Download Language related brain potentials in patients with cortical and

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts
no text concepts found
Transcript
Brain (1999), 122, 1033–1047
Language related brain potentials in patients with
cortical and subcortical left hemisphere lesions
Angela D. Friederici, D. Yves von Cramon and Sonja A. Kotz
Max Planck Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, Leipzig
Correspondence to: Angela D. Friederici, Max Planck
Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, Stephanstrasse 1a,
PO Box 500355, D-04303 Leipzig, Germany
E-mail: [email protected]
Summary
The role of the basal ganglia in language processing is
currently a matter of discussion. Therefore, patients with
left frontal cortical and subcortical lesions involving the
basal ganglia as well as normal controls were tested in a
language comprehension paradigm. Semantically
incorrect, syntactically incorrect and correct sentences
were presented auditorily. Subjects were required to listen
to the sentences and to judge whether the sentence heard
was correct or not. Event-related potentials and reaction
times were recorded while subjects heard the sentences.
Three different components correlated with different
language processes were considered: the so-called N400
assumed to reflect processes of semantic integration;
the early left anterior negativity hypothesized to reflect
processes of initial syntactic structure building; and a late
positivity (P600) taken to reflect second-pass processes
including re-analysis and repair. Normal participants
showed the expected N400 component for semantically
incorrect sentences and an early anterior negativity
followed by a P600 for syntactically incorrect sentences.
Patients with left frontal cortical lesions displayed an
attenuated N400 component in the semantic condition. In
the syntactic condition only a late positivity was observed.
Patients with lesions of the basal ganglia, in contrast,
showed an N400 to semantic violations and an early
anterior negativity as well as a P600 to syntactic violations,
comparable to normal controls. Under the assumption
that the early anterior negativity reflects automatic firstpass parsing processes and the P600 component more
controlled second-pass parsing processes, the present
results suggest that the left frontal cortex might support
early parsing processes, and that specific regions of the
basal ganglia, in contrast, may not be crucial for early
parsing processes during sentence comprehension.
Keywords: left hemisphere lesions; syntactic-semantic processing; event-related potentials; P600; N400
Abbreviations: AAT 5 Aachen Aphasia Test; ANOVA 5 analysis of variance; BA 5 Brodmann area; ELAN 5 early left
anterior negativity; EOG 5 electro-oculogram; ERP 5 event-related brain potential
Introduction
Since the early discoveries of the language–brain relationship
(Broca, 1856; Wernicke, 1874; Geschwind, 1965; Goodglass,
1993) there has been a long debate about which cortical and
subcortical areas support language processes. The classical
brain lesion–behaviour approaches suggest the following
interpretation: patients with left hemisphere posterior
(temporoparietal) lesions including Wernicke’s area speak
fluently and produce at least parts of a sentence with
seemingly correct syntactic structures. However, their speech
and their comprehension reflect limitations to process content
words such as nouns and verbs (e.g. Berndt et al., 1997).
The underlying deficit in these patients has been defined
either as a deficit in the semantic representation of the mental
lexicon (Zurif et al., 1974) or as a deficit ‘in accessing and
operating on semantic properties of the lexicon’ (Milberg
and Blumstein, 1981, p. 381; Blumstein et al., 1982).
© Oxford University Press 1999
Syntactic processing has been linked with the left frontal
cortex including Broca’s area, while lexical processing has
been connected with the left temporal and frontal cortex (for
a review, see Goodglass, 1993). In some cases, patients
with frontolateral lesions in the left hemisphere produce
agrammatic sentences. Short sentences in which function
words and other grammatical morphemes are missing are a
typical example (e.g. Goodglass, 1976). Also, formal testing
that requires the correct interpretation of an underlying
syntactic structure often revealed agrammatic comprehension
(Caramazza and Zurif, 1976; Heilmann and Scholes, 1976).
More recently, this comprehension deficit has been defined
as a failure of fast access of syntactic information (Friederici,
1985; Kolk and Van Grunsven, 1985; Friederici and Kilborn,
1989; Haarmann and Kolk, 1991). In these studies it was
shown that Broca patients, although delayed in their on-
1034
A. D. Friederici et al.
line use of syntactic information, nonetheless, demonstrate
syntactic knowledge as indicated by a syntactic priming
effect (Friederici and Kilborn, 1989; Haarmann and Kolk,
1991). Studies that have investigated aphasia and the
particular brain structures involved, found that persisting
Broca’s aphasia resulted from lesions in Broca’s area, the
underlying white matter compartment and the anterior insula,
but not from focal lesions in Broca’s area itself (Mohr, 1976;
Dronkers et al., 1992). The combined findings suggest that
the left frontolateral cortex is engaged in fast and automatic
procedures necessary for normal production and
comprehension, while syntactic knowledge as such may be
represented elsewhere. This conclusion is further supported
by studies showing that agrammatic patients are able to judge
a sentence’s grammaticality (Linebarger et al., 1983).
Apart from these classical cortical language areas it has
been disputed whether and to what extent subcortical areas
are part of the network supporting language processing
(Damasio et al., 1982; Naeser et al., 1982; Varney and
Damasio, 1986; Alexander et al., 1987; Robin and Schienberg,
1990; Nadeau and Crosson, 1997). While Nadeau and Crosson
(1997) deny a direct role of the basal ganglia in language
processing, others assume such a direct relationship, in
particular in language production (Alexander et al., 1987;
Robin and Schienberg, 1990). It has been proposed that the
basal ganglia might subserve the processing of grammatical
information in both language production and comprehension
(Lieberman et al., 1992; Grossman et al., 1993). Ullman and
colleagues (Ullman et al., 1997) specified this view by
suggesting that the basal ganglia together with frontal regions
are supporting grammatical procedures rather than the
representation of declarative grammatical knowledge. Thus,
these studies suggest that the basal ganglia are involved in
syntactic procedures in particular.
The brain lesion–behaviour approach has been
complemented by neuroimaging studies, such as PET and
functional MRI, which provide further evidence that the
superior temporal gyrus and the supramarginal gyrus support
lexical processes during auditory language comprehension
(Petersen et al., 1989; Zatorre et al., 1996). These types of
studies have led to a more detailed functional description of
the frontal cortices. It appears that the Brodmann areas (BA)
45/47/46 are involved in semantic memory (Kapur et al.,
1994), whereas BA 44 is active during phonologically
sequencing (Buckner et al., 1995) as well as during
grammatical processing (Stromswold et al., 1996).
Rephrasing these latter findings it could be argued that BA
44 supports the application of phonotactic and of syntactic
rules or procedures.
Language-related brain potentials
Although these approaches have advanced our knowledge
concerning the particular subparts of the brain involved in
language processing, they do not suffice to describe the
interplay of different functional subsystems in time. The
measurement of event-related brain potentials (ERPs), whose
temporal resolution is in the millisecond domain, attempts to
study the temporal course and interplay between different
neurofunctional subsystems involved in language processing
(Kutas and Hillyard, 1983; Neville et al., 1991; Friederici
et al., 1993).
ERPs are small voltage oscillations that are measured at
the scalp surface and are time-locked to the processing of
external events. They involve a sequence of deflections (i.e.
components) that reflect stages of information processing in
the brain. Language-related ERP components with different
temporal and spatial characteristics of brain activity have
been identified (for an overview, see Kutas and Van Petten,
1988). Three different ERP components have been reported
to correlate with different aspects of language processing
during comprehension. Semantic processes are reflected in
the so-called N400 component, a centroparietally distributed
negativity that shows up about 400 ms after the onset of a
word, which is not expected given the prior semantic context
(e.g. Kutas and Hillyard, 1983). Studies that utilized
intracranial electrodes recordings suggest the anterior part of
the inferior temporal lobe as a possible generator for the
scalp recorded N400 (Smith et al., 1986; McCarthy and
Nobre, 1995; Nobre and McCarthy, 1995). Syntactic
processes have been found to correlate with two scalprecorded ERP components: a left anterior negativity either
between 150 and 200 ms for phrase structure violations (e.g.
Neville et al., 1991; Friederici et al., 1993) or between
300 and 500 ms for morphosyntactic agreement violations
between lexical elements (e.g. subject–verb agreement)
¨
(Rosler et al., 1993; Osterhout and Mobley, 1995; Gunter
¨
et al., 1997; Munte et al., 1997; Coulson et al., 1998) and a
late centroparietally distributed positivity around 600 ms, the
so-called P600 (i.e. Osterhout and Holcomb, 1992). [Note
that it is still a matter of debate whether the latency difference
of the negativity is due to the functional distinction between
the type of syntactic errors under investigation (phrase
structure versus other types), and/or to the speed of the
input processes (optimal, i.e. normal speech rate in auditory
perception and high contrast in visual perception versus nonoptimal, i.e. word-by-word presentation with pauses between
each word). However, from a linguistic perspective there is
a principle difference between information concerning the
categorical features (word category) and the agreement
features (person, number, gender and case) (Chomsky, 1995).
It is not unlikely that this difference is reflected in the
processes under observation.]
So far, the early left anterior negativity (ELAN) between
150 and 200 ms has only been observed with outright
syntactic phrase structure violations (Neville et al., 1991;
Friederici et al., 1993; Hahne and Friederici, 1999), whereas
the P600 has been found with syntactically non-preferred
structures as well as with outright syntactic violations
(Osterhout and Holcomb, 1992; Hagoort et al., 1993). We
have proposed that the two components, namely the early left
anterior negativity and the P600, reflect different processing
Language related brain potentials
stages during syntactic parsing (Friederici, 1995). This
proposal assumes two parsing stages: an automatic first-pass
parsing stage during which the initial structure is built upon
purely syntactic information and a second stage during which
syntactic and semantic information is mapped onto each
other (see also Frazier, 1987). It should be noted that there
are a number of alternative psycholinguistic models that have
been proposed during recent years. The discussion about the
ultimate description of the processes underlying language
comprehension is still ongoing (for a recent review, see
Kempen, 1998).
Here it is assumed that the early left anterior negativity
reflects initial structure building. The early stage of initial
phrase structure building involves the identification of the
incoming word’s syntactic category (i.e. article, noun, verb,
preposition) upon which a local syntactic structure is built.
For example, the sequence the man is identified as an article–
noun–sequence, which can be structurally represented as a
noun phrase. Other sequences are represented as verb phrases
or prepositional phrases—structural chunking of this kind
not only provides the parsing system with information
necessary to build up structural hierarchies and relationships
between various phrases (e.g. [S[NP the man] [VP[V stands]
[pp on the chair]]]), it also allows the memory system to hold
more information (i.e. it does not have to memorize a list of
isolated words, but structured phrases). The P600 is taken to
reflect secondary parsing processes including re-analysis or
repair depending on whether the sentence under consideration
has a correct but non-preferred structure (e.g. object-first
sentences) or an incorrect structure (e.g. sentences containing
an illegal sequence of words). More recently it has been
proposed that the P600 is an index of the difficulty of
syntactic integration processes in general, rather than of reanalysis and repair processes in particular (Kaan et al., 1998).
Thus, although the functional specification of the two syntax
related ERP effects known from the literature is still ongoing,
it appears that the early anterior negativity reflects a highly
automatic, early process during syntactic parsing while the
P600 reflects later, more controlled processes. This notion is
based on the observation that the latter, in contrast to the
former, is under the influence of strategic control (Coulson
et al., 1998; Hahne and Friederici, 1999) and under the
influence of semantic variables (Gunter et al., 1997).
One way of combining the classical but static brain
lesion–behaviour approach and the more dynamic though
neurotopologically less specific approach is to register eventrelated brain potentials in patients with focal brain lesions.
This line of research has been applied quite successfully in
the domain of attention and memory (Knight et al., 1989;
Knight, 1997). More recently ERPs have been used in the
domain of language comprehension (Hagoort et al., 1996;
Swaab et al., 1997; Friederici et al., 1998). Hagoort and
colleagues (Hagoort et al., 1996) and Swaab and colleagues
(Swaab et al., 1997) investigated semantic aspects of language
processing in aphasic patients with classified syndromes as
determined by a standard aphasia test. They reported a
1035
general decrease in the N400 component in both Broca’s
aphasics and Wernicke’s aphasics. However, the N400
amplitude varied as a function of the degree of the
comprehension deficit in the patients. The authors concluded
that ‘comprehension deficits in the aphasic patients are due
to an impairment in integrating individual word meanings
into an overall meaning representation’ (Hagoort et al., 1996,
p. 627).
Friederici and colleagues (Friederici et al., 1998)
investigated semantic and syntactic processing during
language comprehension in the case of two aphasics with
distinctive lesions by employing the ERP method. Although
inferences on ERP data from single subjects must be
interpreted with caution, we believe that the reported double
dissociation is of interest. The absence of one languagerelated ERP component in the presence of another may
suggest a processing failure in a particular language domain.
Patient G.R. with a circumscribed lesion in the posterior part
of the left hemisphere (involving the posterior third of the
upper temporal lobe, presumably the superior temporal gyrus)
and the adjacent portion of the parietal lobe (presumably the
inferior angular and less likely the supramarginal gyrus) did
show an ELAN, but no N400 or P600. Patient W.S. with an
extended lesion restricted to the anterior part of the left
hemisphere [involving the superior frontal gyrus in its full
length, the middle frontal gyrus (except its most rostral
portion), the opercular and an adjacent band of the triangular
part of the inferior frontal gyrus, the rostral and upper insula
including the external capsules, and the frontodorsal white
matter down to the moderately enlarged lateral ventricle]
showed an N400 component correlated with semantic
processes, a P600 correlated with late syntactic processes,
but no early left anterior negativity correlated with the
processes of fast initial structure building. In this patient the
caudate nucleus and the putamen were intact, suggesting that
the observed deficit was due to the cortical lesions. From
these findings we concluded that left frontal areas subserve
the fast processes of initial structure building reflected in the
early left anterior negativity, and that left temporoparietal
areas support aspects of lexical semantic processing reflected
in the N400 component. However, the nature of this lexical–
semantic processing is unclear. At a functional level it
was proposed that the N400 reflects processes of lexical
integration rather than lexical processing per se (Chwilla
et al., 1996). At a neuroanatomical level it has been claimed
that the generators of the N400 are located in the anterior
portion of the fusiform gyrus (McCarthy et al., 1995; Nobre
and McCarthy, 1995). It remains open to discussion which
aspects of lexical processing are subserved by the left superior
and middle temporal gyri.
The present study
The present ERP study investigated the auditory
comprehension of syntactic and semantic information in
subjects with different circumscribed cortical and subcortical
1036
A. D. Friederici et al.
Fig. 1 All patients suffered a left-sided ischaemic/haemorrhagic stroke. The cortical group (patients
009, 019, 102) displays both cortical as well as subcortical lesions while in the basal ganglia group
lesions are restricted to the neostriatum or the globus pallidum (patient 157). Note that the right side in
the figure indicates a left-hemispheric lesion. For details see Table 1 and text.
lesions within the frontal areas of the left hemisphere. The
experiment included aetiologically homogeneous patients
who had suffered an ischaemic stroke. In addition, three
patients (017, 019 and 157) showed a likely additional
haemorrhagic component of the infarct. The patient groups
were classified by their lesion sites: (i) patients (hereafter
denoted as ‘cortical group’) with left anterior lesions including
parts of the frontolateral cortex and of the neostriatum
(caudate nucleus, putamen), and (ii) patients (hereafter
denoted as ‘basal ganglia group’) with lesions restricted to
the left basal ganglia (caudate nucleus, putamen, globus
pallidus).
Language related brain potentials
1037
Table 1 Lesion sites
Left hemisphere lesions
Patients
009
019
102
017
048
110
157
Inferior frontal gyrus, triangular part
Inferior frontal gyrus, opercular part
Precentral gyrus, inferior part
Anterior insula
(1)
1
–
1
(1)
1
1
(1)
1
1
1
1
–
–
–
–
–
–
–
–
–
–
–
–
–
–
–
–
Caudate nucleus
Putamen
Globus pallidus
1
1
–
–
1
–
–
–
–
1
1
–
1
1
–
–
1
–
–
–
1
Anterior sub-/preinsular white matter
Corona radiata
Internal capsule, anterior limb
External capsules
1
1
(1)
(1)
–
1
–
1
(1)
–
–
1
–
(1)
1
–
–
–
(1)
(1)
(1)
1
(1)
–
–
–
–
–
Description of lesions determined by available CT scans or MRI scans for each individual patient in
the cortical and basal ganglia group. 1 5 unequivocal lesion; (1) 5 likely lesion; – 5 no lesion.
Given the types of violations investigated we focused on
three language-related ERP components: the N400, the early
left anterior negativity and the P600. Subjects listened to
auditorily presented correct and incorrect sentences. The
semantic condition contained sentences with selectional
restriction violations, while the syntactic condition included
sentences with word category violations. On the basis of a
previous case study (Friederici et al., 1998) we predicted
that subjects in the cortical group should show no early left
anterior negativity, but a N400 and a P600. Predictions with
respect to the basal ganglia group were less straightforward.
If the basal ganglia are involved in grammatical procedures
(Ullman et al., 1997) one might expect the absence of the
early left anterior negativity, whereas components that reflect
controlled processes such as the P600 component (secondpass syntactic processes) and the N400 component (postlexical integration) should be present. If, however, the basal
ganglia are not primarily involved in processes related to
language comprehension (e.g. Nadeau and Crosson, 1997)
patients with lesions restricted to the subcortical structures
should demonstrate all three ERP components.
Methods
Subjects
Two groups of brain-damaged subjects and 14 non-braindamaged controls participated in the experiment. The seven
patients were classified into the two groups according to their
lesion sites: (i) the cortical group included three patients with
lesions including the left inferior frontal gyrus, the anterior
insula and parts of the neostriatum; (ii) the basal ganglia
group included four patients with exclusively subcortical
lesions restricted to the caudate nucleus and/or to some parts
of the lenticular nucleus. T1/T2 and 3D datasets were
inspected by two experienced neuroradiologists and no
cortical damage was detected in the basal ganglia group.
Details concerning the patients’ lesion sites are displayed in
Fig. 1 and are listed in Table 1. The individual patient history
is presented in Table 2, including information about age,
gender, time of stroke onset to ERP measurement, specified
scores in the Aachen Aphasia Test (AAT) and premorbid
handedness.
For each patient there were two control subjects matched
in age and gender. They were all right-handed (Oldfield,
1971). The control group for the cortical patients consisted
of four female and two male subjects with a mean age of
42.6 years (C1 5 27 years; C2 5 24 years; C3 5 42 years;
C4 5 46 years; C5 5 58 years; C6 5 59 years). The control
group for the basal ganglia patients consisted of two female
and six male subjects with a mean age of 42.8 years (B1 5
31 years; B2 5 32 years; B3 5 33 years; B4 5 35 years;
B5 5 50 years; B6 5 53 years; B7 5 53 years; B8 5 56
years). All subjects gave informed consent to participate in
the study.
Material
The language material was similar to that used in earlier
studies (Friederici et al., 1998; Groβ et al., 1998; Hahne and
Friederici, 1999). The auditory material was first recorded
on analogue tape and then digitized (20 kHz, 12 bit
resolution). The recording of the sentences was controlled
for pronunciation, prosody and loudness. To ensure that
listeners did not perceive any prosodic or acoustic cues prior
to the critical word in the syntactically incorrect sentences,
correct sentences of types (c) and (d) were recorded (see
examples below). By means of an editing tool the nouns
following a preposition were then spliced out from the
digitized correct sentence. Again, measures were taken to
control for co-articulation. Only nouns with the same
phonological transitions from preposition offset to noun onset
and from noun offset to participle onset were used. Mean
loudness and splicing quality was assessed by an expert. No
1038
A. D. Friederici et al.
Table 2 Patient history
Patient group
Classification
Age
Sex
(years)
Hand
Onset to ERP
measurement
Cortical
Patient 009
Patient 019
Patient 102
Broca
Residual aphasia
Residual aphasia
53
26
41
F
F
M
R
R
R
18 months
12 months
18 months
Basal
Patient
Patient
Patient
Patient
Residual aphasia
Residual aphasia
Residual aphasia
No aphasia
33
38
58
55
F
M
M
M
R
R
R
R
18
8
6
4
017
048
110
157
months
months
months
months
Token
Test
AAT comprehension
score
AAT auditory
comprehension score
5
7
3
99/120
120/120
110/120
47/60
60/60
52/60
15
1
0
0
77/120
111/120
104/120
–
36/60
56/60
52/60
–
Individual patient history. Patients are grouped by their lesion site. Included are the average time between the onset of the stroke to the
ERP measurement. The severity of the language comprehension disorder is indicated by the number of mistakes in the Token Test: no/
very mild disorder (0–6); light (7–23); severe (.40). In addition, the overall comprehension scores of the Aachen Apahsia Test (AAT)
averaged across both the visual and auditory modalities are listed for each patient. The degree of the comprehension disorder is as
follows: no/mild (107–120); light (90–106); middle (67–89); severe (1–66). Since the current task was an auditory sentence judgement
task we also report the individual patient scores of the auditory comprehension subtest of the AAT which tests both the word and the
sentence level of comprehension.
significant difference in loudness between the spliced and
the unspliced conditions was observed.
Subjects listened to 192 sentences spoken by a trained
female speaker. Half of these sentences were correct, 25%
semantically incorrect and 25% syntactically incorrect. The
semantic violation was due to a selectional restriction error
[(a) Das Gewitter wurde gebu¨gelt/The thunderstorm was
ironed] and the syntactic violation resulted from a word
category error [(b) Die Hose wurde am gebu¨gelt/The pants
was on ironed (literal translation)]. A preposition such as
am/on must be followed by a noun phrase. Therefore, a
preposition followed by a verbform violates German phrase
structure rules. Also, since German is a verb-second language
the passive construction requires the verb in the sentence
ending position. For example, a semantically correct form of
sentence (a) would be (c) Das Hemd wurde gebu¨gelt/The
shirt was ironed. The correct counterpart of syntactically
incorrect sentence (b) would be (d) Die Bluse wurde am
Freitag gebu¨gelt/The blouse was on Friday ironed (literal
translation). The critical word was the same in all conditions.
Procedures
Participants were seated in a comfortable chair and listened
to the stimuli via loudspeaker boxes. While listening to the
sentences, subjects were instructed to fixate a small star in
the middle of a CRT screen in front of them and to avoid
blinking during the presentation of the star. The star appeared
500 ms prior to the auditory sentence presentation and
remained on the screen until 3000 ms after the offset of a
sentence. A response sign was then presented for 2000 ms.
During this time subjects were required to respond via push
buttons whether a sentence was correct or incorrect. The next
trial started after an inter-stimulus interval of 1000 ms.
ERP recordings
The EEG was recorded with tin electrodes secured in an
elastic cap. Nineteen electrodes were placed according to the
International 10–20 system with the following locations: FZ,
CZ, PZ, F7, F3, ATL, BL, WL, P7, P3, O1, F8, F4, ATR,
BR, WR, P4, P8, O2 (cf. Sharbrough, 1991). Each EEG
channel was amplified with a bandpass from DC to 40 Hz.
The EEG was recorded continuously and stored for later
analysis at a sampling rate of 250 Hz. Separate ERPs were
averaged (100 ms post-onset stimulus baseline) for each
subject at each electrode site. A post-stimulus baseline, timelocked to the onset of the critical word, rather than a
prestimulus baseline was applied since different word types
(auxiliary versus preposition) were used before the critical
word (cf. Hahne and Friederici, 1999). All electrodes were
referenced to linked mastoids and the impedance was reduced
to below 5 kΩ. Both the vertical electro-oculogram (EOG)
and the horizontal EOG were recorded from electrodes placed
above and below the right eye and the outer canthus of each
eye, respectively. Trials with eye blinks or horizontal eye
movements and other artefacts were removed from the raw
data prior to averaging the data. On average artefact rejection
was 8% in the control groups. Since there was an increase
in eye artefacts in the patient groups (up to 13%) we applied
an eye artefact control measure (Pfeifer et al., 1995) to
ensure that there were sufficient trials to calculate averages
for correctly answered trials. To do this we recorded the
EOG, applied a principal components analysis (PCA) yielding
two statistically independent EOG components and
determined their ‘propagation’ into the individual EEG
channels via linear regression. To correct an EEG channel
the corresponding PCA-transformed EOG components were
weighted with the propagation factors for this channel and
were finally subtracted from the EEG. The ERPs were time-
Language related brain potentials
locked to the onset of the critical word in each sentence and
then calculated from the onset of the critical word until 1500
ms post-stimulus onset.
Data analysis
Correct responses
Correct responses were subject to between-group (Patient
versus Group) analyses of variance (ANOVAs) with a withinfactor Condition (syntactic or semantic). Two separate
ANOVAs were performed to assess behavioural changes due
to cortical and subcortical lesions.
ERPs
ERP data for normal control groups and patient groups for
correct responses were analysed using standard ANOVAs.
Huynh–Feldt corrections (Huynh and Feldt, 1970) were
applied for inappropriate degrees of freedom due to violations
of the sphericity assumption. For each violation type
(syntactic and semantic) an ANOVA was calculated including
the between-subjects factor Group (Patient versus Control),
the within-subjects factor Condition (correct/incorrect) and
the within-subjects factor Electrode Site. Electrode sites for
the statistical analyses were defined as regions of interest for
each critical component. Critical components observed in the
experiment for patient and normal control groups were an
early anterior negativity, a centroparietal positivity (P600)
and a centroparietal negativity (N400). Therefore, electrode
sites for the early left anterior negativity were F7, F3, ATL,
BL, for the P600 CZ, PZ, P3, P4, P7, P8, and for the N400
CZ, WL, WR, P3, PZ, P4. Prior to measuring the mean
amplitude of each respective component time-windows were
defined by visual inspection of the waveforms in both patient
and control groups and then matched between groups for
statistical analyses. The time windows for the respective
statistical analyses of the components were early left anterior
negativity (150–300 ms), P600 (300–800 ms) and N400
(300–600 ms). Significant interactions that included the factor
Electrode Site were normalized with the McCarthy and Wood
procedure (McCarthy and Wood, 1985).
1039
Table 3 Individual judgement performance: percentage
correct responses of normal controls and patients
Participants
Correct
Semantic
violation
Phrase structure
violation
Cortical
Controls
1
2
3
4
5
6
99
96
100
100
100
98
100
100
100
92
100
94
100
100
100
100
100
96
Patients
1
2
3
94
89
100
94
92
92
96
84
100
Basal ganglia
Controls
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
99
99
98
100
97
100
99
93
100
100
100
96
98
90
98
96
100
98
100
100
98
100
96
98
91
98
97
97
93
100
100
100
84
98
100
98
Patients
1
2
3
4
Summary of the individual patient and normal control
performance for the correct, semantically incorrect and
syntactically incorrect conditions. Data is presented as total
percentage correct.
Results
Behavioural data
0.50, P . 0.503], indicating that patients and controls did
not differ in the amount of correct responses to syntactically
correct and incorrect sentences.
The analysis of correct responses in the semantic condition
revealed a main effect of Group [F(1,7) 5 9.47, P , 0.017]
but no main effect of Condition [F(1,7) 5 0.73, P . 0.420]
nor any interaction between Group and Condition [F(1,7) 5
0.02, P . 0.884]. These data show that both patients and
controls responded similarly to correct and incorrect semantic
sentences. However, patients judged both semantically correct
and incorrect sentences at a lower percentage correct rate
than controls.
Percentage correct responses for each subject and each
condition (syntactically correct versus incorrect; semantically
correct versus incorrect) are displayed in Table 3.
Basal ganglia group and controls
Cortical group and controls
The analysis of correct responses in the syntactic condition
did not show any group differences [F(1,7) 5 3.84, P ,
0.090], differences based on Condition [F(1,7) 5 0.02, P .
0.891] nor an interaction of Group 3 Condition [F(1,7) 5
The analysis for correct responses in the syntactic condition
did not reveal any main effects of Group 3 Condition, Group
[F(1,10) 5 2.49, P . 0.145], Condition [F(1,10) 5 0.06, P
. 0.813] or any interaction [F(1,10) 5 0.47, P . 0.509].
There were no significant main effects of Group [F(1,10) 5
0.18, P . 0.679] Condition [F(1,10) 5 0.04, P . 0.845]
nor an interaction of Group 3 Condition [F(1,10) 5 2.19,
1040
A. D. Friederici et al.
Fig. 2 Average ERPs for the ELAN component at selected left and right frontal/anterior sites displaying the cortical group/controls and
the basal ganglia group/controls for the syntactic phrase structure violation condition. The waveforms are superimposed for the correct
(continuous line) and the incorrect (dotted line) condition. The vertical lines indicate the onset of the critical word. The first 100 ms postonset are used as a baseline. ATL 5 left anterior temporal electrode; ATR 5 right anterior temporal electrode.
P . 0.169] in the ANOVA analysis of correct responses in
the semantic condition.
ERPs
ERPs are presented as average waveforms at selected
electrode sites for patient groups and controls. In addition,
each effect is presented as individual data for patients and
controls. The grand average waveform of the ERPs for
correct targets in the syntactic condition for the patient and
control groups are plotted in Fig. 2 for the early anterior
negativity and for the P600 in Fig. 3. ERPs for correct targets
in the semantic condition for both patient and control groups
are presented in Fig. 4. The early anterior negativity, P600
and N400 effects were calculated across the electrodes that
defined a respective region of interest and are presented for
each patient and control in Fig. 5 for the cortical group and
Fig. 6 for the basal ganglia group.
Approximately 8% of the trials were rejected due to eye
blinks, horizontal eye movement, or amplifier blocking in
the control groups. Eye artefact control measures were applied
to the raw data of each patient to increase the number of
critical trials in each condition (Pfeifer et al., 1995). The
early N1 and P2 components are not visible due to the fact
that the critical word was presented in a continuous speech
stream rather then in a word-by-word presentation mode.
Visual inspection suggested that patient and control groups
differed with respect to the latency and frontal distribution
of the early anterior negativity (150–300 ms), while the
centroparietal P600 with a mean latency between 300 and
800 ms was comparable between all groups. A negativegoing wave (N400) with a mean latency of 300–600 ms
occurred with a temporal-parietal bilateral distribution in all
groups and was followed by a broadly distributed positivegoing wave (P300). Statistical analyses revealed the following
differences.
Cortical group versus controls
The ANOVA analysis of repeated measures for the ELAN
component showed a Group 3 Condition interaction effect
[F(1,7) 5 8.09, P , 0.024] and a main effect for Group
[F(1,7) 518.26, P , 0.003]. The main effect for Condition
was not significant [F(1,7) 5 1.15, P . 0.318]. There was
no interaction of the factors Group, Condition and Electrode
Site [F(3, 21) 5 0.62, P . 0.607]. Two-way ANOVAs by
Group showed a Condition effect in the controls [F(1,5) 5
8.44, P . 0.033] but not in the patients [F(1,2) 5 13.65,
P . 0.066]. This result confirms the visual observation that
the cortical group does not display an early left anterior
negativity.
An ANOVA for the P600 component revealed a main
effect of Condition [F(1,7) 5 9.58, P . 0.017] but no main
effect of Group [F(1,7) 5 1.89, P . 0.211] no interaction
of Group 3 Condition [F(1,7) 5 0.05, P . 0.711] nor an
Language related brain potentials
1041
Fig. 3 Average ERPs for the P600 component at selected left and right parietal sites (including the central midline site CZ) displaying
the cortical group/controls and the basal ganglia group/controls for the syntactic phrase structure violation condition. The waveforms are
superimposed for the correct (continuous line) and the incorrect (dotted line) condition. The vertical lines indicate the onset of the critical
word. The first 100 ms post-onset are used as a baseline. Note, that the displayed electrode sites for both the ELAN and the P600
components reflect the distribution across the scalp for the syntactic condition.
interaction of Group 3 Condition 3 Electrode Site [F(5,35) 5
0.46, P . 0.667]. These results indicate that both groups
show a P600.
Lastly, an ANOVA for the N400 displayed an interaction
between Group 3 Condition 3 Electrode Site [F(5,35) 5
2.94, P . 0.033] but no main effect of Group [F(1,7) 5 1.72,
P . 0.231]. The factor Condition approached significance
[F(1,7) 5 4.63, P . 0.068]. The interaction was still
significant when the mean amplitude measures were
normalized (cf. McCarthy and Woods, 1985) [F(5,35) 5
2.98, P . 0.026]. The single group analysis resulted in an
interaction of Condition 3 Electrode Site in the controls
[F(5,25) 5 5.87, P . 0.001; normalized F(5,25) 5 5.75,
P , 0.001], but not in the patients [F(5,10) 5 0.23,
P . 0.704]. The results reveal an N400 effect in the controls
that is more accentuated at the right hemisphere electrode
sites than at the left hemisphere electrode sites, while the
analysis of the N400 in the patients did not confirm an N400
effect. However, the factor Condition approached significance
in the omnibus ANOVA indicating that latency jitters in the
patient group could have undermined the N400 effect (see
individual patient data, Fig. 5).
Basal ganglia group versus controls
The repeated-measures ANOVA for the ELAN resulted in
main effects of Group [F(1,10) 5 11.46, P , 0.006] and
Condition [F(1,10) 5 19.69, P . 0.001], but not an interaction
of Group 3 Condition [F(1,10) 5 0.06, P . 0.810] nor an
interaction of Group 3 Condition 3 Electrode Site [F(3,30) 5
0.24, P . 0.716]. Analysis by group revealed a main effect
of Condition in the patient group [F(1,3) 5 10.63, P ,
0.047] and the control group [F(1,7) 5 12.00, P , 0.010].
Statistical analyses confirmed the visual inspection of the
data with an ELAN elicited in both patient and control
groups.
The ANOVA for the P600 in the syntactic violation
condition revealed a main effect of Condition [F(1,10) 5
21.55, P , 0.000] but no main effect of Group [F(1,10) 5
0.04, P . 0.842]. There was no interaction between
Group 3 Condition [F(1,10) 5 0.67, P , 0.431] or a
three-way interaction of Group 3 Condition 3 Electrode Site
[F(5,50) 5 1.11, P . 0.360]. These data support a P600
component in both groups.
Statistical analyses for the semantic violation condition
resulted in a main effect of Condition [F(1,10) 5 9.34,
P , 0.012]. Again there was no main effect of Group
[F(1,10) 5 1.07, P . 0.326] no interaction between
Group 3 Condition [F(1,7) 5 0.00, P . 0.948] nor an
interaction of Group 3 Condition 3 Electrode Site [F(5,50) 5
0.51, P . 0.647]. These data confirm the visual inspection
that both groups show an N400 effect in the semantic
condition.
1042
A. D. Friederici et al.
Fig. 4 Average ERPs for the N400 component at left and right centroparietal selected sites displaying the cortical group/controls and the
basal ganglia group/controls for the semantic violation condition. The waveforms are superimposed for the correct (continuous line) and
the incorrect (dotted line) condition. The vertical lines indicate the onset of the critical word. The first 100 ms post-onset are used as a
baseline. WL 5 left Wernicke eclectrode; WR 5 right Wernicke electrode.
Discussion
The goal of the paper was to investigate semantic and
syntactic information processing during auditory sentence
comprehension in patients with predominantely cortical
versus exclusively subcortical lesions in the left hemisphere.
Seven patients grouped into two groups according to their
lesion sites and 14 age-matched controls participated in an
experiment in which correct sentences as well as semantically
incorrect sentences and syntactically incorrect sentences were
presented as connected speech. ERPs were registered while
subjects were listening to the sentences. After each sentence
subjects were asked to judge the sentence’s correctness.
Normal controls
As expected, normal subjects showed a N400 component in
correlation with the processing of semantically incorrect
sentences. Processing of syntactically incorrect sentences was
correlated with an early anterior negativity and with a late
centroparietal positivity, the P600 component. This result
replicates earlier findings using similar stimulus material in
auditory comprehension tasks (Friederici et al., 1993, 1996;
Hahne and Friederici, 1999). [The distribution of the frontal
negativity for normal controls in this experiment was less
lateralized than reported in the earlier studies. Although also
present at right anterior electrode sites the maximum of the
negativity was at left anterior electrode sites (Kotz et al.,
1998). Experiments investigating the distributional
differences of the early anterior negativity are currently under
way.] It was argued (Friederici, 1995) that these different
components reflect semantic processes (N400) and two stages
of syntactic processes, respectively. The early left anterior
negativity was viewed to reflect an early stage of initial
syntactic structure building and the P600 a second stage
during which semantic and syntactic information are mapped
onto each other and during which in case of a mismatch
syntactic re-analysis or repair mechanisms take place.
The functional specification of the two syntactic ERP
components is still under discussion. With respect to the
P600 the issue has been raised whether or not it reflects a
general context-updating process (e.g. Coulson et al., 1998;
Osterhout, 1997). Independent of this debate the P600 has
been viewed to reflect processes of syntactic re-analysis and
repair (Friederici et al., 1996) or the difficulty of syntactic
integration in general, independent of a sentence’s correctness
or temporary ambiguity (Kaan et al., 1998). The different
views, however, agree in that they assume the P600 to reflect
late syntactic processes.
With respect to the early left anterior negativity it has been
proposed that it might reflect processes of initial structure
building (Friederici, 1995). The variation in the distribution
of the early anterior negativity either showing a clear left
maximum or displaying a more bilateral distribution raises the
question whether the early anterior negativity is a reflection of
Language related brain potentials
1043
Fig. 5 Mean amplitude of the ELAN, P600 and the N400 effect for each individual patient and control
in the cortical group. Controls are listed by increasing age. The means were averaged across the
electrode sites that defined the region of interest in the ANOVA analyses (see ERPs and judgement
task).
Fig. 6 Mean amplitude of the ELAN, P600 and the N400 effect for each individual patient and control
in the basal ganglia group. Controls are listed by increasing age. The means were averaged across the
electrode sites that defined the region of interest in the ANOVA analyses (see ERPs and judgement
task).
only one functional process. Evidence from experiments
investigating the influence of prosodic information on early
syntactic processes using ERPs (Jescheniak et al., 1998;
Steinhauer et al., 1999) indicate an early influence of prosodic
information on intial syntactic parsing and, moreover, recent
functional imaging data (Steinhauer et al., 1999) suggest
that these prosodic processes are supported by the right
hemisphere including the right inferior frontal gyrus. If this
holds true we may speculate that distributional differences
in the early anterior negativity may be dependent on the
extent to which early structuring of the auditory input is
primarily syntactic (leading to a left maximum) or also
includes aspects of prosodic structuring (leading to a more
bilateral distribution). For the time being this interpretation
for the distributional variation observed in normals must
remain a pure hypothesis. However, its formulation allows
clear predictions which can be evaluated experimentally.
Patients
The two patient groups demonstrate different patterns of
breakdown for the three different language components. We
will discuss these in turn.
Cortical group
For the semantic condition, these patients showed an
attenuated negativity in the time window defined for the
N400. Looking at the individual subject data it is apparent
that the lack of statistical significance in the N400 group
analysis was due to latency jitters in the patient group. This
was further supported by the fact that the factor Condition
in the omnibus ANOVA approached significance. When
evaluating each single patient a N400-like pattern was
observed for each patient, although in different time intervals.
1044
A. D. Friederici et al.
This result is compatible with the finding of Hagoort and
colleagues (Hagoort et al., 1996) who found that aphasics
with minor comprehension deficits showed a N400 effect
similar in size to that of the control subjects.
The early anterior negativity in the syntactic condition was
absent in our cortical patients, but a P600 was found. The
absence of the early anterior negativity and the presence of
the P600 component was observed in each patient. [It should
be noted that the absence of the early anterior negativity in
this patient group and the presence of the early anterior
negativity in the basal ganglia group can not be attributed to
the size of the individual lesions in these two groups.] These
results replicate an earlier case report of a Broca patient who
did not display an early anterior negativity, though did display
a P600 and an N400-like component (Friederici et al., 1998).
The combined data suggest that patients with lesions in the
anterior language cortex suffer from a selective impairment
of the fast initial syntactic processes. In turn, we may
conclude that this brain region is part of the neuronal network
involved in fast syntactic procedures during normal language
processing. The finding that these patients showed a P600 in
the absence of a fast initial parse deserves some further
discussion. How can they demonstrate a repair process
(reflected by the P600), when they are not able to perform
the first-pass parse reflected by the ELAN? The judgement
data indicate that these patients are able to judge a sentence’s
grammatical correctness. The ERP data suggest that off-line
grammaticality judgement does not necessarily rely on an
automatic first-pass parse. Secondary processes reflected in
P600 may suffice to perform such a judgement. Tentative
support for this interpretation comes from a new functional
specification of the P600 as a mechanism of integration that
is not purely structural in nature but may involve thematic
information (Kaan et al., 1998) and from earlier behavioural
data with aphasic patients (Linebarger et al., 1983).
Basal ganglia group
Patients with lesions restricted to focal ischaemic/
haemorrhagic lesions of the left basal ganglia display an
interesting pattern with respect to the three ERP components.
Whether the basal ganglia are involved in language processing
has been discussed quite extensively (Damasio and Damasio,
1992; Lieberman et al., 1992; Grossman et al., 1993; Ullman
et al., 1997). The present patient group appears not to differ
from controls as no interaction between the factors Group
and Condition were observed for any of the three ERP
components. This was expected for the N400 component as
processes underlying this component are thought to be
supported by temporal brain structures.
Since there were different predictions of a possible
involvement of the basal ganglia during syntactic processing,
we conducted separate analyses for these patients and their
controls for the syntactic condition. These analyses revealed
the presence of the early left anterior negativity in the patients
with basal ganglia involvement. The P600 component was
also present, although somewhat reduced in the patients
compared with normal controls. This finding suggests that at
least the portions of the basal ganglia (i.e. caudate nucleus,
putamen, globus pallidus) lesioned in our patient sample are
not a primary part of the network supporting those language
procedures that support early syntactic structure building, i.e.
first-pass parsing processes. We may also conjecture that the
accompanying subcortical lesions in two out of three patients
of the cortical group do not account for the absence of the
early anterior negativity.
This finding has to be discussed with respect to the results
of Ullman and colleagues (Ullman et al., 1997). They reported
that the procedural aspect of grammatical processing is
correlated with relative damage to the left frontal/basal
ganglia system. Our data indicate a differential involvement
of the left subcortical and cortical regions in procedural
aspects of grammatical processing. [It should be noted that
Ullman and colleagues (Ullman et al., 1997) studied patients
diagnosed with Parkinson’s and Huntington’s disease. In
contrast to our present patient group with focal ischaemic/
haemorrhagic vascular lesions, these patients have extended
neuronal degeneration in the basal ganglia and presumably
also in some cortical areas.] Ullman and colleagues (Ullman
et al., 1997) had tested procedural aspects of grammatical
processing using correctly and incorrectly inflected regular
and irregular verbs. The frontal/basal ganglia patients
demonstrated particular problems with regular compared with
irregular verbs. As the former are thought to be generated
by grammatical rules the authors concluded that the frontal/
basal ganglia structure represent the procedural grammatical
knowledge. Although the material and the task used in the
study by Ullman and colleagues (Ullman et al., 1997) was
different from the present study, a link can be made from
their study to the present results via two recent ERP studies
using stimulus material quite similar to the study by Ullman
and colleagues (Ullman et al., 1997). Penke et al. (1997)
reported results from a German language perception study
in which correctly and incorrectly inflected regular and
irregular verbs were presented in lists and in sentences.
Incorrectly inflected irregular verbforms were associated with
a negative waveform with a left anterior and temporal
distribution starting around 200 ms extending to 500 ms.
Similar results were also obtained in an other ERP study
investigating the processing of regular and irregular verbs in
English (Newman et al., 1998). Penke et al. (1997) take
their findings to suggest a functional similarity between the
application of rules of affixation (subject–verb agreement)
and the application of phrase structure rules (structure
building). This is an interesting proposal. The difference in
the latency and the distribution of the left anterior negativity
for the processing of phrase structure violations and the one
for affixation raises the question whether both negativities
indeed reflect the same function. If they do, then it would
be difficult to explain the difference between the low
performance on regular verbs in patients with lesions in the
basal ganglia in the study by Ullman and colleagues (Ullman
Language related brain potentials
et al., 1997) and the ERP pattern seen for the basal ganglia
group in the present study. Note, however, that the view
would be compatible for the results reported for their patients
with left frontal cortical lesions.
Under the assumption that the early left anterior negativity
reflects automatic first-pass parsing processes the findings
from the present ERP study suggest that these automatic
parsing procedures are supported by the left frontolateral
cortex whereas the basal ganglia are not necessarily involved
in these automatic structure building processes (see also
Friederici et al., 1998). ERP research with patients suffering
from temporal lesions, in contrast, suggests that lexical–
semantic integration and knowledge guided second-pass
parsing primarily involve the left temporal language cortex
(Revonsuo and Laine, 1996; Friederici et al., 1998).
Interestingly, the latter two processes reflected by the N400
and the P600 can be distinguished at a functional level from
first-pass parsing processes as they behave differently with
respect to attentional aspects. Lexical–semantic processes
reflected by the N400 (Chwilla et al., 1995) as well as
grammatical processes reflected by the P600 (Vos et al.,
1995; Coulson et al., 1998; Gunter and Friederici, 1999)
involve aspects of attentional control, while first-pass parsing
procedures do not (Hahne and Friederici, 1999). This
functional difference is compatible with a general distinction
between procedural knowledge available in automatic
procedure and declarative knowledge being accessed in a
more controlled manner. Results from studies with patients
suffering from circumscribed brain lesions are compatible
with a view that the distinction between procedural versus
declarative knowledge may be mapped neuroanatomically
onto left frontal versus left temporal language areas. The
present ERP data even suggest a more fine-grained
dissociation within the anterior part of the left hemisphere,
namely between cortical areas and the basal ganglia.
1045
Buckner RL, Petersen SE, Ojemann JG, Miezin FM, Squire LR,
Raichle ME. Functional anatomical studies of explicit and implicit
memory retrieval tasks. J Neurosci 1995; 15: 12–29.
Caramazza A, Zurif EB. Dissociation of algorithmic and heuristic
processes in language comprehension: evidence from aphasia. Brain
Lang 1976; 3: 572–82.
Chomsky N. The minimalist program. Cambridge (MA): MIT
Press; 1995.
Chwilla DJ, Brown CM, Hagoort P. The N400 as a function of the
level of processing. Psychophysiology 1996; 32: 274–85.
Coulson S, King JW, Kutas M. Expect the unexpected: event-related
brain responses to morphosyntactic violations. Lang Cogn Proc
1998; 13: 21–58.
Damasio AR, Damasio H. Brain and language. Sci Am 1992; 267:
88–95.
Damasio AR, Damasio H, Rizzo M, Varney N, Gersh F. Aphasia
with nonhemorrhagic lesions in the basal ganglia and internal
capsule. Arch Neurol 1982; 39: 15–20.
Dronkers NF, Shapiro JK, Redfern B, Knight RT. The role of
Broca’s area in Broca’s aphasia [abstract]. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol
1992; 14: 52–3.
Frazier L. Syntactic processing: evidence from Dutch. Nat Lang
Ling Theory 1987; 5: 519–60.
Friederici AD. Levels of processing and vocabulary types: evidence
from on-line comprehension in normals and agrammatics. Cognition
1985; 19: 133–66.
Friederici AD. The time course of syntactic activation during
language processing: A model based on neuropsychological and
neurophysiological data. [Review]. Brain Lang 1995; 50: 259–81.
Friederici AD, Kilborn K. Temporal constraints on language
processing: syntactic priming in Broca’s aphasia. J Cogn Neurosci
1989; 1: 262–72.
Acknowledgement
Friederici AD, Pfeifer E, Hahne A. Event-related brain potentials
during natural speech processing: effects of semantic, morphological
and syntactic violations. Cogn Brain Res 1993; 1: 183–92.
The authors wish to thank Anja Hahne for providing the
stimulus material first used in Hahne and Friederici (1999)
for the present study.
Friederici AD, Hahne A, Mecklinger A. The temporal structure of
syntactic parsing: early and late ERP effects elicited by syntactic
anomalies. J Exp Psych: Learn, Mem Cogn 1996; 22: 1219–48.
References
Alexander MP, Naeser MA, Palumbo CL. Correlations of subcortical
CT lesion sites and aphasia profiles. Brain 1987; 110: 961–91.
Berndt RS, Haendiges AN, Mitchum CC, Sandson J. Verb retrieval
in aphasia. 2. Relationship to sentence processing. Brain Lang 1997;
56: 107–37.
Blumstein SE, Milberg W, Shrier R. Semantic processing in aphasia:
Evidence from an auditory lexical decision task. Brain Lang 1982;
17: 301–15.
´
´
Broca P. Sur la faculte du language articule. Bull Soc Anthrop
Paris–1865; 6: 493–94.
Friederici AD, Hahne A, von Cramon DY. First-pass versus secondpass parsing processes in a Wernicke’s and a Broca’s aphasic:
Electro-physiological evidence for a double dissociation. Brain Lang
1999; 62: 311–41.
Geschwind N. Disconnexion syndromes in animals and man. I.
[Review]. Brain 1965; 88: 237–94.
Goodglass H. Agrammatism. In: Whitaker HA, editor. Studies in
Neurolinguistics. New York: Academic Press; 1976. p. 237–60.
Goodglass H. Understanding aphasia. San Diego: Academic Press;
1993.
¨
Groβ J, Ioannides AA, Dammers J, Maeβ B, Friederici AD, Muller¨
Gartner H-W. Magnetic field tomography analysis of continuous
speech. Brain Tom 1998; 10: 273–81.
1046
A. D. Friederici et al.
Grossman M, Carvell S, Gollomp S, Stern MB, Reivich M,
Morrison D, Alavi A, et al. Cognitive and physiological substrates
of impaired sentence processing in Parkinson’s disease. J Cogn
Neurosci 1993; 5: 480–98.
Kutas M, Van Petten C. Event-related potential studies of language.
In: Ackles PK, Jennings JR, Coles MGH, editors. Advances in
psychophysiology, Vol. 3. Greenwich (CT): JAI Press; 1988.
p. 139–87.
Gunter ThC, Stowe JL, Mulder G. When syntax meets semantics.
Psychophysiology 1997; 34: 660–676.
Lieberman P, Kako E, Friedman J, Tajchman G, Feldman LS, Jiminez
EB. Speech production, syntax comprehension, and cognitive deficits
in Parkinson’s disease. Brain Lang 1992; 43: 169–89.
Gunter ThC, Friederici AD. Concerning the automaticity of syntactic
processing. Psychophysiology 1999; 36: 1–12.
Haarmann HJ, Kolk HHJ. Syntactic priming in Broca’s aphasia:
evidence for slow activation. Aphasiol 1991; 5: 247–63.
Hagoort P, Brown C, Groothusen J. The syntactic positive shift
(SPS) as an ERP measure of syntactic processing. Lang Cogn Proc
1993; 8: 439–83.
Hagoort P, Brown CM, Swaab TY. Lexical–semantic event-related
potential effects in patients with left hemisphere lesions and aphasia,
and patients with right hemisphere lesions without aphasia. Brain
1996; 119: 627–49.
Hahne A, Friederici AD. Functional neurotopography of syntactic
parsing: Early automatic and late controlled processes. J Cogn
Neurosci 1999; 11: 193–204.
Heilman K, Scholes RJ. The nature of comprehension errors in
Broca’s conduction and Wernicke’s aphasics. Cortex 1976; 12:
258–65.
Huynh H, Feldt LA. Conditions under which mean square ratios in
repeated measurement designs have exact F-distributions. J Am Stat
Assoc 1970; 65: 1582–9.
Jescheniak JD, Hahne A, Friederici AD. Brain activity patterns
suggest prosodic influences on syntactic parsing in the
comprehension of spoken sentences. Music Perception 1998; 16:
55–62.
Linebarger MC, Schwartz MF, Saffran EM. Sensitivity to
grammatical structure in so-called agrammatic aphasics. Cognition
1983; 13: 361–92.
McCarthy G, Wood CC. Scalp distributions of event-related
potentials: an ambiguity associated with analysis of variance models.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1985; 62: 203–8.
McCarthy G, Nobre AC, Bentin S, Spencer DD. Language-related
field potentials in the anterior-medial temporal lobe: I. Intracranial
distribution and neural generators. J Neurosci 1995; 15: 1080–89.
Milberg W, Blumstein SE. Lexical decision and aphasia: evidence
for semantic processing. Brain Lang 1981; 14: 371–85.
Mohr JP. Broca’s area and Broca’s aphasia. In: Whitaker H, Whitaker
HA, editors. Studies in neurolinguistics, Vol. 1. New York: Academic
Press; 1976. p. 201–35.
¨
Munte TF, Heinze H-J, Matzke M, Wieringa BM, Johannes S.
Brain potentials and syntactic violations revisited: No evidence for
specificity of the syntactic positive shift. Neuropsychol 1998; 36:
217–26.
Nadeau SE, Crosson B. Subcortical aphasia [see comments].
[Review]. Brain Lang 1997; 58: 355–402. Comment in: Brain Lang
1997; 58: 403–9.
Naeser MA, Alexander MP, Helm-Estabrooks N, Levine HL,
Laughlin SA, Geschwind N. Aphasia with predominantly subcortical
lesion sites. Arch Neurol 1982; 39: 2–14.
Kaan E, Harris A, Gibson E, Holcomb PJ. The P600 as an index
of syntactic integration difficulty. In: AMLaP-98 Architecture and
Mechanisms for Language Processing; 1998; Freiburg.
Neville H, Nicol JL, Barss A, Forster KI, Garrett MF. Syntactically
based sentence processing classes: evidence from event-related brain
potentials. J Cogn Neurosci 1991; 3: 151–65.
Kapur S, Rose R, Liddle PF, Zipursky RB, Brown GM, Stuss D,
et al. The role of the left prefrontal cortex in verbal processing:
semantic processing or willed action? Neuroreport 1994; 5: 2193–96.
Newman AJ, Neville HJ, Ullman MT. Neural processing of
inflectional morphology: an event-related potential study of English
past tense [abstract]. J Cogn Neurosci 1998; Suppl: 27.
Kempen G. Sentence parsing. In: Friederici AD, editor. Language
comprehension: a biological perspective. Berlin: Springer-Verlag;
1998. p. 213–28.
Nobre AC, McCarthy G. Language-related field potentials in the
anterior-medial temporal lobe: II. Effects of word type and semantic
priming. J Neurosci 1995; 15: 1090–8.
Knight RT. Distributed cortical network for visual attention. J Cogn
Neurosci 1997; 9: 75–91.
Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the
Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychol 1971; 9: 97–113.
Knight RT, Scabini D, Woods DL, Clayworth CC. Contributions of
temporal-parietal junction to the human auditory P3. Brain Res
1989; 502: 109–16.
Osterhout L. On the brain response to syntactic anomalies.
Manipulations of word position and word class reveal individual
differences. Brain Lang 1997; 59: 494–522.
Kolk HHJ, Van Grunsven MJF. Agrammatism as a variable
phenomenon. Cogn Neuropsychol 1985; 2: 347–84.
Osterhout L, Holcomb PJ. Event-related brain potentials elicited by
syntactic anomaly. J Mem Lang 1992; 31: 785–806.
Kotz SA, von Cramon DY, Friederici AD. A time-course analysis
of auditory and visual comprehension in an aphasic patient: ERP
evidence. Brain Lang 1998; 65: 44–6.
Osterhout L, Mobley LA. Event-related brain potentials elicited by
failure to agree. J Mem Lang 1995; 34: 739–73.
¨
Penke M, Weyerts H, Gross M, Zander E, Munte TF, Clahsen A.
How the brain processes complex words: an event-related potentialstudy of German verb inflections. Cogn Brain Res 1997; 6: 37–52.
Kutas M, Hillyard SA. Event-related brain potentials to grammatical
errors and semantic anomalies. Mem Cogn 1983; 11: 539–50.
Language related brain potentials
1047
Petersen SE, Fox PT, Posner MI, Mintum M, Raichle ME. Positron
emission tomographic studies of the processing of single words. J
Cogn Neurosci 1989; 1: 153–70.
Stromswold K, Caplan D, Alpert N, Rauch S. Localization of
syntactic comprehension by positron emission tomography. Brain
Lang 1996; 52: 452–73.
Pfeifer E, Novagk R, Maeβ B. Software for EEG/ERP evaluation. In:
Friederici A, editor, Max Planck Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience
Leipzig. Annual Report; 1995. p. 51–5.
Swaab T, Brown C, Hagoort P. Spoken sentence comprehension in
aphasia: event-related potential evidence for a lexical integration
deficit. J Cogn Neurosci 1997; 9: 39–66.
Revonsuo A, Laine M. Semantic processing without conscious
understanding in a global aphasic: evidence from auditory eventrelated brain potentials. Cortex 1996; 32: 29–48.
Ullman MT, Corkin S, Coppola M, Hickok G, Growdon JH,
Koroshetz WJ, et al. A neural dissociation within language: evidence
that the mental dictionary is part of declarative memory, and that
grammatical rules are processed by the procedural system. J Cogn
Neurosci 1997; 9: 266–76.
Robin DA, Schienberg S. Subcortical lesions and aphasia [see
comments]. [Review]. J Speech Hear Disord 1990; 55: 90–100.
Comment in: J Speech Hear Disord 1990; 55: 799–802.
¨
¨
Rosler F, Putz P, Friederici A, Hahne A. Event-related brain
potentials while encountering semantic and syntactic constraint
violations. J Cogn Neurosci 1993; 5: 345–62.
Varney NR, Damasio H. CT scan correlates of sound recognition
defect in aphasia. Cortex 1986; 22: 483–6.
Vos SH, Gunter ThC, Mulder G, Kolk HHJ. Working memory and
syntactic processing: an ERP research. Eighth Annual CUNY
Conference on Human Sentence Processing. Tucson (AZ), USA;
1995.
Sharbrough F. American electroencephalographic Society guidelines
for standard electrode position nomenclature. J Clin Neurophysl
1991; 8: 200–2.
Wernicke C. Der aphasische Symptomenkomplex. Breslau: Cohn
and Weigert; 1874.
Smith ME, Stapleton JM, Halgren E. Human medial temporal lobe
potentials evoked in memory and language tasks. Electroencephalogr
Clin Neurophysiol 1986; 63: 145–59.
Zatorre RJ, Meyer E, Gjedde A, Evans AC. PET studies of phonetic
processing of speech: review, replication, and reanalysis. Cereb
Cortex 1996; 6: 21–30.
Steinhauer K, Alter K, Friederici AD. Brain potentials indicates
immediate use of prosodic cues in natural speech processing. Nat
Neurosci 1999; 2: 191–6.
Zurif E, Caramazza A, Myerson R, Galvin J. Semantic feature
representation for normal and aphasic language. Brain Lang 1974;
1: 167–87.
Steinhauer K, Alter K, Meyer M, Friederici AD, von Cramon DY.
Brain activation related to prosodic processing in natural speech:
an event-related fMRI study [abstract]. J Cog Neurosci 1999; Suppl.
Received May 20, 1998. Revised August 24, 1998.
Second revision December 1, 1998. Accepted January 27, 1999