sauer for providing additional information on the studies included in the analyses. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Marc J. Diener, PhD, American School of Professional Psychology, Clinical Psychology Program, Argosy University, Washington, DC, 1550 Wilson Boulevard, Suite 600, Arlington, VA 22209. E-mail: [email protected] .edu 237 DIENER AND MONROE 238 studies (i.e., Dolan, Arnkoff, & Glass, 1993; Hardy, Stiles, Barkham, & Startup, 1998; Saatsi, Hardy, & Cahill, 2007; Satterfield & Lyddon, 1998), despite the fact that these studies met the review’s eligibility criteria. In addition, at least two additional studies (i.e., Marmarosh et al., 2009; Schiff & Levit, 2010) were published after the deadline set by Smith et al. (2010) in their review. A meta-analytic review recently conducted by Diener, Hilsenroth, and Weinberger (2009) examined the relationship between adult attachment style and patient-reported therapeutic alliance in individual psychotherapy as a running example in their primer on meta-analysis of correlation coefficients. Results indicated a positive, statistically significant relationship in which greater attachment security was associated with stronger therapeutic alliance and in which greater attachment insecurity was associated with weaker therapeutic alliance (k ⫽ 12, N ⫽ 581, weighted average r ⫽ .17, p ⬍ .001, 95% confidence interval ⫽ .13–.21). However, the search for studies ended in 2007, the data analytic procedures utilized Hunter and Schmidt’s (1990) calculations rather than the presently more popular method of Hedges et al. (Hedges & Olkin, 1985; Hedges & Vevea, 1998), results were also limited to only data based on patient self-reported alliance, and moderator analyses were not conducted. The present meta-analysis, therefore, aims to fill in the gaps in the literature by presenting a more updated and sophisticated synthesis of the research. First, however, we present an overview of the relevant theories, concepts, and empirical findings from the attachment and therapeutic alliance literature. Adult Attachment Style Bowlby’s (1969/1982) original articulation of attachment theory focused on what he termed the “attachment behavioral system,” or the interconnected series of thoughts, feelings, and behaviors which serve to maintain the biologically based connection between infant and caregiver. According to Bowlby (1969/1982), the attachment system serves an evolutionary function of protection and survival; when frightened, tired, or ill, children seek security and comfort from a primary caregiver. Bowlby (1988) maintained that the ability to create and sustain intimate emotional bonds with others is a primary characteristic of effective personality functioning and overall mental health. The classification system of secure, anxious, and avoidant attachment put forth by Ainsworth et al. (initially, these categories were referred to without the descriptive labels as simply Groups B, C, and A, respectively; Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007)—later expanded to include an additional category of disorganized attachment by Main and Solomon (1990)—was used to describe infant behavior in the Strange Situation. The concepts of attachment style were later applied in research to adult relationships in one of the two general ways: either via use of (a) quick, self-report measures popular in social psychology, or (b) narrative-based measures such as the Adult Attachment Interview (AAI; George, Kaplan, & Main, 1985) often used in developmental psychology (Meyer & Pilkonis, 2001; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007; Steele, Steele, & Murphy, 2009). Recent metaanalytic data (Roisman et al., 2007) suggest that despite some similar terminology in their classification systems, data from selfreport measures and the AAI have small, if any, correlations with each other. According to attachment theory, interactions with caregivers over time influence the development of internal working models of self and other (Meyer & Pilkonis, 2001; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007). These models, in turn, influence an individual’s enduring pattern of relationships. Bowlby (1988) outlined the essential task of psychotherapy as facilitating the exploration and restructuring of the patient’s attachment representations. He maintained that these processes resulted from new understanding and relationship experiences with the therapist. The underlying assumption that the therapeutic relationship will, in part, reflect the patient’s attachment representations has stimulated numerous research studies. These studies (e.g., Satterfield & Lyddon, 1995) examined the hypotheses that secure attachment would predict a stronger therapeutic alliance, whereas insecure attachment would be associated with a weaker alliance. Therapeutic Alliance The concept of the therapeutic alliance appeared in Freud’s (1912/1958) discussion of the various types of transferences. Freud defined transference as the template (or templates) that guides an individual’s erotic life. He specified three aspects of this template, namely (a) the preconditions necessary for a person to fall in love, (b) the instincts that this person chooses to satisfy, and (c) the ways in which this person will go about satisfying them. Freud distinguished between various types of transferences. The broadest distinction is between the positive and negative transferences, or between affectionate and hostile feelings, respectively. Freud then differentiated between positive transference that is accessible to consciousness versus positive transference that is inaccessible to consciousness and which derives from sexual instincts. Using these distinctions, Freud argued that resistance to treatment includes only the negative transference and the unconscious positive transference. The conscious and positive transference, on the other hand, constitutes a welcome and useful component of the therapeutic enterprise. Although Sterba (1934, 1940); Fenichel (1941); Zetzel (1956); Stone (1961); and Gitelson (1962) all took up these ideas in various forms, the term working alliance was not coined until it was first used by Greenson in 1967 (Horvath & Symonds, 1991). Greenson (1967), who described the working alliance as a “rational relationship between patient and analyst” (p. 46), argued that this positive collaboration between therapist and patient is crucial for effective treatment. Subsequent writers have alternated between the terms working alliance and therapeutic alliance. In his pantheoretical concept of the working alliance, Bordin (1979) outlined three major components: (a) agreement on goals for treatment, (b) agreement on tasks to achieve those goals, and (c) the emotional bond of trust and attachment that develops between therapist and patient. Consistent with attachment theory (Bowlby, 1988) and hypotheses articulated in prior research (e.g., Eames & Roth, 2000; Reis & Grenyer, 2004; Satterfield & Lyddon, 1995), we predicted for the present study that secure attachment would be positively correlated with alliance, whereas insecure attachment would be negatively correlated with alliance. In addition, as a result of shared method variance, we predicted that the relationship between selfreported alliance and self-reported attachment would be more robust than the relationship between therapist-rated alliance and SPECIAL SECTION: ATTACHMENT-ALLIANCE META-ANALYSIS patient self-reported attachment. We also sought to examine the potential impact of attrition on the relationship between attachment and alliance. Specifically, increased levels of attrition could lead to biased results in which patients with lower levels of alliance drop out of research studies, leaving only patients with stronger alliances (Smith et al., 2010). This potential bias could restrict the variance and thus attenuate the true attachment-alliance relationship. As a result, we predicted that studies with higher attrition rates would yield smaller effect sizes. Finally, results from Smith et al. (2010) suggested that effect sizes based on data from the secure global attachment category would be more consistent (i.e., statistically significant, larger, or both) than effect sizes based on data from the anxious and avoidant global attachment category. However, these conclusions were based on a limited review of the research and they were not drawn from any theoretical developments within the attachment literature. As a result, in the present study we predicted that effect sizes based on data from all three global attachment categories (i.e., secure, anxious, and avoidant) would be similar in direction, magnitude, and statistical significance. Method Literature Search The following procedures were used in our search of the literature to find individual studies for inclusion in the meta-analysis. We conducted a series of PsycINFO searches, using the terms “attachment AND alliance” through 2010; the final search was performed on July 6, 2010. Each abstract was reviewed. Potentially relevant publications were retrieved in full and examined. Second, we also used several review articles/chapters (e.g., Meyer & Pilkonis, 2001, 2002; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007) to locate relevant references. Third, we checked the reference sections of relevant publications retrieved in the first two steps to locate additional references. The following criteria were used to determine eligibility for inclusion in the present study: (a) Only published articles (rather than books, book chapters, or unpublished studies) were included in our analysis; (b) articles had to be published in English; (c) articles had to present data relevant to our research hypotheses; (d) studies had to examine the relationship between adult attachment style and therapeutic alliance in individual psychotherapy; studies of marital and group treatments were excluded; (e) attachment measures had to assess attachment style in adults’ close interpersonal relationships; that is, they needed to measure relatively enduring patterns of thought, feeling, and behavior which cut across significant relationships and which reflect the individual’s interpersonal trust, concern for rejection, and desire for closeness (Meyer & Pilkonis, 2001); methods such as scoring systems applied to the AAI were excluded given that they appear to measure a different construct, namely the individual’s representation of parental behavior (Crowell, Fraley, & Shaver, 1999) and the “adult’s sense of the way these relationships and events had affected adult personality” (Main, Kaplan, & Cassidy, 1985. p. 90); (f) studies using only measures of patient attachment to the therapist were excluded as the underlying construct is conceptually very close to therapeutic alliance, potentially leading to artifactual inflation of effect sizes; (g) studies had to provide sufficient 239 data to permit calculation of effect sizes. All abstracts or full-text publications were reviewed by either the first or second author for potential inclusion in the meta-analysis. Data Abstraction Effect sizes were calculated by the first author for each study included in the meta-analysis. Primary studies often presented correlational results which were used as the effect size metric. In a number of cases, though, means, standard deviations, and sample sizes were presented and these were transformed into r. In the case of one study (i.e., Hardy et al., 1998), only group means and an overall sample size were presented; however, group sample sizes and standard deviations were not available from the first author of the publication (G. Hardy, personal communication, January 20, 2010). To obtain the number of participants in each group, the overall sample size (N ⫽ 79) was subdivided into three groups (Overinvolved, Underinvolved, and Balanced), using a prorating procedure based on percentages of each group from the larger sample reported in the study. The means for the intervention arms receiving 8 and 16 sessions reported in Table 5 of Hardy et al. (1998) were aggregated to yield an overall mean for the Openness subscale of the Agnew Relationship Measure (ARM; AgnewDavies, Stiles, Hardy, Barkham & Shapiro, 1998). To estimate the relevant standard deviations, the full sample standard deviations for client or therapist ratings reported in the study by AgnewDavies et al. (1998) was used, following procedures outlined by Lipsey and Wilson (2001). The means, standard deviations, and sample sizes for the ARM Openness data were then used to calculate Cohen’s d which was transformed into r. Next, post hoc tests appeared to have been conducted comparing the three interpersonal styles on the ARM ratings for the Partnership (patientand therapist-rated data) and Initiative (therapist-rated data) subscales. Although the number of post hoc tests reported in the study did not seem to fit the number of relevant comparisons, the following effect sizes were coded as r ⫽ .00 in order to be conservative: (a) comparisons between Balanced versus Overinvolved and between Balanced and Underinvolved clients, using both patient and therapist ratings from the Partnership subscale (four comparisons in total); (b) comparisons between Balanced versus Overinvolved and Balanced versus Underinvolved, using therapist ratings from the Initiative subscale (two comparisons in total). All effect sizes were assigned a positive value if they were consistent with our a priori predictions, or a negative value if they were inconsistent with our a priori predictions. Given the skewed distribution of correlation coefficients, effect sizes were first transformed into Fisher’s Z of r, weighted by their inverse variances, averaged and then transformed back into r following standard meta-analytic procedures (e.g., Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, 2009; Lipsey & Wilson, 2001). If a study reported relevant analyses but stated only that the results were nonsignificant without providing data to permit calculation of an effect size, the effect size was entered as r ⫽ .00 to be conservative following standard meta-analytic convention (Horvath & Symonds, 1991; Martin, Garske, & Davis, 2000). Only one effect size and p value was calculated for each study in order to maintain the assumption of independence that is necessary for a meta-analysis. When studies reported multiple effect sizes, these effect sizes were averaged, 240 DIENER AND MONROE again following standard meta-analytic convention (Horvath & Symonds, 1991; Martin et al., 2000). All calculations were conducted using Fisher’s Zr transformation. Results were then transformed back into r. The first author also coded each study for the following features in order to examine the relationship between potential methodological or substantive moderators and the resulting effect sizes: average age of participants; gender; ethnicity; education; average treatment length; attrition rate, mean of Working Alliance Inventory (WAI; Horvath & Greenberg, 1989) scores (12 out of 17 studies included in the meta-analysis provided this data); primary diagnosis; alliance rater; attachment rater; country; primary treatment type; treatment setting; alliance measure; attachment measure; and global attachment category for alliance measure used (e.g., security, anxiety, avoidance, as per Smith et al., 2010; the full coding forms, adapted in part from Sharf, Primavera, & Diener, 2010, are found in Appendix A). Given that the metaanalysis included only 17 independent samples, a number of the levels of the moderator variables were collapsed in order to yield greater statistical power for the analyses. Quantitative Data Synthesis Effect sizes were aggregated across studies using the random effects method of Hedges and colleagues (Hedges & Olkin, 1985; Hedges & Vevea, 1998). Random effects methods are considered to be more representative of real-world data (National Research Council, 1992) and yield results that are more generalizable than their fixed-effect counterparts (Hedges & Vevea, 1998). These calculations, performed using the Comprehensive Meta-Analysis (Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, 2005) software, involve weighting each effect size by the inverse of its variance (Borenstein et al., 2009). The variance used in the weighting procedure has two components: a within-study variance and a between-study variance. Continuous moderator analyses were conducted using mixed effects (method of moments) meta-regression analyses, with the average effect size for each study serving as the dependent variable and each continuous moderator variable serving as a covariate. Because current meta-analytic software will not conduct a multiple meta-regression analysis, each covariate was examined using a separate meta-regression. Most of the categorical moderator analyses were conducted using simple subgroup random effects meta-analyses. However, two of the moderator variables (i.e., global attachment category, alliance rater) required a different approach. Given the fact that current meta-analytic software cannot calculate exact values of the study variances for these subgroup analyses with the type of complex data structure of the present meta-analysis (i.e., some studies provided only patient-rated alliance data, some provided both therapist-rated data and patient-rated data, etc.), we opted to treat each level of the moderator variable (i.e., patient-rated data, therapist-rated data, etc.) as independent of the others in order to be conservative (M. Borenstein, personal communication, January 1, 2010). For the global attachment category moderator variable, subgroup random effects meta-analyses were conducted using the following levels of the grouping variable: (a) security, (b) anxiety, (c) avoidance, (d) security and anxiety (Hardy et al., 1998, and Saatsi et al., 2007, each presented data comparing alliance scores for secure vs. anxious patients), and (e) security and avoidance (Hardy et al., 1998, and Saatsi et al., 2007, each presented data comparing alliance scores for secure vs. avoidant patients). For the alliance moderator variable, subgroup random effects metaanalyses were conducted using the following levels of the grouping variable: (a) patient-rated alliance data and (b) therapist-rated alliance data. For all categorical moderator analyses, Q tests, analogous to analysis of variance in primary research (Borenstein et al., 2009; Lipsey & Wilson, 2001), were calculated to determine whether the various levels of the moderator variable differed significantly from each other. When all subgroups in a particular analysis had at least six studies, estimates of the variance of true effect sizes were not pooled; however, when at least one subgroup had fewer than six studies, estimates of the variance of true effect sizes were pooled because the accuracy yielded by pooling is likely to be greater than any real differences between subgroups (Borenstein et al., 2009). Publication Bias Potential publication bias of the overall meta-analysis was assessed in multiple ways, including (1) a funnel plot display, (2) Duval and Tweedie’s (2000a, 2000b) trim and fill procedure, (3) Begg and Mazumdar’s (1994) rank correlation, and (4) Egger’s regression intercept (Egger, Davey Smith, Schneider, & Minder, 1997). Results Study Flow Figure 1 contains a chart detailing the flow of studies through the present meta-analysis. The PsycINFO searches identified 167 abstracts for potential inclusion. Examination of review articles/ chapters, back-checking of reference sections, and fortuity (e.g., finding additional articles in the same issue as one containing a previously identified publication) yielded another 159 publications for potential inclusion. In all, 326 records were screened. One hundred thirty-seven did not meet criteria for inclusion and were immediately excluded. The remaining 189 articles were retrieved in full and examined further. Of these, 172 were excluded; see Appendix B for details of these exclusions at the online website for the journal’s supplementary materials at http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ a0022425.supp. Seventeen studies remained for inclusion in the present meta-analysis. Several individual effect sizes were excluded from several of the publications that were otherwise included in the meta-analysis; details of these exclusions are also listed in Appendix B. Study Characteristics Relevant information from the 17 articles is briefly summarized in Table 1. More detailed information can be found in Supplementary Table 1 in the journal’s online supplementary materials Web site. The meta-analytic sample consisted of a total of 886 participants (when discussing numbers of participants in the current meta-analysis, these numbers are rounded to the nearest whole number to facilitate ease of comprehension), with a mean sample Identification SPECIAL SECTION: ATTACHMENT-ALLIANCE META-ANALYSIS Abstracts identified through Additional records identified through PsycINFO database searches other sources (e.g., backwards (n = 167) reference search, literature reviews; 241 Eligibility Screening (n = 159) Records screened Records excluded (n = 326) (n = 137) Full-text articles Full-text articles assessed for excluded, with reasons eligibility (see Appendix B for (n = 189) details; Included (n = 172) Studies included in meta-analysis (n = 17) Figure 1. Flow diagram for attachment-alliance meta-analysis. Adapted from flow diagram in Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., Altman, D. G., The PRISMA Group (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA Statement. PLoS Med, 6, e1000097. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed1000097 size of 52 and a standard deviation of 24. Table 2 contains the moderator codes for each independent sample. Quantitative Data Synthesis Overall effect size. As predicted, greater attachment security predicted stronger alliance scores, whereas weaker attachment security predicted weaker alliance scores with an overall weighted effect size of r ⫽ .17, p ⬍ .001 (95% confidence interval ⫽ .10 –.23). The size of this association falls in between a small and medium effect size (Cohen, 1988); it is in the middle third of the treatment effects (although the lower end of the 95% confidence interval dips below this benchmark), and the lower third of the overall effect sizes, found in Hemphill’s (2003) review. Several supplementary analyses were conducted to explore whether the decision to include two studies (i.e., Romano et al., 2008; Schiff & Levit, 2010) in the meta-analysis may have biased the results. Romano et al.’s (2008) study investigated the relationship between attachment and alliance for volunteer rather than for “real” clients, thus distinguishing its sample from those of the remaining studies in the meta-analysis. Schiff and Levit’s (2010) study explored the relationship between attachment and alliance in treatment offered by social workers to their female patients in a methadone clinic; only one-fourth of the clients met weekly with their social workers and more than 50% of clients reported meeting “seldom” or “never,” thus distinguish- ing the intervention provided from the remaining studies in the meta-analysis. As a result of the unique characteristics of each of these studies, the analyses were conducted excluding each one as well as excluding both of them. Results of all analyses were essentially identical to those of the main analyses presented earlier. Results of the homogeneity test (Q ⫽ 6.10, df ⫽ 16, p ⫽ .99) indicate no demonstrable evidence for potential moderators. Nevertheless, following the recommendations of Rosenthal and DiMatteo (2001), we present the results of several moderator analyses later to aid future researchers in further clarifying the results. Publication bias. Figure 2 presents a funnel plot with data from the meta-analysis together with imputed data to correct for potential publication bias using the Trim and Fill procedure outlined by Duval and Tweedie (2000a, 2000b). An “eye-ball” test of the funnel plot does not indicate any potential for publication bias because the studies with the larger effect sizes at the bottom of the plot do not tend to fall on the right side of the graph. Even if publication bias did exist, the impact is likely negligible, given the fact that the Trim and Fill procedure did not trim a single effect, leaving the adjusted and observed overall effects identical. Results of Begg and Mazumdar’s (1994) rank correlation ( ⫽ ⫺0.02, p [one-tailed] ⫽ .45) and Egger’s (Egger et al., 1997) regression intercept (intercept ⫽ ⫺0.08, p [one-tailed] ⫽ .45) are not demonstrably consistent with the conclusion that publication bias exists, DIENER AND MONROE 242 Table 1 Overall Random Effects Meta-Analysis of Attachment and Alliance Discussion Hypothesis 1: Relationship Between Attachment and Alliance 95% confidence interval Study name r Lower limit Upper limit Z-value p-value Bruck et al., 2006 Dolan et al., 1993 Eames & Roth, 2000 Goldman & Anderson, 2007 Hardy et al., 1998 Kivlighan, Patton, & Foote, 1998 Mallinckrodt, Coble, & Gantt, 1995 Mallinckrodt, Porter, & Kivlighan, 2005 Marmarosh et al., 2009 Parish & Eagle, 2003 Reis & Grenyer, 2004 Romano, Fitzpatrick, & Jansen, 2008 Saatsi et al., 2007 Satterfield & Lyddon, 1995 Satterfield & Lyddon, 1998 Sauer, Lopez, & Gormley, 2003 Schiff & Levit, 2010 Weighted Mean r .04 .12 .13 .29 .000 ⫺.25 ⫺.39 ⫺.28 ⫺.08 ⫺.27 .33 .57 .50 .59 .27 .28 .43 .61 1.53 .002 .78 .67 .54 .13 1.00 .31 ⫺.003 .57 1.94 .05 .19 ⫺.04 .40 1.61 .11 .29 .09 .18 .14 ⫺.04 ⫺.20 ⫺.01 ⫺.12 .55 .37 .36 .39 1.74 .62 1.82 1.08 .08 .53 .07 .28 .22 .30 .21 .12 ⫺.04 .03 ⫺.05 ⫺.13 .45 .52 .44 .36 1.67 2.21 1.59 .96 .09 .03 .11 .34 .03 .14 .17 ⫺.41 ⫺.06 .10 .45 .33 .23 .11 1.35 4.91 .91 .18 <.001 although these results may be affected by the low power of the tests (Borenstein et al., 2009). Moderator analyses. For a number of analyses, moderator data could not be coded from the overwhelming number of studies and the analyses were therefore not conducted; these included the metaregression for attrition rate (only five studies out of 17 provided codeable data), the metaregression for education (only three studies out of 17 provided codeable data), and the categorical subgroup analysis for primary diagnosis (only five studies out of 17 provided codeable data). None of the continuous moderator analyses were statistically significant. These results included the metaregressions for age (slope ⫽ ⫺.01, p ⫽ .23), gender (slope ⫽ ⫺.002, p ⫽ .46), ethnicity (slope ⫽ .000, p ⫽ .92), and mean WAI (slope ⫽ .16, p ⫽ .22). For the categorical subgroup analyses, patient-rated alliance demonstrated a statistically significant stronger correlation with attachment style (average weighted r ⫽ .17, 95% confidence interval ⫽ .10 –.24, p ⬍ .001) than therapist-rated alliance (average weighted r ⫽ .01, 95% confidence interval ⫽ ⫺.14 - .16, p ⫽ .89; Qbetween ⫽ 3.95, df ⫽ 1, p ⫽ .047). The remaining categorical subgroup analyses were nonsignificant, including the moderator variables for average treatment length (Qbetween ⫽ .03, p ⫽ .86, df ⫽ 1, k ⫽ 8), country (Qbetween ⫽ .05, p ⫽ .82, df ⫽ 1, k ⫽ 17), primary treatment type (Q between ⫽ 2.13, p ⫽ .55, df ⫽ 3, k ⫽ 11), treatment setting (Qbetween ⫽ .19, p ⫽ .66, df ⫽ 1, k ⫽ 17), alliance measure (Qbetween ⫽ .80, p ⫽ .67, df ⫽ 2, k ⫽ 17), attachment measure (Qbetween ⫽ .06, p ⫽ .97, df ⫽ 2, k ⫽ 17), and global attachment category (Qbetween ⫽ .88, p ⫽ .93, df ⫽ 4). As predicted, the results indicate that individuals with more secure attachment styles demonstrated stronger alliances, whereas individuals with more insecure attachment styles demonstrated weaker alliances. Although not directly comparable, the overall effect size in the present study resembles the magnitude of the relationship between alliance and outcome (average weighted r ⫽ .22, r ⫽ .28; respectively, Martin et al., 2000; Horvath, Del Re, Flückiger, & Symond, 2011), a relationship considered reflective of the power of alliance as “the most robust predictor of treatment success” (Safran & Muran, 2000; p. 1). Results also indicated no demonstrable evidence for publication bias and further suggested that its effect would likely be negligible even if it did exist. These findings are consistent with attachment theory’s emphasis on the importance of attachment representations in influencing enduring relationship patterns. Although none of the studies used methodology that would permit causal inferences, the findings do suggest the contiguity between general patterns of relational functioning outside of the consulting room and the quality of the therapeutic alliance in individual psychotherapy. Attachment theory would point to the centrality of internal working models in explaining this overlap across the different types of interpersonal relationships. Internal working models contain implicit, unconscious assumptions about self and others, which are used to make sense of the inherently ambiguous interpersonal world (Main et al., 1985; cf. Wachtel, 1993). For individuals with more secure attachment styles, the assumptions connected to their internal working models reflect a trust in the benevolence of others, the adequacy and essential goodness of the self, and the desire for interpersonal connection. In working together with their therapists, therefore, these individuals are more likely to be able to form an emotional bond, to agree on goals for treatment, and to agree on tasks to achieve those goals (Bordin, 1979). In contrast, for individuals with more insecure attachment styles, the assumptions connected to their internal working models reflect a distrust of the motives and intentions of others, a more negative self representation, a wariness to engage intimately with others, a pressing need to be reassured of the love of others, or some combination of that. Therefore, while working together with their therapist, these individuals have a more difficult time cultivating an emotional bond, agreeing with their therapist on goals for treatment and on tasks to achieve those goals. In psychotherapy, therefore, clinicians are encouraged to pay particular attention to the quality of the therapeutic alliance when working with individuals with a history of more insecure attachment. These types of attachment histories could serve as “red flags,” allowing the therapist to predict the potential for ruptures in the alliance and intervene proactively to minimize their deleterious effects while also capitalizing on the therapeutic opportunities inherent in working through them. With such patients, therapists would do well to carefully monitor the relationship for signs of distance or discontent. When therapists spot these signs, they can use relationally based interventions to repair the alliance (e.g., Crits-Christoph et al., 2006). As an University-counseling center Outpatient clinics University-based Tx center Research clinic University counseling centers Mixed University counseling center University counseling center Mixed Outpatient university clinic Tx setting for students serving as volunteer clients Research clinic University-based counseling clinic University-based counseling service center Mixed Methadone clinics Dolan et al., 1993 Eames & Roth, 2000 Goldman & Anderson, 2007 Hardy et al., 1998 Kivlighan et al., 1998 Mallinckrodt et al., 1995 Mallinckrodt et al., 2005 Marmarosh et al., 2009 Parish & Eagle, 2003 Reis & Grenyer, 2004 Romano et al., 2008 Satterfield & Lyddon, 1995 Satterfield & Lyddon, 1998 Sauer et al., 2003 Schiff & Levit, 2010 — — — Equivalent # received 2 types of Tx Psychodynamic/ psychoanalytic Substance use — Mood — Mood — — — — — Mood — — — Mixed — Cognitive therapy — Eclectic/integrative Psychodynamic/ psychoanalytic Psychodynamic/ psychoanalytic Eclectic/integrative Equivalent # received 2 types of Tx — — Eclectic Psychodynamic/ psychoanalytic — CBT Primary Tx type Primary Dx 39.35 32.75 23.37 21.93 34.92 28.97 45.98 27.39 — 34.50 24.68 33.60 40.30 21.60 — 34.70 39.40 Mean age .00 35.29 19.05 28.33 27.27 8.47 41.38 36.84 — 24.76 32.50 .00 46.49 10.90 — 43.33 43.48 % Male — 100 — — — 100 — — — — — — — 96.40 — — — % HS or more — 88.00 61.90 65.00 — 66.10 — 84.21 — 92.38 90.00 88.16 — 85.50 — 100 — % Caucasian — — — Short-term (fewer than eight sessions) Medium (8–16 sessions) — Medium (8–16 sessions) — — Longer (17 or more sessions) Medium (8–16 sessions) Medium (8–16 sessions) Medium (8–16 sessions) — Longer (17 or more sessions) — — Mean Tx lengthb Israel USA 20.25i — USA United Kingdom USA Canada Australia USA USA USA United Kingdom USA USA USA United Kingdom USA USA Countryc — 15.63 33.64 — — — — — — — — 43.64 — — .00 Attrition rate 5.70 5.72j 6.04 6.10 N/Ag 5.93 5.56 5.95 5.35h — 5.81 5.62 N/Ag 6.08f — — 4.94e Mean WAI scored Note. Dx ⫽ diagnosis; CBT ⫽ cognitive-behavioral therapy; HS ⫽ high school; Tx ⫽ treatment; — ⫽ study did not provide this data. Moderator codings for the effect size level variables as well as the study level variables of alliance measure and attachment measure (data for the latter two variables are available in Supplementary Table 1) are not presented in this table due to space considerations but are available upon request from the first author. a For purposes of the analysis, all data from this variable were collapsed into one of two categories: (a) university-based outpatient treatment center, (b) other. b For purposes of the analysis, all data from this variable were collapsed into one of two categories: (a) medium (8 –16 sessions), (b) other (longer [17 or more sessions] or short-term [fewer than eight sessions]). c For purposes of the analysis, all data from this variable were collapsed into one of two categories: (a) USA, (b) non-USA. d Some studies report the sum of all items averaged across participants, whereas others report only the average WAI item score. In addition, some studies utilized the 36 item version of the WAI, whereas others used the 12 item version. To make the results comparable across studies, therefore, data were transformed when necessary into the average WAI item score. e This value was obtained by averaging the Working Alliance Inventory (WAI) scores from both patient (5.13) and therapist (4.74) ratings. f This value was obtained by averaging the WAI scores across the three sessions for which data were reported. g The WAI was not used as the alliance measure in this study. h This value was obtained by averaging the scores from patient ratings (5.54) and therapist ratings (5.16). i In this study, patients dropped out of treatment at several different points; given the heterogeneity in the literature about how to define dropouts (Sharf et al., 2010), we decided to take the average of number of participants who dropped out across each of the three different time periods. This number was then used in a ratio to the total number of participants to yield the attrition rate. j This value was obtained by taking the mean of the client and therapist WAI ratings across all three time points for which data were presented in the study. Saatsi et al., 2007 Outpatient psychiatry department Tx settinga Bruck et al., 2006 Study Table 2 Moderator Codes for Each Independent Sample SPECIAL SECTION: ATTACHMENT-ALLIANCE META-ANALYSIS 243 DIENER AND MONROE 244 Funnel Plot of Standard Error by Fisher's Z 0.0 Standard Error 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 -2.0 -1.5 -1.0 -0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0 Fisher's Z Figure 2. Graphical representation of potential publication bias. The white circles represent each of the studies that were actually included plotted by the size of the effect (in Fisher’s Z) on the x-axis and the standard error on the y-axis. If there was publication bias, we would expect that the largest effects would have the largest standard error (yielding void in the lower left quadrant; Borenstein et al., 2009). Studies toward the tip of the triangle have the smallest standard error. The white diamond represents the weighted average effect size of the actual studies included in the meta-analysis. The following iterative procedure is used for the trim and fill: studies at the extreme positive side of the graph are removed, the weighted average effect is recalculated, and this process of trimming the plot continues until the distribution of studies is symmetric around the weighted average effect. Next, each removed study is added back in and a mirror image of the study is imputed to correct for reduction in the variance of effects as a result of the trimming procedure. The black diamond represents the weighted average effect size calculated using the studies actually included in the meta-analysis as well as the imputed studies. In the present meta-analysis, there were no studies that needed to be removed and therefore the observed and imputed overall effect size is identical. example, therapists can point out instances in which patient behaviors reflect problems in the alliance, discuss their meaning, explore the connection to the ongoing therapeutic work, and draw parallels to interpersonal expectations and emotional reactions in patients’ attachment relationships outside of therapy. In addition, explicit discussion of therapeutic goals and tasks to achieve those goals as well as attention to the overall emotional climate can facilitate improvements in the alliance while simultaneously opening up avenues of fruitful exploration of repetitive interpersonal patterns. Nevertheless, the magnitude of the relationship between attachment and alliance suggests that much of the variance in alliance remains to be accounted for even after taking into consideration patients’ attachment styles. In this respect, the findings suggest that an individual’s enduring relationship pattern outside of therapy does not automatically map onto the therapeutic relationship. Instead, individuals with more insecure attachment styles can still develop positive working alliances with their therapists. This possibility is supported by the finding that the mean of WAI scores across 12 studies providing sufficient data was 5.74 on a 7-point scale (both the unweighted and weighted means were identical in this instance), suggesting generally strong alliances across the different individual studies. Perhaps therapists in the original studies tailored their interpersonal stance to the specific attachment styles of their patients, allowing for increased engagement in the therapeutic relationship. Alternatively, the unique nature of the therapeutic relationship may itself permit a more adaptive and collaborative interpersonal approach, allowing patients to diverge from their well-trodden paths in relationships. Hypothesis 2: Patient-Reported Alliance Versus Therapist-Reported Alliance in Relation to Attachment Consistent with a priori predictions, there was a significant difference in the magnitude of the relationship between patientreported alliance and patient-reported attachment on the one hand, and the relationship between therapist-reported alliance and patient self-reported attachment on the other hand. These results suggest that the similarity in relational patterns between relationships in general and the therapeutic relationship may be more robust when viewed from the patient’s perspective. That is, patients may perceive the working alliance in ways that are more similar to their general attachment style than do their therapists. However, given the number of studies included in the present meta-analysis relative to the number of moderator analyses conducted, these results should be considered exploratory and interpreted with caution (Borenstein et al., 2009). Hypothesis 3: The Impact of Attrition on the Relationship Between Attachment and Alliance Given the small number of studies that provided data on attrition rates (i.e., five out of 17 studies), the analyses to test this particular hypothesis could not be conducted. Researchers are enjoined to regularly provide these data for readers and future researchers. Given the positive and statistically significant relationship between alliance and psychotherapy dropouts (Sharf et al., 2010), it is likely that individuals who dropped out of treatment prematurely had SPECIAL SECTION: ATTACHMENT-ALLIANCE META-ANALYSIS weaker alliances than those who remained in psychotherapy. As a result, the effect size found in the present study may have been attenuated due to restrictions on the range of alliance scores. However, this possibility cannot be explored without additional data. Hypothesis 4: Effect Sizes and Global Attachment Categories As predicted, no differences in magnitude, direction, or statistical significance were found between effect sizes based on data from the secure, anxious, or avoidant global attachment categories (average weighted r values ⫽ .19, ⫺.14, and ⫺.15, respectively; all p values ⬍ .001). Effect sizes based on data from both secure and anxious or secure and avoidant global attachment categories, however, were both nonsignificant (p values ⬎ .10). Nevertheless, they were in the same direction (i.e., they reflect correlations with alliance in the predicted directions, albeit nonsignificantly) and general range as results from the aforementioned three global attachment categories (average weighted r values ⫽ .14 for security and anxiety, and .14 for security and avoidance). The average weighted effect sizes for these two global attachment categories (i.e., secure and anxious; secure and avoidant) are reported in the present study with a positive valence as they reflect comparisons in the original studies between secure versus anxious and secure versus avoidant patients that were in the predicted direction, namely secure patients had higher mean alliance scores than either anxious or avoidant patients.1 Their lack of statistical significance likely stemmed from the fact that each of these global attachment categories had data from only two studies. Limitations and Conclusion Limitations of the present study include the fact that only published studies were included in the meta-analysis. This inclusion criterion may have biased the results given that stronger, more significant findings tend to find their way into the published literature more often than weaker, less significant ones (Borenstein et al., 2009). Nevertheless, results of the publication bias analyses did not provide any demonstrable evidence for bias, and they indicated that the potential impact of any such bias is likely negligible. In addition, the statistical power for the moderator analyses may have been hampered by the relatively small number of studies included in the meta-analysis. Future research, therefore, would benefit from a larger pool of studies (both published and nonpublished, as explained earlier) to permit identification of additional potential moderator variables. Finally, the study is limited by the fact that ratings were completed by a single judge. This limitation is partially mitigated by the fact this rater has demonstrated reliability in coding effect sizes and other relevant data in a recent meta-analytic investigation; at the study level, the codes of the rater demonstrated an average intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC; results were identical within rounding error for all three models of ICC) of .84 with an independent rater (Diener, Hilsenroth, Shaffer, & Sexton, in press) which is considered to be indicative of excellent reliability (Cicchetti, 1994). There was only a single variable coded at the level of the effect size, and this rater demonstrated good reliability with an ICC of .60 when compared 245 with the independent rater (Cicchetti, 1994). In addition, the data for the present study are clearly detailed both in this publication and in the online supplementary materials, allowing researchers to form their own conclusions. Overall, though, results suggested the convergence between adult attachment style and the quality of the therapeutic alliance. Individuals with more secure attachment styles tend to develop stronger alliances, whereas individuals with less secure attachment styles tend to develop weaker alliances. These findings highlight the relational consistencies across different interpersonal arenas and suggest the potential utility of attending to the therapeutic implications of patient attachment histories. These findings may also be robust to the challenge of publication bias, further strengthening their evidentiary basis. The relationship between attachment and alliance did not appear to differ, depending on whether secure, anxious, or avoidant attachment was assessed. Finally, patients may perceive the therapeutic alliance in a manner more consistent with their general attachment style when compared with their therapists’ perception of the alliance. Taken together, these results support attachment theory’s emphasis on the clinical significance of patient attachment representations, and suggest the potential therapeutic utility of integrating attachment-specific relational interventions. References References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis. Ackerman, S. J., Benjamin, L. S., Beutler, L. E., Gelso, C. J., Goldfried, M. R., Hill, C., . . . Ranier, J. (2001). Empirically supported therapy relationships: Conclusions and recommendations of the Division 29 Task Force. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, and Training, 38, 495– 497. doi:10.1037/0033–3204.38.4.495 Agnew-Davies, R., Stiles, W. B., Hardy, G. E., Barkham, M., & Shapiro, D. A. (1998). Alliance structure assessed by the Agnew Relationship Measure (ARM). British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 37, 155–172. Retrieved from http://www.bpsjournals.co.uk/journals/bjcp/ Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Begg, C., B., & Mazumdar, M. (1994). Operating characteristics of a rank correlation test for publication bias. Biometrics, 50, 1088 –1101. Retrieved from http://www.biometrics.tibs.org/ 1 The effect sizes discussed earlier in the paragraph, however, for the global attachment categories of (a) secure, (b) anxious, and (c) avoidant were presented in positive or negative valence, depending on the actual direction of the underlying effect sizes. The average weighted effect for measures of secure global attachment was reported as positive to indicate that greater attachment security was associated with stronger alliance. The average weighted effects for measures of anxious or avoidant global attachment were reported as negative to indicate that greater attachment insecurity was associated with weaker alliance. In discussing the global attachment categories, though, of (a) secure and anxious, and (b) secure and avoidant, it was necessary to report the valence of the effect size in terms of whether it was consistent with a priori predictions rather than the direction of the underlying effect size as secure attachment should be positively related to alliance, whereas anxious and avoidant attachment should be negatively associated with alliance, precluding the possibility of reporting the valence in terms of a single, underlying direction of the effects. 246 DIENER AND MONROE Bordin, E. S. (1979). The generalizability of the psychoanalytic concept of the working alliance. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research & Practice, 16, 252–260. Borenstein, M., Hedges, L., Higgins, J., & Rothstein, H. (2005). Comprehensive meta-analysis Version 2. Englewood, NJ: Biostat. Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. (2009). Introduction to meta-analysis. Chichester, UK: Wiley, Ltd. Bowlby, J. (1969/1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment (2nd ed.) (Original work published 1969). New York: Basic Books. Bowlby, J. (1988). Attachment, communication, and the therapeutic process. In J. Bowlby (Ed.), A secure base: Parent-child attachment and healthy human development (pp. 137–157). New York: Basic Books. *Bruck, E., Winston, A., Aderholt, S., & Muran, J. C. (2006). Predictive validity of patient and therapist attachment and introject styles. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 60, 393– 406. Retrieved from http:// www.ajp.org Cicchetti, D. V. (1994). Guidelines, criteria, and rules of thumb for evaluating normed and standardized assessment instruments in psychology. Psychological Assessment, 6, 284 –290. doi:10.1037/1040 –3590.6.4.284 Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). New Jersey: Erlbaum. Crits-Christoph, P., Gibbons, M. B. C., Crits-Christoph, K., Narducci, J., Schamberger, M., & Gallop, R. (2006). Can therapists be trained to improve their alliances? A preliminary study of alliance-fostering psychotherapy. Psychotherapy Research, 16, 268 –281. doi:10.1080/ 10503300500268557 Crowell, J. A., Fraley, R. C., & Shaver, P. R. (1999). Measurement of adult attachment. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 434 – 465). New York: Guilford Press. Diener, M. J., Hilsenroth, M. J., Shaffer, S. A., & Sexton, J. E. (in press). A meta-analysis of the relationship between the Rorschach Ego Impairment Index (EII) and psychiatric severity. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy. doi:10.1002/cpp.725 Diener, M. J., Hilsenroth, M. J., & Weinberger, J. (2009). A primer on meta-analysis of correlation coefficients: The relationship between patient-reported therapeutic alliance and adult attachment style as an illustration. Psychotherapy Research, 19, 519 –526. doi:10.1080/ 10503300802491410 *Dolan, R. T., Arnkiff, D. B., & Glass, C. R. (1993). Client attachment style and the psychotherapist’s interpersonal stance. Psychotherapy, 30, 408 – 412. doi:10.1037/0033–3204.30.3.408 Duval, S., & Tweedie, R. (2000a). A nonparametric “trim and fill” method of accounting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 95, 89 –98. Retrieved from http:// www.amstat.org/publications/jasa.cfm Duval, S., & Tweedie, R. (2000b). Trim and fill: A simple funnel-plotbased method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in metaanalysis. Biometrics, 56, 455– 463. Retrieved from http://www.biometrics .tibs.org/ *Eames, V., & Roth, A. (2000). Patient attachment orientation and the early working alliance: A study of patient and therapist reports of alliance quality and ruptures. Psychotherapy Research, 10, 421– 434. doi: 10.1093/ptr/10.4.421 Egger, M., Davey Smith, G., Schneider, M., & Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple graphical test. British Medical Journal, 315, 629 – 634. Retrieved from http://www.BritishMedicalJournal .com/ Fenichel, O. (1941). Problems of psychoanalytic technique. Albany, NY: Psychoanalytic Quarterly. Freud, S. (1912/1958). The dynamics of transference. In J. Strachey (Ed. & Trans.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 12, pp. 97–108) (Original work published 1912). London: Hogarth Press. George, C., Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1985). Adult attachment interview (2nd ed.). Berkeley, CA: University of California at Berkeley. Gitelson, M. (1962). The curative factors in psycho-analysis. Part I. The International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 43, 194 –205. Retrieved from http://www.wiley.com/bw/journal.asp?ref⫽0020 –7578 *Goldman, G. A., & Anderson, T. (2007). Quality of object relations and security of attachment as predictors of early therapeutic alliance. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 54, 111–117. doi:10.1037/0022– 0167 .54.2.111 Greenson, R. R. (1967). The technique and practice of psychoanalysis (Vol. 1). Madison, CT: International Universities Press. *Hardy, G. E., Stiles, W. B., Barkham, M., & Startup, M. (1998). Therapist responsiveness to client interpersonal styles during time-limited treatments for depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 304 –312. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.66.2.304 Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 511–524. doi:10.1037/0022–3514.52.3.511 Hedges, L. V., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Hedges, L. V., &Vevea, J. L. (1998). Fixed- and random-effects models in meta-analysis. Psychological Methods, 3, 486 –504. doi:10.1037/1082989X.3.4.486 Hemphill, J. F. (2003). Interpreting the magnitudes of correlation coefficients. American Psychologist, 58, 78 –79. doi:10.1037/0003-066X .58.1.78 Horvath, A. O., Del Re, A. C., Flückiger, C., & Symonds, D. (2011). Alliance in individual psychotherapy. Psychotherapy, 48, 9 –16. Horvath, A. O., & Greenberg, L. S. (1989). Development and validation of the Working Alliance Inventory. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 36, 223–233. doi:10.1037/0022– 0167.36.2.223 Horvath, A. O., & Symonds, B. D. (1991). Relation between working alliance and outcome in psychotherapy: A meta-analysis. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 38, 139 –149. doi:10.1037/0022– 0167.38.2.139 Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, F. L. (1990). Methods of meta-analysis: Correcting error and bias in research findings. Newbury Park: Sage. *Kivlighan, D. M., Patton, M. J., & Foote, D. (1998). Moderating effects of client attachment on the counselor experience-working alliance relationship. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 45, 274 –278. doi:10.1037/ 0022– 0167.45.3.274 Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy, childhood, and adulthood: A move to the level of representation. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50, 66 –104. doi:10.1111/ 1540 –5834.ep11889989 Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1990). Procedures for identifying infants as disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth Strange Situation. In M. T. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, & M. Cummings (Eds.), Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research, and intervention (pp. 121–160). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. *Mallinckrodt, B., Coble, H. M., & Gantt, D. L. (1995). Working alliance, attachment memories, and social competencies of women in brief therapy. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 42, 79 – 84. doi:10.1037/0022– 0167.42.1.79 *Mallinckrodt, B., Porter, M. J., & Kivlighan, D. M. (2005). Client attachment to therapist, depth of in-session exploration, and object relations in brief psychotherapy. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, and Training, 42, 85–100. doi:10.1037/0033–3204.42.1.85 *Marmarosh, C. L., Gelso, C. J., Markin, R. D., Majors, R., Mallery, C., & Choi, J. (2009). The real relationship in psychotherapy: Relationships to adult attachments, working alliance, transference, and therapy outcome. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 56, 337–350. doi:10.1037/a0015169 Martin, D., Garske, J., & Davis, M. (2000). Relation of the therapeutic SPECIAL SECTION: ATTACHMENT-ALLIANCE META-ANALYSIS alliance with outcome and other variables: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 438 – 450. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.68.3.438 Meyer, B., & Pilkonis, P. (2001). Attachment style. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 38, 466 – 472. doi:10.1037/0033–3204 .38.4.466 Meyer, B., & Pilkonis, P. A. (2002). Attachment style. In J. C. Norcross (Ed.), Psychotherapy relationships that work: Therapist contributions and responsiveness to patients (pp. 367–382). New York: Oxford University Press. Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Attachment in adulthood: Structure, dynamics, and change. New York: The Guilford Press. National Research Council. (1992). Combining information: Statistical issues and opportunities for research. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. *Parish, M., & Eagle, M. N. (2003). Attachment to the therapist. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 20, 271–286. doi:10.1037/0736 –9735.20.2.271 *Reis, S., & Grenyer, B. F. S. (2004). Fearful attachment, working alliance and treatment response for individuals with major depression. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 11, 414 – 424. doi:10.1002/cpp.428 Roisman, G. I., Holland, A., Fortuna, K., Fraley, R. C., Clausell, E., & Clarke, A. (2007). The adult attachment interview and self-reports of attachment style: An empirical rapprochement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 678 – 697. doi:10.1037/0022–3514.92.4.678 *Romano, V., Fitzpatrick, M., & Janzen, J. (2008). The secure-base hypothesis: Global attachment, attachment to counselor, and session exploration in psychotherapy. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 55, 495– 504. doi:10.1037/a0013721 Rosenthal, R., & DiMatteo, M. R. (2001). Meta-analysis: Recent developments in quantitative methods for literature reviews. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 59 – 82. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.59 *Saatsi, S., Hardy, G., & Cahill, J. (2007). Predictors of outcome and completion status in cognitive therapy for depression. Psychotherapy Research, 17, 185–195. doi:10.1080/10503300600779420 Safran, J. D., & Muran, J. C. (2000). Negotiating the therapeutic alliance: A relational treatment guide. New York: The Guilford Press. 247 *Satterfield, W., & Lyddon, W. (1998). Client attachment and the working alliance. Counseling Psychology Quarterly, 11, 407– 415. doi:10.1080/ 09515079808254071 *Satterfield, W. A., & Lyddon, W. J. (1995). Client attachment and perceptions of the working alliance with counselor trainees. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 42, 187–189. doi:10.1037/0022– 0167.42.2.187 *Sauer, E. M., Lopez, F. G., & Gormley, B. (2003). Respective contributions of therapist and client adult attachment orientations to the development of the early working alliance: A preliminary growth modeling study. Psychotherapy Research, 13, 371–382. doi:10.1093/ptr/kpg027 *Schiff, M., & Levit, S. (2010). Correlates of therapeutic alliance and treatment outcome among Israeli female methadone patients. Research on Social Work Practice, 20, 380 –390. doi:10.1177/ 1049731509347854 Sharf, J., Primavera, L. H., & Diener, M. J. (2010). Dropout and therapeutic alliance: A meta- analysis of adult individual psychotherapy. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, and Training, 47, 637– 645. Smith, A. E. M., Msefti, R. M., & Golding, L. (2010). Client self-rated adult attachment patterns and the therapeutic alliance: A systematic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 326 –337. doi:10.1016/j.cpr .2009.12.007 Steele, H., Steele, M., & Murphy, A. (2009). Use of the adult attachment interview to measure process and change in psychotherapy. Psychotherapy Research, 19, 633– 643. doi:10.1080/10503300802609698 Sterba, R. F. (1934). The fate of the ego in analytic therapy. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 15, 117–126. Retrieved from http:// www.wiley.com/bw/journal.asp?ref⫽0020 –7578 Sterba, R. F. (1940). The dynamics of the dissolution of the transference resistance. The Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 9, 363–379. Stone, L. (1961). The psychoanalytic situation. New York: International University Press. Wachtel, P. L. (1993). Therapeutic communication: Knowing what to say when. New York: The Guilford Press. Zetzel, E. R. (1956). Current concepts of transference. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 37, 369 –376. Retrieved from http:// www.wiley.com/bw/journal.asp?ref⫽0020 –75 (Appendix follows) DIENER AND MONROE 248 Appendix A Coding Criteria for Moderator Analysesa Study Level Variables Treatment setting 1 ⫽ University-based treatment center 2 ⫽ Outpatient psychiatry department 3 ⫽ Outpatient clinic 4 ⫽ Mixture of two or more of the following: university counseling center, community counseling agency, independent practice, mental health center, or other 5 ⫽ Research clinic 9 ⫽ Cannot tell Primary treatment type (code for primary treatment type, e.g., most patients received CBT or therapists primarily identified with CBT relative to other orientations) 1 ⫽ Cognitive– behavioral therapy 2 ⫽ Psychodynamic/psychoanalytic 3 ⫽ Eclectic/integrative 4 ⫽ Equivalent number of patients received two types of treatments (e.g., eclectic tied with CBT) 9 ⫽ Cannot tell Primary diagnosis 1 ⫽ Psychotic disorder 2 ⫽ Mood disorder 3 ⫽ Anxiety disorder 4 ⫽ Eating disorder 5 ⫽ Personality disorder 6 ⫽ Other 7 ⫽ Mixed diagnoses 99 ⫽ Cannot tell Average age of sample participants (“99” if cannot tell): ______ Gender [% Male (“999” if cannot tell)]: ______ Education [% Completed High School or Higher (“999” if cannot tell)]: ______ Ethnicity [% White (“999” if cannot tell)]: ______ Average treatment length 1 ⫽ short-term (⬍8 sessions) 2 ⫽ Medium (8 –16 sessions) 3 ⫽ Longer (17 or more sessions) 9 ⫽ Cannot tell Attrition rate (“999” if cannot tell): ______ Country 1 ⫽ U.S.A. 2 ⫽ U.K. 3 ⫽ Canada 4 ⫽ Europe View publication stats 5 ⫽ Australia 9 ⫽ Other (write in :________________) 99 ⫽ Cannot tell Mean of WAI scores: ______ Effect Size Level Variables Alliance rater 1 ⫽ Patient 2 ⫽ Therapist 3 ⫽ External rater Alliance measure 1 ⫽ WAI- 36 items 2 ⫽ WAI- 12 items 3 ⫽ Other (specify:____________________________) Attachment rater 1 ⫽ Patient 2 ⫽ Therapist 3 ⫽ External rater Attachment measure 1 ⫽ Adult Attachment Scale 2 ⫽ Attachment History Questionnaire 3 ⫽ Experiences in Close Relationships Scale 4 ⫽ Relationship Styles Questionnaire 5 ⫽ Relationship Questionnaire 6 ⫽ Other (specify: ____________________________) Global attachment category 1 ⫽ Security (e.g., Secure/security; Depend; Close) 2 ⫽ Avoidance (e.g., Avoidance; Dismissive; Fearful; Relationships as secondary; Discomfort with closeness) 3 ⫽ Anxiety (e.g., Anxiety; Preoccupied; Need for approval) 4 ⫽ Secure and Anxious (e.g., study reports contrast between secure and anxious participants) 5 ⫽ Secure and Avoidant (e.g., study reports contrast between secure and avoidant participants) a Adapted from J. Sharf, L. H. Primavera, & M.J. Diener 2010. Dropout and therapeutic alliance: A meta-analysis of adult individual psychotherapy. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice and Training, 47, 637645. Received November 19, 2010 Accepted November 22, 2010 䡲