Download The assessment of depth of anaesthesia in animals and man

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Animal testing wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Laboratory
Animals
http://lan.sagepub.com/
The assessment of depth of anaesthesia in animals and man
G. Whelan and P. A. Flecknell
Lab Anim 1992 26: 153
DOI: 10.1258/002367792780740602
The online version of this article can be found at:
http://lan.sagepub.com/content/26/3/153
Published by:
http://www.sagepublications.com
On behalf of:
Laboratory Animals LtdLaboratory Animals Ltd
Additional services and information for Laboratory Animals can be found at:
Email Alerts: http://lan.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts
Subscriptions: http://lan.sagepub.com/subscriptions
Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav
Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
>> Version of Record - Jul 1, 1992
What is This?
Downloaded from lan.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on March 3, 2014
Laboratory Animals (1992) 26, 153-162
153
REVIEW ARTICLE
The assessment of depth of anaesthesia in animals and man
G. WHELAN & P. A. FLECKNELL
Comparative Biology Centre, Medical School, Framlington Place, Newcastle upon Tyne NE2 4HH, UK
Summary
The assessment of the depth of anaesthesia in
man has received much attention in recent years,
following a number of reports of people being
aware during surgery. A range of different
measures have been suggested for determining
the adequacy of anaesthesia in man, but such a
critical assessment is rarely applied to laboratory
animals. This article describes the methods used
to assess anaesthetic depth in both man and
animals, and compares the relative states of
knowledge about anaesthetic depth in animals
and man.
Keywords:
Animal
Anaesthesia;
Anaesthetic
depth;
Anaesthetics
are administered
to laboratory
animals for a variety of reasons. In some
instances, all that is required is a convenient and
humane method of restraint to enable a nonpainful manipulation to be carried out. On other
occasions, anaesthesia is required to allow major
operative procedures to be undertaken without
the animal experiencing pain. Since all anaesthetics have depressant effects on a range of body
systems, it is desirable to adjust the depth of
anaesthesia to be the minimum required for the
particular procedure which is to be undertaken.
In order to make this adjustment, some method
of assessing anaesthetic depth is required. It is,
of course, possible to use a very crude estimate
of depth of anaesthesia, and in these circumstances the situation is similar to that prevalent
Received 11 October 1991; accepted 12 March 1992
before the introduction of balanced anaesthesia
in humans:
and if the patient came off the
operating
table not too depressed,
not too
cyanotic and not too clammy to the touch, the
operation was deemed a success and the anaesthesia at least adequate, if not indeed thoroughly
satisfactory'
(Little & Stephen,
1954). This
approach is clearly outdated, and most research
workers would make some attempt to judge the
depth of anaesthesia. If the animal is too deeply
anaesthetized, then a higher incidence of death
due to anaesthetic overdose will occur; if the
animal is too lightly anaesthetized, then it may
experience pain.
As well as concerns related to the welfare of
laboratory
animals, other reasons exist for
assessing anaesthetic depth. Although anaesthesia
is necessary to provide unconsciousness, restraint
and abolition of pain sensation, the anaesthetic
agents used may interfere with the aims of the
experiment. Since the effects of anaesthetics are
almost always dependent upon the quantity
administered, reducing the depth of anaesthesia
to the lightest level possible may minimize this
interference and enable more meaningful scientific
data to be acquired. Many studies carried out
entirely under general anaesthesia also require
that a constant and reproducible depth of anaesthesia is maintained throughout the procedure.
Both requirements necessitate the development
of reliable and reproducible methods of assessing
depth of anaesthesia. The development of such
techniques becomes even more important when
neuromuscular blocking agents are administered.
Since these prevent all voluntary movement by
paralysing
the skeletal muscles, assessment
of anaesthetic
depth by the relatively crude
I •••
Downloaded from lan.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on March 3, 2014
Whelan & Flecknell
154
technique of testing withdrawal reflexes is no
longer possible.
A reproducible and reliable means of assessing
anaesthetic depth is also required when comparative studies of different anaesthetic regimens are
undertaken. Assessing which anaesthetic causes
least depression of, for example, cortical function,
blood pressure or respiratory rate, is meaningless
unless such an assessment is made at dose rates
which produce identical depths of anaesthesia.
Assessment of anaesthetic depth in man
Almost since the introduction of anaesthetic
agents, attempts have been made to quantify
their effects on the patient. Snow described 'five
degrees of narcotism' with ether in 1847, and in
1937 Guedel described the four stages of anaesthesia, subdividing the third stage (surgical
anaesthesia) into four planes. These were based
upon the reproducible and consistent loss of a
variety of reflexes, mostly noted during the
induction of anaesthesia.
Opioid premedication was introduced at the
turn of the century, followed by the introduction
of more potent volatile agents and, later still,
barbiturates. These new anaesthetic regimens
enabled the first two stages of anaesthesia to be
traversed very rapidly, and surgical anaesthesia
readily obtained. At this time the object of
anaesthesia was to keep the patient at a depth
of anaesthesia whereby respiratory and cardiovascular depression was minimal, yet the patient
was sufficiently anaesthetized for the procedure
to be carried out. The quotation from Little and
Stephen (1954) cited above illustrates this
perfectly. This approach was designed to reduce
morbidity and mortality as much as possible, and
is still the objective when most anaesthetics are
given today. At this time most anaesthetics
consisted of one or two drugs, and these had to
provide unconsciousness (or hypnosis), analgesia
and relaxation.
During the 1940s neuromuscular blocking
agents, starting with d-tubocurarine, began to
gain popularity. At first small doses were used,
and ventilation was assisted as necessary. This
then progressed to the use of much larger doses,
ventilation being assisted from the outset. This
technique of using neuromuscular blocking agents
to provide muscle relaxation, and low doses of
other drugs to provide unconsciousness and analgesia became known as 'balanced anaesthesia'.
As experience with the technique increased, it
was found that lower doses of anaesthetic drugs
could be used with consequently less cardiac and
respiratory depression. The advantage of these
'balanced anaesthetic' regimens was summed up
by Ogilvie (1954) when he said 'So fearful was
I of anaesthetics ...
from 1927-1939, I
performed all my gastrectomies and much of my
major surgery under local analgesia alone'. He
went on to say that 'The modern anaesthetist ...
presents the operator with tissues in a condition
to suit him, relieves him of all anxiety except for
the technical details of the operation itself, and
hands him back a patient alive and well and
about to come round'. However, neuromuscular
blocking agents provide no analgesia or hypnosis,
only immobility, so reports began to appear of
patients being able to recall events and conversations that took place (Winterbottom, 1950;
Bogetz & Katz, 1984; Lyons & MacDonald,
1991). This has led to considerable efforts to
improve the methods of assessing depth of
anaesthesia in man.
Definition of anaesthesia
The arrival of 'balanced anaesthesia' has led to
considerable discussion regarding the definition
of anaesthesia. A definition was offered by PrysRoberts (1987), that anaesthesia is the 'state in
which, as a result of drug-induced unconsciousness, the patient neither perceives nor recalls
noxious stimulation'. This implies that fluctuations in blood pressure, heart rate and other
autonomic responses traditionally associated
with light anaesthesia therefore have no relevance
as long as the patient is unconscious. An
alternative definition was offered by Pinsker
(1986), who regarded unconsciousness, paralysis
and attenuation of the stress response as the
requirements of anaesthesia. However, he
pointed out that with the use of neuromuscular
blocking agents, the only reliable way to monitor
Downloaded from lan.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on March 3, 2014
Assessment of depth of anaesthesia
unconsciousness was to ask the patient postoperatively! Pinsker also regarded analgesia as
not necessary for anaesthesia-if
a patient is
unconscious then there is no need for analgesia.
However, in a balanced regimen, if a patient is
conscious but is unable to move, analgesic drugs
may be the only thing preventing excruciating
pain being felt, and enormous psychological
damage to the patient. Prys-Roberts' (1987)
definition of anaesthesia includes the assumption
that loss of consciousness is an all or nothing
event, hence measuring 'depth of anaesthesia',
in so much as it classically implies a gradation
of anaesthesia, is not possible. This author
implies that all that should be done is to assess
whether the subject is unconscious and monitor
the physiological effects of anaesthetic agents on
the body at different doses, to ensure homeostasis is maintained.
This all or nothing view of consciousness is not
a universally held view. Other research workers
claim that loss of consciousness is a graded event
and that at different levels of unconsciousness,
different cerebral responses can be measured.
For example, at relatively deep planes of anaesthesia, patients may retain memories of conversations that occurred intra-operatively at a
subconscious level, if the content of the conversation is significant to the patient (Levenson 1965;
in this study the anaesthetist asked the surgeon
to stop as the patient was going blue!). The
extensive study of auditory evoked potentials (see
below) shows that cerebral response to sounds is
lost gradually. Russell (1989) goes so far as to
suggest that 'the state of anaesthesia may not
even include unconsciousness'. Artusio (1954)
described a series of operations he performed on
patients in a state of 'analgesia'. These patients
were able to converse with the author during
thoracic surgery, yet at no time complained of
pain, and remembered nothing post-operatively.
Use of the isolated forearm technique (see below)
suggests that in a balanced regimen, using low
doses of anaesthetic drugs, the patient is in a
similar state to that described by Artusio.
Although general anaesthesia has been
variously defined in animals (Hall & Clarke, 1983;
155
Lumb & Jones, 1984)these definitions seem to be
based on historical human anaesthetic practices.
Surgical anaesthesia in animals is rarely defined,
Lumb and Jones (1984)state 'Surgical anaesthesia
is unconsciousness, accompanied by muscular
relaxation to such a degree that surgery can be
performed painlessly and without struggling on
the part of the patient'. Since assessment of
consciousness in animals is rarely attempted, we
would suggest the following definition of surgical
planes of general anaesthesia in laboratory
animals as being the state in which an animal
is immobile, unaware of the procedure being
performed and has an attenuated stress response.
This does not necessarily imply that the animal
will be completely unresponsive to surgical
stimuli. Spinal reflexes and autonomic responses
may occur despite a lack of conscious awareness
(see below).
Monitoring the depth of anaesthesia in man
Although there is considerable discussion as to
the definition of general anaesthesia in man,
there is general agreement that it is necessary to
monitor the patient to try to prevent awareness,
pain sensation, recall or, at the other end of the
spectrum, life-threatening depression of body
systems. To achieve this aim, a variety of
parameters are monitored.
Gross signs
These include the loss of consciousness (depending
on how you view that event), failure to respond
to verbal stimuli, loss of reflexes such as
palpebral and corneal reflexes, reduction of
muscle tone, changes in the rate and depth of
breathing and assessment of the somatic response
to surgical stimuli. These responses are all
masked by neuromuscular blockade, but are
extensively used in day-to-day laboratory animal
anaesthesia where neuromuscular blocking agents
are rarely used. When neuromuscular blocking
agents are used in man, response to verbal and
surgical stimuli can be ascertained by the use of
the isolated forearm technique (Tunstall, 1977).
This involves applying a tourniquet to occlude
the blood supply to the forearm before the
Downloaded from lan.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on March 3, 2014
Whelan & Flecknell
156
neuromuscular
blocking agent is administered.
In this way the patient retains somatic control
over the limb, and the anaesthetist can monitor
the response to noxious stimuli and verbal
instructions
(usually to squeeze a switch in
response to a recorded message). This technique
has not become a part of routine anaesthetic
practice,
despite initial favourable
reports.
Breckenridge and Aitkenhead (1981) complained
that 'surgery proved impossible while the cuff
was inflated because of purposeful movement of
the arm' , and concluded that this was not a good
indicator of light anaesthesia. The problem in
this case was resolved by removing the tourniquet
thus causing paralysis of the arm. Post-operatively
they found no instances of either dreaming or
recall of intra-operative
events. It is interesting
to speculate, in the light of this information, on
how the whole body would react to surgery if no
neuromuscular
blocking drugs were given, and
raises the question as to whether or not we are
harming our animals by anaesthetizing
them
deeply to achieve immobility?
Gross signs, taken as a whole, can give a good
picture as to the depth of anaesthesia, but any
one sign viewed alone can be misleading. In the
'simple' anaesthetic regimens there are many
different gross criteria available for assessing
depth,_ so if one parameter is unusual there are
many others that can be used. It is generally
assumed that if the patient moves in response to
surgical stimulation
he (she) is insufficiently
anaesthetized, although this assumption may not
be correct, since movement may not indicate
conscious perception
of surgical stimuli (see
below).
Autonomic parameters
Generally,
an increase in pulse rate at the
time of surgical stimulation
is assumed to
indicate an inadequate
depth of anaesthesia,
and more anaesthetic is given until the response
is inhibited. The same is true of arterial blood
pressure; a significant rise is countered with
more anaesthetic (a significant rise is assumed
to be approximately 20070;Cullen, 1981). Sweating
and tear production
are treated in the same
way (both are absent in surgical planes of
anaesthesia).
Guedel described the use of the
pupil as a guide to depth in ether anaesthesia,
but these responses vary with individual anaesthetic agents (for example halothane tends to
constrict the pupil whereas nitrous oxide and
ether dilate the pupil), and with premedication
(for example atropine produces pupillary dilation
and opioids produce pupillary constriction).
However, some generalizations can still be made;
deep anaesthesia almost always produces an
absence of eye movement,
and reduces or
abolishes the light response (Cullen et al.,
1972).
However, the response may not necessarily
be due to the higher centres of the brain
perceiving pain, since it has been noted that
'brain dead' organ donor patients can show
haemodynamic
changes in response to surgery
(Wetzel et al., 1985), yet by definition there
can be no conscious perception of pain. In
the majority of cases though, if more anaesthetic agent is given after noting any of these
responses, and the change is reversed, then the
anaesthetist can be satisfied that the patient is
anaesthetized to the best possible extent using
these signs.
Unfortunately,
there have been a number of
cases where all these parameters were stable, yet
the patient was aware for the whole procedure
(Cullen, 1981). Whitacre and Fisher (1945)
describe a case where they used local anaesthesia
with curare for muscle relaxation. They said 'at
first the patient complained of considerable pain
and discomfort but at the end of one hour, after
200 mg of curare had been given, respiratory
arrest occurred and consciousness was lost'. It
is probable that the patient was aware of the
whole procedure (curare produces no analgesia
or alteration of consciousness),
yet his blood
pressure and pulse rate are reported as having
been constant. This, together with the fact that
no convincing evidence appears to have been
published to support the contention that these
changes accurately reflect the depth of anaesthesia, makes reliance on these criteria questionable (Hug, 1985).
Downloaded from lan.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on March 3, 2014
Assessmentof depth of anaesthesia
Minimum alveolar concentration and
depth of anaesthesia
Merkel and Eger (1963) first described the
concept of Minimum Alveolar Concentration
(MAC), which states that if a constant endexpiratory anaesthetic gas concentration is maintained for around 15min, then this could be
assumed to be in equilibrium with the blood and
brain gas concentrations. In experiments with
dogs he found that a relatively constant alveolar
gas concentration would suppress the response
(a gross purposeful movement) to the same
noxious stimulus in all dogs, although different
stimuli required different MAC values to suppress
responses. Eger then tried a similar experiment
in humans. Using no muscle relaxants and
inducing anaesthesia with the anaesthetic gas, he
was able to determine the alveolar gas concentration that would suppress the response to surgical
stimulation in 500/0 of patients. This value is the
MAC so (usually referred to as MAC). It is
interesting to note that although in 50llJo of the
patients movement occurred, no recall was
reported by any of his patients. Further work has
shown that N20 reduces the MAC value of
volatile agents (for example 70070 N20 reduces
halothane requirement by 61llJoand isoflurane by
60llJo;Cullen, 1986), that opioid analgesics reduce
MAC values (for example fentanyl reduced
isoflurane MAC by up to a maximum of
65-670/0; Cullen, 1986) and that MAC values are
different for different procedures (MAC for
incision of the skin is lower than that required
for intubation; Stanski, 1986). Also, extrapolation
of MAC soexperimental data can be used to find
the MAC9s figure, the end expiratory concentration that ensures that 95llJo of people will not
move in response to a defined stimulus. This,
however, implies that the majority of patients are
receiving an overdose of anaesthetic and so is of
limited usefulness.
Commercial in-line monitors of alveolar gas
concentration are readily available, in the form
of small in-theatre units and mass spectrometers
that take samples from numerous operating
theatres in rotation, although expense precludes
their use on a day-to-day basis in most animal
157
facilities. However, MAC values do exist for
anaesthesia of most commonly used laboratory
animals (Green, 1979; Cullen, 1986).
The use of MAC as a means of assessing
anaesthetic depth assumes that the lack of
purposeful movement in response to a noxious
stimulus equates with the loss of conscious
awareness of pain. This assumption may not be
valid, and movement responses may occur without conscious pain perception. If movement is
abolished, then this may result in the patient
being maintained at excessively deep levels of
anaesthesia.
Minimum infusion rate and minimum
intravenous concentration
With the increased awareness of anaesthetic gas
pollution, and the potential health risks to
theatre staff, there has been a trend towards total
intravenous anaesthesia using agents such as
thiopentone and propofol in conjunction with
infusions of opioid analgesics like fentanyl and
aIfentaniI. In this situation the main problem is
that the pharmacokinetics of the intravenous
agents are such that there tends to be a build up
in body tissues due to redistribution of the drugs
(metabolism being slow for barbiturates, but
more rapid for propofol). Also there are wide
differences between individual patients in their
responses to particular drugs.
The principle of a 'minimum infusion rate'
(MIR) for a particular drug has been proposed.
However, this tends to be calculated in the early
portion of anaesthesia when redistribution is at
its greatest, and does not allow for build up.
White and Kenny (1990) suggest that the MIR
should be regarded as equivalent to the setting
on a vaporizer, and that the equivalent to MAC
values for any drug is the 'minimum intravenous
concentration' (MIC) required to produce anaesthesia. Unfortunately the technology is not yet
available to allow intraoperative measurement of
MIC. What is available is computer assisted
constant infusion using a standard infusion
pump and a personal computer, together with
software that allows for the pharmacokinetics of
a particular drug, and also takes some account
Downloaded from lan.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on March 3, 2014
Whelan & Flecknell
158
of the patient's build and body composition.
Whilst being useful, it is still essential to monitor
the patient constantly to ensure the correct depth
of anaesthesia is maintained.
As with MAC, the use of MIR or MIC
assumes that the absence of movement ensures
that an appropriate depth. of anaesthesia has
been attained.
Electroencephalogram
From as early as 1875 it has been known that
anaesthetic drugs affect brain electrical activity,
and there have been numerous reports on the
effects of different anaesthetics on electroencephalogram (EEG) activity. The EEG reflects
the degree of cortical activity in a patient, and
can therefore be regarded as representing the
degree of central nervous system (CNS) depression
and therefore depth of anaesthesia. Courtin
et al. (1950) described the use of the raw EEG
to assess the depth of ether anaesthesia, but the
use of this method, and any other use of the raw
EEG, relies on subjective interpretation of the
signal which relies on the experience of the
operator.
The processed EEG offers more scope to the
less experienced operator for monitoring the
depth of anaesthesia. The raw signal is taken,
usually from two or three leads, and subjected
to Fast Fourier Transformation (FFT); a method
of processing that reduces the raw EEG signal
into its component parts, displaying them as a
graph of amplitude against frequency. FFT has
the advantage of allowing the processed EEG to
be displayed in real time, so changes can be
identified rapidly. The transformed signal can be
analysed further to produce individual parameters
that can be used to gauge anaesthetic depth.
One such method of processing which has been
investigated is the spectral edge frequency; the
frequency below which 950/0of the total power
lies. This produced reasonable results for assessing
anaesthetic depth, within the limited trials that
have been performed (Rampil & Matteo, 1987;
White & Boyle, 1989). A similar type of derivation is the median frequency; the frequency
which divides the total power of the EEG exactly
in two. Again, results for this are very promising
within the limited trials that have been performed
(Schwilden, 1989; Schwilden & Stoeckel, ]990).
Both these methods have yet to be assessed in
large trials in day to day anaesthetic practice,
with the usual 'cocktail' of drugs that this entails.
A commercial EEG monitor, the Cerebral
Function Analysing Monitor (CFAM) has been
used as an anaesthetic depth monitor (Frank &
Prior, 1987), together with the suggestion that
it could be linked to a computer program that
could recognize the signs of lightening of the
anaesthetic, and warn the anaesthetist of the
approaching problem. Unfortunately, to date no
reports have appeared describing the use of the
CFAM in this way.
Cortical evoked responses
When a patient is subjected to a repetItIve
stimulus, such as a series of audible clicks
[auditory evoked potential (AEP) ] , a changing
pattern of light [visual evoked potential (YEP) ]
or repeated stimulation of a peripheral nerve
[somatosensory evoked potential (SEP)], and
the EEG is averaged with respect to the
application of the stimulus, then a distinct
response can be found. This is seen as a series
of waves that represent the passage of information from the brainstem to the cortex. Anaesthesia
causes an increase in the time lag, or latency,
between the stimulus and different response
waves, which appears to be related to the depth
of anaesthesia. Unfortunately, these potentials
require the summation of at least 1000 responses
(Lloyd-Thomas et al., 1990), preferably more,
to produce the evoked potential, so the process
is not suited to a rapidly changing situation. Chi
et al. (1990) found that VEPs can be significantly
different with different agents. Again more work
is needed before a final assessment can be made.
Lower oesophageal contractility
This was investigated as a possible method of
assessing anaesthetic depth, owing to the fact
that lower oesophageal musculature is smooth
rather than striated muscle, and should be
unaffected by neuromuscular blockade. This was
Downloaded from lan.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on March 3, 2014
Assessment of depth of anaesthesia
found to be true, but various authors have
concluded that lower oesophageal contractibility
(LOC) is a poor indicator of anaesthetic depth
(Isaac, 1989; Pace, 1990) with large numbers of
false positives and negatives, leading to a poor
predictive value of 25070 (Bogod et al., 1990).
Anaesthetic depth in laboratory animals
Current anaesthetic methodology in laboratory
animals
The vast majority of animal anaesthetics are
performed using a limited number of agents; in
laboratory animals these are usually administered
as a single (intraperitoneal
or intramuscular)
injection,
and we rely on these to provide
adequate analgesia, hypnosis and muscle relaxation for the procedure to be performed. The dose
of an injectable agent is also calculated to
anaesthetize all the animals that it is administered
to; MAC and MIC studies both show that there
is a wide individual variation in response to a
drug, so the administration of a single bolus will
inevitably be overdosing many of the animals.
If the anaesthetic agent has a narrow safety
margin, then this will result in the deaths of many
animals, and an unnecessarily severe challenge
to many more. Administering intravenous anaesthetic agents slowly and to effect, or the use of
inhalant agents for the induction of anaesthesia
to a given stage is a far safer method and will
give greater consistency from animal to animal.
Mortality rates for laboratory animal anaesthesia
appear
to be published
relatively
infrequently; they range from 8% in guineapigs
(Hoar, 1969), 1'4% in rabbits (Mero et al.,
1987), to a number of studies that report zero
mortality (Jones & Simmons, 1968; Olson &
Renchko, 1988). The latter tend to be reports of
new techniques using a very limited number of
animals, and reliable data concerning the mortality associated with commonly used anaesthetics
in laboratory species are not available at the
present time. A recent report (Clarke & Hall,
1990) found an overall mortality
rate in
veterinary practice of one in 679 cases, compared
to the human figure of one in 10 000. The
attitude of researchers towards acceptable losses
159
varies however, from 1 . 4% being unacceptable
(Mero et al., 1989) to 9% mortality being
considered low (Wixson et al., 1987); obviously,
given our current concern for animal welfare we
should be aiming for the former attitude to
predominate.
The considerably higher success rate in human
anaesthesia seems to underline the inherent safety
of 'balanced anaesthesia', and also highlights the
value of the much more intense monitoring
during the recovery period that occurs with
human patients (a large proportion
of the
anaesthetic deaths in animals were thought to be
due to inadequate
airway protection).
These
comparisons
raise the possibility that we are
over-anaesthetizing
laboratory animals, and/or
are guilty of insufficient physiological monitoring, and that this is having a detrimental effect
on the well-being of the animals, and possibly
the validity of the experimental results which are
obtained.
Assessment of depth of anaesthesia in animals
In general, anaesthetic depth in laboratory species
is assessed by extrapolation
from the human
classification of depth, where, primarily, the
abolition of reflexes is used. However, the reflex
chosen has considerable
bearing upon the
apparent depth of anaesthesia.
For example,
Peeters et al. (1988) found that in the rabbit,
using loss of the pedal withdrawal reflex as
equating to surgical anaesthesia, administration
of pentobarbitone
resulted in the death of 5/6
of their animals through respiratory depression,
and was not, therefore, a suitable anaesthetic.
However, pentobarbitone
has been used for
many years to anaesthetize rabbits for surgical
procedures (although its use has declined with
the advent of safer regimens). To make matters
more confusing Peeters et al. also found that the
forelimb and hindlimb reflexes disappeared at
different anaesthetic depths. Similarly in the rat,
laparotomies have been performed on animals
that lay quietly and unresponsive throughout the
operation, yet a pedal withdrawal reflex was
present at the start and finish of the operation
(personal observation).
These animals had no
Downloaded from lan.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on March 3, 2014
Whelan & Fiecknell
160
response to pinching of the abdominal skin and
had a very sluggish response to tail pinching.
Again, there is a lack of information concerning
the loss of which reflex responses in a particular
species represents adequate anaesthesia for a
particular procedure.
Wixson et 01. (1987) performed perhaps the
most objective assessment of the effects of
anaesthesia on the rat. This included assessment
of depth of anaesthesia by the abolition of
various reflexes; unfortunately, these were not
related to the depth of anaesthesia required for
surgery, so we still do not have an indicator of
the lightest depth of anaesthesia that is still
compatible with safe and humane surgery.
The other mainstay of assessing anaesthetic
depth in humans is to assess the response to
a particular stimulus, usually the initial skin
incision; if the patient shows no response (which
could be anything from purposeful movement to
a significant rise in blood pressure), then the
anaesthesia is judged to be sufficient. This is not
used commonly in laboratory animals because
of concerns of causing pain to inadequately
anaesthetized animals. Interestingly, in the initial
studies to establish MAC values for volatile
anaesthetics, none of the human patients who
moved in response to a skin incision could
remember the event (Eger et 01., 1965), emphasizing once again that abolition of these reflex
movements may not be necessary to produce
surgical anaesthesia.
In larger animals, such as dogs, cats and
rabbits, the measurement of heart rate, pulse
and respiratory rate is very straightforward,
and should be performed regularly throughout
the anaesthetic period and into the recovery
period. Blood pressure monitoring, if available,
should also be used. These parameters are
used to monitor the depth of anaesthesia,
but as discussed earlier, the basis for this
appears unsubstantiated.
Monitoring these
parameters does, however, give a picture
of the physiological status of the animal.
This should enable the anaesthetist to
respond to any changes and provide a safer
anaesthetic.
Smaller animals are more difficult to monitor;
peripheral pulse is not palpable, respiratory rates
are rapid and difficult to monitor, so that even
a 300/0 drop may be difficult to pick up with
simple observation. This does not mean that
monitoring is any less desirable, and in certain
cases it may be more so. Heart rate can be
monitored by the use of an ECG machine, and
non-invasive blood pressure monitors are available for some species, so where possible these
should be used to assess the status of the animal.
Also a rectal thermometer should be used to
ensure hypothermia does not occur.
Neuromuscular blocking agents are used in
a variety of research applications. They are
frequently employed to allow intermittent positive
pressure ventilation, for example during thoracic
surgery. They are also used extensively during
neurophysiological studies to abolish skeletal
muscle responses that would interfere with the
neurone recordings which are being made. When
these agents are used in experimental animals in
the United Kingdom, the Home Office require
that continuous monitoring of heart rate and/or
blood pressure be performed to ensure adequate
anaesthesia (Home Office Guidelines, 1988).
Autonomic responses such as changes in heart
rate and blood pressure have been shown to be
highly variable, and not necessarily to equate
with a particular level of consciousness, with the
possibility of awareness occurring undetected. At
present, laboratory animal anaesthetists have
little choice other than to use autonomic parameters as indicators of anaesthetic depth. In
doing so, we may be producing dangerously deep
levels of anaesthesia which can result in unnecessary deaths of our subjects, or conversely
we may inadvertently cause the animals intense
suffering. Clearly there is still much work to be
done before we have the answer to this particular
problem.
Conclusions
Anaesthesia in man was long considered to be
an art, rather than a science. The use of
neuromuscular blocking agents resulted in a shift
in emphasis towards the scientific basis of
Downloaded from lan.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on March 3, 2014
Assessment
of depth of anaesthesia
anaesthesia, and produced a turn around in
survival rates of surgery. Many laboratory animal
anaesthetists and veterinary anaesthetists appear
unaware that a scientific approach to the practice
of anaesthesia is possible. The introduction of
a more critical assessment of anaesthetic practice,
particularly in relation to the assessment of
anaesthetic depth, should lead to improved
161
anaesthetic methodology, improved welfare for
laboratory animals, and an improvement in the
quality of scientific data obtained from experimental animals.
Acknowledgments
G Whelan was supported by the British Veterinary
Association Animal Welfare Foundation.
References
Artusio JF (1954) Diethyl ether analgesia: detailed description
of the first stage of ether anaesthesia in man. Journal of
Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics 111,
343-348
Bogetz MS & Katz JA (1984) Recall of surgery for major
trauma. Anaesthesiology 61, 6-9
Bogod DO, Orton JK, Yau HM & Oh TE (1990) Detecting
awareness during general anaesthetic Caesarian section.
Anaesthesia 45, 279-284
Breckenridge J & Aitkenhead AR (1981) Isolate forearm
technique for detection of wakefulness during general
anaesthesia. British Journal of Anaesthesia 53, 665-666
Chi OZ, Subramoni J & Jasaitis D (1990) Visual evoked
potentials during etomidate administration in humans.
Canadian Journal of Anaesthesia 37, 452-456
Clarke KW & Hall LW (1990)A survey of anaesthesia in small
animal practice: AVA/BSA VA report. Journal of The
Association of Veterinary Anaesthetists 17, 4-10
Courtin RF, Bickford RO & Faulconer A (1950) The
classification and significance of e1ectro-encephalographic
patterns produced by nitrous oxide-ether anaesthesia during
surgical operations. Proceedings of The Staff Meetings of
The Mayo Clinic 25, 197-206
Cullen D, Eger E, Stevens W, et al. (1972) Clinical signs of
anaesthesia. Anesthesiology 36,21-36
Cullen D (1981) Drugs and anesthetic depth. In Drug
Interactions in Anesthesia (eds Smith N, Miller RD
& Carbascio AN), pp 287-304. Philadelphia: Lea and
Febiger
Cullen DJ (1986)Anaesthetic depth and MAC. In Anaesthesia,
Vol I, Chap 17 (ed. Miller RD). London: Churchill
Livingstone
Eger EI, Saidman LJ & Brandstater B (1965) Minimum
alveolar anesthetic concentration: a standard of anesthetic
potency. Anesthesiology 26, 756-763
Frank M & Prior FP (1987) The cerebral function analysing
monitor: principles and potential use. In: Consciousness,
Awareness and Pain in General Anaesthesia (eds Rosen M
& Lunn IN), London: Butterworths
Green CJ (1979)General and balanced anaesthesia. In: Animal
Anaesthesia. London: Laboratory Animals Ltd
Guedel AE (1937) Inhalational Anesth...:ia, A Fundamental
Guide. New York: Macmillan
Hall LW & Clarke KW (1983) VeterinaryAnaesthesia, 8th edn,
p 4. London: Bailliere Tindall
Hoar RM (1969) Anaesthesia in the guinea pig. Federation
Proceedings 28, 1517-1521
Home Office (1988) Guidelines on the Use of Neuro-muscular
Blocking Agents. London: HMSO
Hug CC (1985) Lipid solubility, pharmacokinetics and the
EEG: are you better off now than you were four years ago?
Anesthesiology 62, 221-226
Isaac PA (1989) Lower oesophageal contractility and depth
of anaesthesia. In Bailliere's Clinical Anaesthesiology
(December 1989) Depth of Anaesthesia (ed. Jones JG).
London: Bailliere Tindall
Jones J8 & Simmons ML (1968) Innovar-Vet as an
intramuscular anesthetic for rats. Laboratory Animal Care
18, 642-643
Levenson BW (1965) States of awareness during general
anaesthesia. British Journal of Anaesthesia 37, 544-546
Little DM & Stephen CR (1954)Modern balanced anaesthesia:
a concept. Anesthesiology IS, 246-261
Lloyd-Thomas AR, Cole PV & Prior PF (1990) Quantitative
EEG and brainstem auditory evoked potentials: comparison
of isoflurane with halothane using the cerebral function
analysing monitor. British Journal of Anaesthesia 65,
306-312
Lumb WV & Jones EW (1984) Veterinary Anaesthesia, 2nd
edn, p 3. Philadelphia: Lea and Febiger
Lyons G & MacDonald R (1991)Awareness during Caesarian
section. Anaesthesia 46, 62-64
Merkel G & Eger EI (1963)A comparative study of halothane
and halopropane anaesthesia. Anesthesiology 24, 346-357
Mero M, Vainionpaa S, Vasenius J, Vihtonen K & Rokkanen
P (1989) Medetomidine-ketamine-diazepam anaesthesia in
the rabbit. Acta Vet Scand 85, 135-137
Mero M, Makela A, Vainionpaa S, Vihtonen K & Rokkanen
P (1987) The use of neuroleptanaesthesia for experimental
orthopaedic surgery in the rabbit. Acta Veterinaria
Scandinavica (Copenhagen) 28, 251-252
Ogilvie H (1954) The future of surgery. British Medical
Journal 2, 1435-1439
Olson ME & Renchko P (1988) Azaperone and azaperoneketamine as a sedative and anesthetic in rats and mice.
Laboratory Animal Science 38, 299-304
Pace NL (1990) Evaluation of new medical devices.
Anesthesiology 72, 953
Peeters ME, Oil D, Teske E, Eyzenbach V, Van Der Brom
WE, Lumeij JT & De Vries HW (1988) Four methods of
general anaesthesia in the rabbit: a comparative study.
Laboratory Animals 22, 355-360
Pinsker MC (1986) Anaesthesia: a pragmatic construct.
Anaesthesia and Analgesia 65, 819-827
Downloaded from lan.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on March 3, 2014
Whelan & Frecknell
162
Prys-Roberts C (1987) Anaesthesia: a practical or impractical
construct. British Journal of Anaesthesia 59, 1341-1345
Rampil IJ & Matteo RS (]987) Changes in EEO spectral edge
frequency correlate with the hemodynamic response to
laryngoscopy and intubation. Anesthesiology 67, 139-142
Russell IF (1989) Awareness during general anaesthesia. In
Bailliere's Clinical Anesthesiology (December 1989) Depth
of Anaesthesia (ed. Jones JO). London: Bailliere Tindall
Schwilden H (1989) Use of the median EEG frequency and
pharmacokinetics in determining depth of anaesthesia. In
Bail/iere's Clinical Anaesthesiology (December 1989) Depth
of Anaesthesia (ed. Jones JO). London: Bailliere Tindall
Schwilden H & Stoeckel H (1990) Effective therapeutic
infusions produced by closed-loop feedback control of
methohexital administration during total intravenous
anaesthesia with fentanyl. Anesthesiology 73, 225-229
Snow J (1847) On The Inhalation of the Vapours of Ether
in Surgical Operations. Containing a Description of the
Various Stages of Etherisation, and a Statement of the
Result of Nearly Eighty Operations in Which Ether has
been Employed in St George's and University College
Hospitals. London: John Churchill
Stanski DR (1986) Monitoring depth of anaesthesia. In
Anaesthesia (ed. Miller RD), Vol 2, Chap 30. London:
Churchill Livingstone
Tunstall ME (1977) Detecting awareness during Caesarian
section. British Medical Journal 1, 132]
Wetzel RC, Setzer N, Stiff JL & Rogers MC (1985)
Hemodynamic responses in organ donor patients.
Anaesthesia and Analgesia 64, 125-128
Whitacre RJ & Fisher AJ (]945) Clinical observations on
the use of curare in anaesthesia. Anaesthesiology 6,
]24-130
White PF & Boyle WA (1989) Relationship between hemodynamic and electroencephalographic changes during
general anaesthesia. Anaesthesia and Analgesia 68, 177-18]
White M & Kenny ONC (1990) Intravenous propofol
anaesthesia using a computerised infusion system.
Anaesthesia 45, 204-209
Winterbottom E (1950) Insufficient anaesthesia. British
Medical Journal 1, 247-248
Wixson SK, White WJ, Hughes HC, Lang CM & Marshall
WK (1987) A comparison of pentobarbital, fentanyldroperidol, ketamine-xylaxine and ketamine-diazepam
anaesthesia in adult male rats. Laboratory Animal Science
37, 726-730
Downloaded from lan.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on March 3, 2014