Download HANDEDNESS AND LANGUAGE LATERALIZATION: WHY ARE WE

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts
no text concepts found
Transcript
Hellenic Journal of Psychology, Vol. 8 (2011), pp. 248-265
HANDEDNESS AND LANGUAGE LATERALIZATION:
WHY ARE WE RIGHT-HANDED AND
LEFT-BRAINED?
Marietta Papadatou-Pastou
National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Greece
Abstract: Around 90% of humans prefer their right hand for unimanual actions and are lefthemisphere dominant for language functions; a pattern far from negligible. The phenomena of
handedness and cerebral lateralization for language are presented along with the different
theories that attempt to explain the presence of these functional asymmetries. The focus is on
the adaptive advantages both on the individual and the population level. Most importantly, the
intriguing question of why humans are right-handed and left-brained and not the other way
around is tackled; a number of evolutionary, cultural, and genetic accounts are presented,
along with theories that explain the observed pattern of asymmetries by means of the different
properties of the two cerebral hemispheres.
Key words: Handedness, Language, Laterality.
INTRODUCTION
Morphological right-left asymmetry appears to be the rule, rather than the exception in nature, all the way from chiral molecules (Quack, 1989) to the Baryon asymmetry in the universe (Sakharov, 1991). Asymmetry is the rule for biological systems
as well (Geschwind & Galaburda, 1987; Kimura, 1973), whereby even single-celled
organisms are commonly asymmetric (Nelson, 2003). Human beings are certainly
structurally and functionally asymmetric, from the size of their feet and hands to the
placement of visceral organs and facial features (Levy & Levy, 1978; Purves, White,
& Andrews, 1994). In fact, the two aspects of behavior specific to humans, the use
Address: Marietta Papadatou-Pastou, Research Centre for Psychophysiology and Education,
National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, 27 Deinokratous Str., 106 75 Kolonaki,
Athens, Greece. E-mail: [email protected]
Handedness and language lateralization
249
of language (Lieberman, 1984) and the strong population-level preference for hand
use (Annett, 1985; McManus, 1991), are both asymmetric, lateralized traits. When
it comes to language, the majority of humans are left-hemisphere dominant for language functions, while the right hand is the preferred hand for over 9 out of 10 individuals.
One may wonder why these functional asymmetries have emerged. Most importantly, why would this specific pattern be the case? Why are humans right-handed
and left-brained and not the other way around? The present review sets out to
answer these questions, after defining and discussing the phenomena of handedness
and language lateralization. The mere volume of published work in the field of laterality (e.g., under the heading of handedness 2,195 articles were cited in PsycINFO
only between 1989 and 2006; see Papadatou-Pastou, Martin, Munafó, & Jones, 2008.
Between 2007 and September 2011, 746 new papers were cited under the same heading in Psych INFO) makes such an orderly synopsis imperative. This endeavour, apart
from its intrinsic interest, gains importance from the fact that the study of handedness
and language lateralization contributes to the broader question of individual differences in brain organisation and abilities. Such differences are of key importance to
psychiatric, neurological, and neuropsychological research and practice.
WHAT IS HANDEDNESS?
Handedness is the best-known and most studied human asymmetry, and it can be
defined as “the individual’s preference to use one hand predominately for unimanual tasks and/or the ability to perform these tasks more efficiently with one hand”
(Corey, Hurley, & Foundas, 2001). Left-handedness incidence is a point of dispute
among different studies; percentages range from 1.6% (Hoosain, 1990) to 32.2%
(Gladue & Bailey, 1995), and are usually reported as “around 10%” (Cavill &
Bryden, 2003; Holtzen, 1994). An unpublished, large-scale systematic review including 1.8 million participants found that the incidence of left-handedness lies between
7.52% (using the most stringent criterion of extreme left-handedness) and 17.42%
(using the most lenient criterion of non-right handedness; Papadatou-Pastou,
Martin, Munafó, & Jones, 2010).
Handedness is a uniquely human characteristic; humans appear to be almost
alone in exhibiting a strong population-level preference for the use of one limb – the
right hand – rather than its mirror limb. While individual members of other species
may exhibit preferences for the use of a right or a left limb, there is little or no evidence for a species-wide preference (Annett, 1985; Corballis, 1991; Martin & Jones,
250
M. Papadatou-Pastou
1999). Considering studies in which hand or paw preference has been measured
directly, there is no evidence for population-level handedness in mice (Collins,
1985), rats (Kirk, 1935; Uguru-Okorie & Arbuthnott, 1981), cats (Burgess &
Villablanca, 1986), and potentially apes (Byrne & Byrne, 1991), although the latter
case is controversial (MacNeilage, Studdert-Kennedy, & Lindblom, 1987; Marchant
& McGrew, 1991). In non-mammalian species, the parrot may be an unusual exception to the above rule (Harris, 1989).
A wealth of evidence is available supporting the notion that handedness is an
early developmental characteristic, both phylogenetically (i.e., in the evolution of
the human species) and ontogenetically (i.e., in the development of the individual).
As far as phylogenesis is concerned, comparative observations suggest that the
event that shaped the evolution of human handedness must have taken place after
the split between humans and chimpanzees (Corballis, 1991). The oldest published
evidence of human handedness is from the Pleistocene period (Bahn, 1989;
deCastro, Bromage, & Jalvo, 1988; Lalueza & Frayer, 1997; Lewin, 1986; Toth,
1985), when incisive marking indicates the existence of right- and left-handed homo
neanderthalensis for sharp tool manipulation. In the homo sapiens taxonomy, the
oldest evidence is from the upper Palaeolithic period, when right and left tube holders for blow painting were both present, with a predominance of right tube holders,
as indicated by the record of negative hand painting in caves (Groenen, 1988; 1997).
Other artefacts such as bone and antler implements from the Neolithic period,
about 7000 years ago, also show evidence of a predominance of right-handedness
(Spenneman, 1984). The assessment of manual preference from 12,000 works of art
from European, Asian, African, and American sources (Coren & Porac, 1977) and
rich evidence from anthropological research (Black, Young, Pei, & de Chardin,
1933; Brinton, 1986; Dart, 1949; Mason, 1896) have further shown that the approximated magnitude of this preference does not seem to have undergone any systematic change over the past 50 centuries.
Ontogenetically, handedness also appears quite early in development. In most
human embryos, the right hand is more developed than the left at seven weeks postconception (O’Rahilly & Muller, 1987). Using ultrasonography, Hepper,
Shahidullah, and White (1991) observed that 92% of the foetuses who sucked their
thumbs tended to choose the right thumb. They further reported that 10-week-old
foetuses moved their right arm more often that their left, with 75% of foetuses
showing a right arm bias. Goodwin and Michel (1981) studied hand preference
among neonates at 19 weeks of age and found that 64% preferred the right hand in
a reaching task. Gesell and Ames (1947) found that the tonic neck reflex observed
in newborns strongly predicted handedness at the ages of one, five, and ten years.
Handedness and language lateralization
251
Other researchers have observed sidedness in hand use (or other motor behaviors
associated with handedness) among neonates and infants (Bates, O’Connell, Vaid,
Sledge, & Oakes, 1986; Cioni & Pellegrinetti, 1982) and have noted significant stability over time (Archer, Campbell, & Segalowitz, 1988).
WHAT IS LANGUAGE LATERALIZATION?
A brain is considered to be asymmetrical or lateralized if one hemisphere or other
brain region is structurally different from the other and/or performs a different set of
functions (Bisazza, Rogers, & Vallortigara, 1998). The human brain is typically lateralized with the left hemisphere being the locus of language skills and of analytical processing of stimuli, whereas the right hemisphere is dominant for spatial-constructional
skills, for a more global way of processing information, and it is moreover the locus of
emotions (McManus & Bryden, 1993). Traditionally, hemispheric asymmetries were
considered dichotomous. Today, the generally accepted view is that the two hemispheres show complementary specialization (Bradshaw & Nettleton, 1983).
This critical insight concerning functional lateralization, with language functions
being generally located in the left hemisphere was first clearly enunciated by Paul
Broca when he said “Nous parlons avec l’hemisphère gauche” (“We speak with the
left hemisphere; Broca, 1965)”. Broca reached this conclusion when he examined
over 25 patients, all of whom suffered from a type of aphasia causing an impairment
in language production, and all of whom had suffered lesions to the left side of the
brain, in the most anterior part of the frontal lobe (Berker, Berker, & Smith, 1986),
in the area now known as Broca’s area. In 1874, Carl Wernicke further discovered
that damage to a region of the left hemisphere posterior to Broca’s area, now known
as Wernicke’s area, could cause another type of aphasia that resulted in a language
comprehension impairment (Wernicke, 1984).
Due to all of Broca’s patients being right-handed and having their language
areas lateralized to the left hemisphere, it was initially speculated that the reverse,
that is right hemisphere language dominance, should be true for left-handers. This
claim has been widely accepted as the “Broca rule”, although Broca never explicitly postulated such a rule (Harris, 1983). Such explanations would have restored a
higher-order symmetry of the brain, in which left-handers simply showed the
reverse pattern of language dominance to right-handers (McManus & Bryden,
1993). Luria (1976) was amongst the first to point out that such an association could
not be universally true because even in left-handers aphasia usually occurs after a
lesion in the left hemisphere.
252
M. Papadatou-Pastou
The relationship between handedness and language lateralization is now known
to be much more complex. Knecht et al. (2000), who measured lateralization directly by functional transcranial Doppler ultrasonography (fTCD) in 326 healthy individuals using the Word Generation task, showed that language dominance depends
not only on the direction, but also on the degree of handedness. More specifically,
they showed that the incidence of right-hemispheric dominance increases linearly
with the degree of left-handedness, as measured by the Edinburgh Handedness
Inventory (Oldfield, 1971), from 4% in strong right-handers, to 15% in ambidextrous individuals, and 27% in strong left-handers.
Language lateralization patterns are often described as typical (left-hemispheric)
or atypical (symmetrical and right-hemispheric), but categorisation schemes of this
kind are probably an oversimplification (Szaflarski et al., 2002). Although languagerelated activation in normal right-handed individuals is predominantly left-hemispheric, almost all individuals activate right hemisphere areas to some extent in language studies with large participant samples using functional magnetic resonance
imaging (fMRI), positron emission tomography (PET), and functional transcranial
Doppler ultrasonography (fTCD) (Buckner et al., 1995; Frost et al., 1999; Knecht et
al., 2000; Pujol et al., 1999; Springer et al., 1999; Tzourio et al., 1998). In other words,
there is a continuum of language lateralization patterns ranging from strongly leftdominant to strongly right-dominant. Thus, language is better described as being
“actuated by a distributed cerebral network with differences in regional involvement
related to specific language subfunctions, with essential regions within this network
lateralized to one hemisphere, typically the left” (Frith et al., 1991).
Left Sylvian fissure asymmetry is the best known anatomical asymmetry with
regards to language lateralization (for a review see Jäncke & Steinmetz, 1993).
Studies mainly focus on the planum temporale (PT), a triangular structure on the
supratemporal plane in the depth of the Sylvian fissure, although findings remain
inconclusive. A number of studies have shown that individuals with left-hemispheric
language representation show a strong leftward asymmetry in the PT, while individuals with right-hemispheric language representation do not exhibit a consistent PT
asymmetry (Moffat, Hampson, & Lee, 1998; Ratcliff, Dila, Taylor, & Milner, 1980).
Foundas, Leonard, Gilmore, Fennell, and Heilman (1994) reported that asymmetry
of the PT correlated to cerebral dominance as assessed with the Wada test in 11 individuals. Tzourio et al. (1998), however, found no correlation between the same measures in 14 individuals. In a much larger sample, Jäncke and Steinmetz (1993) replicated the finding of no significant relationship between dichotic listening scores and
PT asymmetry. Similarly, Hellige, Taylor, Lesmes, and Peterson (1998) were not successful in demonstrating this link. Güntürkün and Hausmann (2003) suggested that
Handedness and language lateralization
253
it might not be the asymmetry of the PT as such, but the absolute size of the left PT
that determines language lateralization, as a number of studies, which could not
reveal meaningful relationships between language lateralization measures from
imaging data and PT asymmetry, showed stronger relations to absolute left PT size
(for a review see Habib & Robichon, 2003).
While language, like handedness, is a striking human characteristic, the basic
aspects of brain lateralization are common to both birds and mammals (Bradshaw
& Nettleton, 1983; Denenberg, 1981). From a phylogenetic point of view this indicates that lateralization emerged early in vertebrate evolution (Vallortigara,
Rogers, & Bisazza, 1999). Language-related functional asymmetry also seems to
emerge early in ontogenesis. For example, asymmetry in auditory perception exists
at or shortly after birth (see Hahn, 1987). Moreover, the left Sylvian fissure asymmetry probably originates during the first trimester of pregnancy (LeMay, 1976).
WHY DO HUMANS EXHIBIT ASYMMETRY OF FUNCTION?
A number of adaptive advantages certainly accompany functional asymmetry. With
regards to handedness, Bishop (1990) has put forward two related hypotheses: the
motor learning hypothesis and the inference hypothesis. According to the motor
learning hypothesis, specializing one hand for unimanual actions can result in
advanced performance through practice. In fact, this would also be the case for
actions that require bilaterality of movement, where the two hands have complementary roles; instead of both hands learning to execute either role, each hand
would specialize in one role in a display of division of labour (Corballis & Beale,
1976). The inference hypothesis builds on the fact that learning a motor movement
with one hand facilitates performance of mirror-image movements with the other
hand. Therefore, Bishop (1990) claims that for non-mirror reversible activities, such
as handwriting, whereby an attempt to switch between hands could interfere with
what is learned by each hand, restricting learning to one hand is critical.
Lateralization of manual praxis may further convey increased neural capacity,
because specializing one hemisphere for a particular function leaves the other
hemisphere free to perform other additional functions (Levy, 1977). This would
enable brain evolution to avoid useless duplication of functions in the two hemispheres, saving in neural circuitry.
Similarly to handedness, language lateralization may have come about because
the segregation of functions of the separate halves of the brain allows for simultaneous parallel processing (Vallortigara & Rogers, 2005). Thus, it represents a solu-
254
M. Papadatou-Pastou
tion to the competition for space within the brain and to the problem of functional
incompatibility (Vallortigara et al., 1999). Processing and storing information about
invariance and variance among experiences are mutually incompatible processes,
which might best be handled by functionally separate systems (right and left hemispheres accordingly; ibid.) Moreover, functional neuronal clustering in one hemisphere during language development allows faster linguistic processing because
transition times are shorter than in interhemispheric operations (Lieberman, 1984).
Language lateralization may thus have an adaptive value and it has even been
claimed to present a prerequisite for the full realisation of the linguistic potential
(Geschwind & Galaburda, 1985; Hiscock, 1998).
This rationale, useful as it is in explaining lateralization at the individual level,
fails to explain the presence of either handedness or brain laterality at the population level. In other words, even though it is clear that asymmetry bears advantages
for the individual, it does not address the question of why there are not equal numbers of right- and left-handed humans (and accordingly right- and left-hemisphere
dominant humans). Further, it fails to explain why the preference for one of the
two sides is not species-universal, as is the case with the position of visceral organs
(e.g., the heart is placed to the left of the body). The latter is better explained by
genetic models, which typically assume that the genetic variation is preserved
through heterozygote advantage (Annett, 1985; Corballis, 1991; McManus &
Bryden, 1992). Annett’s (1985) theory, for example, postulates a gene for left cerebral dominance that increases the probability of right-handedness (the rs gene)
and suggests that the heterozygote advantage is such that those with the rs+- genotype have optimal brain organization. A situation of balanced polymorphism is
obtained, as reproductive success of rs+- is higher than both rs- and rs++, thereby maintaining the rs- allele in the gene pool. (A detailed account of the different
genetic theories on laterality is beyond the scope of the present paper, but can be
found in Papadatou-Pastou et al., 2008.)
WHY ARE HUMANS RIGHT-HANDED AND LEFT-BRAINED?
Solving the riddle of both individual- and population-level preference, still does not
explain right hand and left hemisphere preference, as opposed to a possible left
hand and right hemisphere preference, or a combination thereof. (Even Annett’s
genetic theory, which postulates that the right-handedness and left-brainedness
gene is the dominant one, does so in an attempt to explain the mechanisms behind
humans displaying this observed asymmetry.) Let us begin by asking why the right
Handedness and language lateralization
255
hand is preferred over the left one. Evolutionary theories claim that right hand preference may have evolved from warriors who were carrying their shields with their
left hand, leaving the right hand free for fighting, and who consequently had better
survival rates since their hearts were protected (Van Biervleit, 1899). Alternatively,
such theories put forward the tendency of human (and presumably pre-human)
mothers to hold infants on the left side of their body (Huheey, 1975). This latter
practice is ascribed to imprinting and the soothing sound of the mother’s heartbeat
on the infant. Given the practice of holding the child in this manner, dextral mothers are more skillful in the manipulation of objects and thus selectively favoured.
These two theories, interesting as they might be, do not seem plausible. As Bishop
(1990) argues, the position of the heart is only slightly biased to the left side of the
body. Thus, it is unlikely that a right-handed grip on the sword could significantly
improve one’s chance of surviving a battle (and indeed there is no evidence that the
prevalence of right-handedness has increased since the introduction of the sword)
or that it could significantly help mothers soothe their babies.
It could further be the case that right hand preference is merely a learned behavior or a “cultural law” in the early words of Blau (1946). One cannot deny that we
live in a right-handed world, all the way from right-handed scissors and door handles to table manners. In other words, social organization forces us to use the right
hand in order to share tools and equipment specialized for one hand and to develop social customs based on the conventional use of the right hand, such as in handshakes (Bishop, 1990). Yet, this theory does not explain how this preference originated and took precedence over a possible left one. Even if this were a chance
event, or an event shaped by tool use (which should be designed for use by as many
individuals in the community as possible) one would expect to find the opposite pattern in at least some isolated communities. Yet, no evidence has emerged to date of
communities where there are equal numbers of right- and left-handers or a population-level preference towards left-hand use. Another question that might arise with
regards to this account is the following: if right hand preference is merely a learned
behavior why aren’t all humans right-handed? The persisting minority of left-handers, a minority which is subject to the same cultural pressures as the right-handed
majority (or even stronger pressures, as they deviate from the norm) and whose percentage seems to have remained fairly constant throughout history, race, and geographical variation refutes the theory of environmental pressures. Moreover, asymmetry in behavior is evident even before birth, as discussed above (Hepper et al.,
1991; O’Rahilly & Muller, 1987). It thus seems fair to conclude that there must be
a biological predisposition toward using the right hand rather than the left one.
Previc (1991) claims that asymmetries derive from maternal anatomy in that the
256
M. Papadatou-Pastou
intrauterine environment is laterally asymmetric in many different respects (for a
review, see Previc, 1991). These asymmetries lead to a leftward bias in foetal positioning (Calkins, 1939), which has been shown to predict later handedness
(Churchill, Igna, & Senf, 1962). Handedness is associated with parental hand preference, but Churchill et al. (1962) still found an association between foetal positioning and child handedness, even after excluding children with left-handed parents. However, the associations found were too weak to support this theory as a persuasive explanation for the strong population bias towards right-hand preference.
The most compelling theory to date is the one explaining right-hand preference by
brain hemisphere division of labour: since both speaking and handiwork require
fine motor skills, having one hemisphere of the brain (that would be the left hemisphere for reasons described below) do both would be more efficient than having it
divided up. In other words, right hand preference might as well be a secondary characteristic of cerebral lateralization for language.
Therefore, language dominance in one of the two hemispheres is advantageous
for the full realisation of language skills, and it may also drive right-hand preference. The question that comes about is why would the left hemisphere be the dominant one, instead of the right. One might argue that this was merely a chance event:
out of the two hemispheres, the left one just happened to develop into the dominant
one for language functions and fine motor skills. For example, McManus (1991),
claims that handedness and language dominance are perhaps a result of a genetic
mutation whereby a gene that once influenced the asymmetry of the viscera, caused
instead the asymmetric development of the brain. Thus, the hypothesis was put forward (Seddon & McManus, 1993) that right-handedness, along with the capacity to
make and use tools, use language, and show functional and anatomical cerebral specialization, are characteristics which are intimately tied together in the divergence
of human beings from the apes, an evolutionary event placed at around two and a
half million years ago (Calvin, 1982; Frost, 1980; Varney & Vilensky, 1980).
However, evidence points away from left-hemisphere dominance being a chance
event, and towards different properties of the two hemispheres that could have led
to the segregation of functions. For example, timing differences between the hemispheres have been proposed to explain left-hemispheric dominance for language
(Belin, Zilbovicius, Crozier, Thivard, & Fontaine, 1998). More specifically, a prelinguistic advantage of the left-hemisphere for processing information with a high
temporal precision has been described (Hammond, 1982; Lackner & Teuber, 1973).
The presence of this advantage is supported by psychoacoustic and neuropsychological studies, which have outlined the very rapid acoustic changes of speech and
their critical importance for speech perception (Belin et al., 1998; Merzenich et al.,
Handedness and language lateralization
257
1996; Tallal et al., 1996; Tallal & Piercy, 1973).
Another explanation focuses on action coding. In the left hemisphere, actions
are coded through auditory, visual, and motor components, whereas in the right
hemisphere action coding seems to occur only via the visual and motor channels
(Aziz-Zadeh, Iacoboni, Zaidel, Wilson, & Mazziota, 2004). Therefore, the coding
in the left hemisphere encompasses all the contents of an action. This greater number of modalities available selectively to the left hemisphere may have allowed for
a more abstract representation of actions that make this hemisphere better suited
for facilitating the emergence of language (Hauser, Chomsky, & Fitch, 2002). In
addition to the above theories, it has been suggested that the right hemisphere
matures before the left (Geschwind & Galaburda, 1987), which means that it is subjected to fewer influences during development. Thus, the right hemisphere is the
locus of the processes essential to survival, such as attention and analysis of external space and emotion (Geschwind & Galaburda, 1987).
On a different line of thinking, the hypothesis has been put forward that a gestural system of communication with dominance of the right hand provided the neural architecture for vocal articulation in human evolution (Hewes, 1973; Kimura,
1984). This communication system may be based on “mirror” neurons, that is, premotor neurons that fire when monkeys perform goal-directed actions but also when
monkeys observe another monkey making the same actions (Arbib, 2001; Gallese,
Fadiga, Fogassi, & Rizzolatti, 1996; Rizzolatti & Arbibi, 1998). A mirror system for
manual actions may have been important in establishing a means of nonverbal communication and it is possible that neural properties supporting language might have
evolved from this system (Fadiga, Craighero, Buccino, & Rizzolatti, 2002; Hari et
al., 1998; Meister et al., 2003). The critical role of Broca’s area in manual imitation
(Heiser, Iacoboni, Maeda, Marcus, & Mazziota, 2003; Iacoboni et al., 1999) supports this hypothesis. In fact, Rizzolatti and Arbib (1998) showed that the mirror
system in monkeys is the homologue of Broca’s area and argued that this observation provides the missing link for the suggestion that primitive forms of communication based on manual gesture preceded speech in the evolution of language (as
opposed to previous theories treating right hand preference as a secondary characteristic of left hemisphere dominance). Furthermore, the recent discovery of auditory mirror neurons that fire when monkeys make an action, watch the same action,
or hear the sound of the action (e.g., breaking a peanut) in the dark, has tied this
system to the auditory modality, which is of crucial importance to human language
(Keysers et al., 2003; Kohler et al., 2002). Using transcranial magnetic stimulation
(TMS) for measuring motor corticospinal excitability of hand muscles in humans
while listening to sounds, Aziz-Zadeh et al. (2004) recently showed that sounds
258
M. Papadatou-Pastou
associated with manual actions produced greater corticospinal excitability than
sounds associated with leg movements or control sounds. More importantly, they
demonstrated that this facilitation was exclusively lateralized to the left hemisphere.
CONCLUSION
Handedness and cerebral laterality for language share an intimate relationship, with
around 90% of the population preferring the right hand for unimanual actions,
while over 90% of right-handers and around 70-80% of left-handers have their language functions located at their left hemisphere. These functional asymmetries are
argued to have emerged due to the adaptive advantages they convey both for the
individual, namely advanced performance through practise and increased neural
capacity, and the population as a whole through a heterozygote advantage that
maintains the genetic variation and which accounts for optimal brain organization.
A number of evolutionary, cultural and genetic theories, as well as theories
focusing on maternal anatomy have been developed in order to explain why the
right hand/left hemisphere pattern has been established, over a possible left
hand/right hemisphere one; they have been unconvincing. The most plausible explanation put forward to date is that certain properties of the left-hemisphere make it
more adept for hosting language functions. At the same time, speaking and handiwork both require fine motor skills and should be ideally handled by the same hemisphere, resulting in a right-hand preference. These left-hemisphere properties
might include the processing of information with a high temporal precision or the
fact that coding in the left hemisphere encompasses all the contents of an action
(i.e., auditory, visual, and motor components). Other plausible explanations include
maturation differences between the two hemispheres, or the fact that a right-handed
gestural system of communication might have preceded the emergence of language,
and provided the basis of its neural architecture in the left hemisphere.
In conclusion, asymmetry seems to be a prerequisite for optimal function rather
than an epiphenomenon of evolution and critical for an organism’s adaptation to
the environment. The ever-important functions of manual praxis and communication via language not only exhibit strong asymmetries, but their right hand/left
hemisphere pattern is as intriguing as it is a paradigm of the efficiency and adeptness of the human brain.
Handedness and language lateralization
259
REFERENCES
Annett, M. (1985). Left, right, hand and brain: The right shift theory. Hove, UK: Erlbaum.
Arbib, M. A. (2001). Co-evolution of human consciousness and language. Annals of the New
York Academy of Sciences, 929, 195-220.
Archer, L. A., Campbell, D., & Segalowitz, S. J. (1988). A prospective study of hand preference and language development in 18- to 30-month-olds: I. Hand preference.
Developmental Neuropsychology, 4, 85-92.
Aziz-Zadeh, L., Iacoboni, M., Zaidel, E., Wilson, S., & Mazziota, J. (2004). Left hemisphere
motor facilitation in response to manual action sounds European Journal of Neuroscience,
19, 2609-2612.
Bahn, P. G. (1989). Early teething troubles. Nature, 337, 693.
Bates, E., O’Connell, B., Vaid, J., Sledge, P., & Oakes, L. (1986). Language and hand preference in early development. Developmental Neuropsychology, 2, 837-842.
Belin, P., Zilbovicius, M., Crozier, S., Thivard, L., & Fontaine, A. (1998). Lateralization of
speech and auditory temporal processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(4), 536-540.
Berker, E. A., Berker, A. H., & Smith, A. (1986). Translation of Broca’s 1865 report:
Localisation of speech in the third left frontal convolution. Archives of Neurology, 43,
1065-1072.
Bisazza, A., Rogers, L. J., & Vallortigara, G. (1998). The origins of cerebral asymmetry: A
review of evidence of behavioural and brain lateralization in fishes, reptiles and amphibians. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 22(3), 411-426.
Bishop, D. V. M. (1990). Handedness and developmental disorder. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Black, D., de Chardin, T., Young, C. C., & Pei, W. C. (1933). Fossil man in China: The
Choukoutian cave deposits with synopsis of our present knowledge of the late Cenozoic
in China. Geological Memoirs: Series A11. Geological Survey of China, pp. 1–158.
Blau, A. (1946). The master hand: A study of the origin and meaning of right and left sidedness
and its relation to personality and language (Research Monograph No 5). New York: The
American Orthopsychiatric Association.
Bradshaw, J. L., & Nettleton, N. C. (1983). Human cerebral asymmetry. New York: PrenticeHall.
Brinton, D. (1986). Left-handedness in North American aboriginal art. American
Anthropologist, 9, 175-181.
Broca, P. (1965). Sur le siège de la faculté du language articulé; suivies d’une observation
d’aphémie (perte de la parole) [Remarks on the seat of the faculty of articulated language; Following an observation of aphemia (loss of speech)]. Bulletin de la Société
d’Anthropologie de Paris, 6, 377-393.
Buckner, R. L., Raichle, M. E., & Petersen, S. E. (1995). Dissociation of human prefrontal
cortical areas across different speech production tasks and gender groups. Journal of
Neurophysiology, 74, 2163-2173.
Burgess, J. W., & Villablanca, J. R. (1986). Recovery of function after neonatal or adult
260
M. Papadatou-Pastou
hemispherectomy in cats. II: Limb bis and development, paw usage, locomotion and
rehabilitative effects of exercise. Behavioural Brain Research, 20, 1-18.
Byrne, R. W., & Byrne, J. M. (1991). Hand preferences in the skilled gathering tasks of
mountain gorillas (Gorilla g. berengei). Cortex, 27, 521-546.
Calkins, L. A. (1939). The etiology of occiput presentations. American Journal of Obstetrics
and Gynecology, 37, 918-623.
Calvin, W. H. (1982). Did throwing stones shape hominid brain evolution? Ethology and
Sociobiology, 3, 115-124.
Cavill, S., & Bryden, P. (2003). Development of handedness: comparison of questionnaire
and performance-based measures of preference. Brain and Cognition, 53, 149-151.
Churchill, J. A., Igna, E., & Senf, R. (1962). The association of position at birth and handedness. Pediatrics, 29, 307-309.
Cioni, G., & Pellegrinetti, G. (1982). Lateralization of sensory and motor functions in human
neonates. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 54, 1151-1158.
Collins, R. L. (1985). On the inheritance of direction and degree of asymmetry. In S. D. Glick
(Ed.), Cerebral lateralization in non-human species (pp. 41-71). New York: Academic.
Corballis, M. C. (1991). The lopsided ape. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Corballis, M. C., & Beale, I. L. (1976). The psychology of right and left. Oxford, UK: Erlbaum.
Coren, S., & Porac, C. (1977). Fifty centuries of right-handedness: The historical record.
Science, 198, 631-632.
Corey, D. M., Hurley, M. M., & Foundas, A. L. (2001). Right and left handedness defined:
A multivariate approach using hand preference and performance measures.
Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology, 14, 144-152.
Dart, R. A. (1949). The predatory implemental technique of Australopithecus. American
Journal of Physical Anthropology, 7, 1-38.
deCastro, J. M. B., Bromage, T. G., & Jalvo, Y. F. (1988). Buccal striations on fossil human
anterior teeth: evidence of handedness in the middle and early Upper Pleistocene.
Journal of Human Evolution, 17, 403-412.
Denenberg, V. H. (1981). Hemispheric laterality in animals and the effects of early experience. Behavior and Brain Sciences, 4, 1-49.
Fadiga, L., Craighero, L., Buccino, G., & Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Speech listening specifically
modulates the excitability of tongue muscles: A TMS study. European Journal of
Neuroscience, 15, 399-402.
Foundas, A. L., Leonard, C. M., Gilmore, R., Fennell, E., & Heilman, K. M. (1994). Planum
temporale asymmetry and language dominance. Neuropsychologia, 32, 1225-1231.
Frith, C. D., Friston, K. J., Liddle, P. F., & Franckowiak, R. S. (1991). A PET study of word
finding. Neuropsychologia, 29, 1137-1148.
Frost, G. T. (1980). Tool behavior and the origins of laterality. Journal of Human Evolution,
9, 447- 459.
Frost, J. A., Binder, J. R., Springer, J. A., Hammeke, T. A., Bellgowan, P. S., & Rao, S.
(1999). Language processing is strongly left lateralized in both sexes. Evidence from functional MRI. Brain, 122, 199-208.
Handedness and language lateralization
261
Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119, 593-609.
Geschwind, N., & Galaburda, A. M. (1985). Cerebral lateralization -Biological mechanisms,
associations, and pathology: II. A hypothesis and a program for research. Archives of
Neurology, 42, 521-552.
Geschwind, N., & Galaburda, A. S. (1987). Cerebral lateralization: Biological mechanisms,
associations, and pathology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Gesell, A., & Ames, L. B. (1947). The development of handedness. Journal of Genetic
Psychology, 70, 155-175.
Gladue, B. A., & Bailey, J. M. (1995). Spatial ability, handedness, and human sexual orientation. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 20(5), 487-497.
Goodwin, R. S., & Michel, G. F. (1981). Head orientation position during birth and in infant
neonatal period, and hand preference at nineteen weeks. Child Development, 52, 819-826.
Groenen, M. (1988). Les représentations de mains négatives dans les grottes de Gargas et de
Tibiran (Hautes-Pyrénées) [Representations of hand stencils in caves and Tibiran Gargas
(Hautes-Pyrenees)]. Bulletin de la Société Royale Belge d’Anthropologie et de Préhistoire,
99, 81-113.
Groenen, M. (1997). La latérisation dans les représentation de mains négatives
paléolithiques [The lateralization of the representation of hands in the Paleolithics].
Monovre, 14, 31-59.
Güntürkün, O., & Hausmann, M. (2003). The dual coding hypothesis of human cerebral
asymmetries. Journal of Neurological Science, 20(3), 140-150.
Habib, M., & Robichon, F. (2003). Structural correlates of brain asymmetry: studies in lefthanded and dyslexic individuals. In K. Hugdahl & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The asymmetrical brain (pp. 681-716). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Hahn, W. K. (1987). Cerebral lateralization of function: From infancy through childhood.
Psychological Bulletin, 101, 376-392.
Hammond, G. R. (1982). Hemispheric differences in temporal resolution. Brain and
Cognition, 1, 95-118.
Hari, R., Forss, N., Avikainen, S., Kirveskari, E., Salenius, S., & Rizzolattin, G. (1998).
Activation of human primary motor cortex during action observation: a neuromagnetic
study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 95, 15061-15065.
Harris, L. J. (1983). Broca on cerebral control for speech in righ-handers and left-handers: A
note on translation and some further comments. Brain and Language, 45, 108-120.
Harris, L. J. (1989). Footedness in parrots: three centuries of research, theory, and mere
surmise. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 43, 369-376.
Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: what is it, who
has it, and how did it evolve? Science, 298, 1569-1579.
Heiser, M., Iacoboni, M., Maeda, F., Marcus, J., & Mazziota, J. (2003). The essential role of
Broca’s area in imitation. European Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 1123-1128.
Hellige, J. B., Taylor, K. B., Lesmes, L., & Peterson, S. (1998). Relationship between brain
262
M. Papadatou-Pastou
morphology and behavioral measures of hemispheric asymmetry and interhemispheric
interaction. Brain and Cognition, 36, 158-192.
Hepper, P. G., Shahidullah, S., & White, R. (1991). Handedness in the human fetus.
Neuropsychologia, 29, 1107-1111.
Hewes, G. W. (1973). Primate communication and the gestural origin of language. Current
Anthropology, 14, 5-32.
Hiscock, M. (1998). Brain lateralization across the life span. In B. Stemmer & H. A.
Whitaker (Eds.), Handbook of neurolinguistics (pp. 357-368). San Diego, CA: Academic.
Holtzen, D. W. (1994). Handedness and sexual orientation. Journal of Clinical and
Experimental Neuropsychology, 16, 702-712.
Hoosain, R. (1990). Left-handedness and handedness switch amongst the Chinese. Cortex,
26, 451-454.
Huheey, J. E. (1975). Concerning the origin of handedness in humans. Behavior Genetics,
7(1), 29-32.
Iacoboni, M., Woods, R. P., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziotta, J. C., & Rizzolatti, G.
(1999). Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science, 286, 2526-2528.
Jäncke, L., & Steinmetz, H. (1993). Auditory lateralization and planum temporale asymmetry. Neuroreport, 5, 169-172.
Keysers, C., Kohler, E., Umilta, M. A., Nanetti, L., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2003). Audiovisual
mirror neurons and action recognition. Experimental Brain Research, 153, 628-636.
Kimura, D. (1973). The asymmetry of the human brain. Scientific American, 228, 70-78.
Kimura, D. (1984). The origin of human communication. In J. Robson (Ed.), Origin and evolution of the universe: Evidence for design? (pp. 227-246). Montreal, Canada: McGillQueens Press.
Kirk, S. A. (1935). Hemispheric cerebral dominance and hemispheric equipotentiality.
Comparative Psychological Monographs, 11, 1-41.
Knecht, S., Dräger, B., Deppe, M., Bobe, L., Lolmann, H., Flöel, A., Ringelstein, E.-B., &
Henningsen, H. (2000). Handedness and hemispheric language dominance in healthy
humans. Brain, 123, 2512-2518.
Kohler, E., Keysers, C., Umilta, M. A., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2002).
Hearing sounds, understanding actions: action representation in mirror neurons. Science,
297, 846-848.
Lackner, J. R., & Teuber, H.-L. (1973). Alterations in auditory fusion thresholds after cerebral injury in man. Neuropsychologia, 11, 409-415.
Lalueza, C., & Frayer, D. W. (1997). Non-dietary marks in the anterior dentition of the
Krapina neanderthals. International Journal of Osteoarchaelogy, 7, 133-149.
LeMay, M. (1976). Morphological cerebral asymmetries of modern man, fossil man and nonhuman primate. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 280, 349-366.
Levy, J. (1977). The mammalian brain and the adaptive advantage of cerebral asymmetry.
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 299, 264-272.
Levy, J. L., & Levy, J. M. (1978). Human lateralization from head to foot: Sex-related fac-
Handedness and language lateralization
263
tors. Science, 200, 1291-1292.
Lewin, R. (1986). Dexterous early hominids. Science, 231, 115.
Lieberman, P. (1984). The biology and evolution of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Luria, A. R. (1976). Basic problems in neurolinguistics. The Hague, The Netherlands:
Mouton.
MacNeilage, P. F., Studdert-Kennedy, M. G., & Lindblom, B. (1987). Primate handedness
reconsidered. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 10, 247-263.
Marchant, L. F., & McGrew, W. C. (1991). Laterality of function in apes: A meta-analysis of
methods. Journal of Human Evolution, 21, 425-438.
Martin, M., & Jones, G. V. (1999). Motor imagery theory of a contralateral handedness effect
in recognition memory: Toward a child psychology of cognition. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General, 128, 265-282.
Mason, O. T. (1896). Throwing sticks. American Anthropologist, 9, 226-232.
McManus, I. C. (1991). The inheritance of left-handedness. In G. R. Bock & J. Marsh (Eds.),
Biological asymmetry and handedness (Ciba foundation symposium 162) (pp. 251-281).
Chichester, UK: Wiley.
McManus, I. C., & Bryden, M. P. (1992). The genetics of handedness, cerebral dominance
and lateralization. In I. Rapin & S. J. Segalowitz (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology:
Vol.6. Child neuropsychology (pp. 115-142). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
McManus, I. C., & Bryden, M. P. (1993). The neurobiology of handedness, language, and
cerebral dominance: a model for the molecular genetics of behavior. In M. H. Johnson
(Ed.), Brain development and cognition: A reader (pp. 679-702). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Meister, I. G., Boroojerdi, B., Foltys, H., Sparing, R., Huber, W., & Topper, R. (2003). Motor
cortex hand area and speech: Implications for the development of language.
Neuropsychologia, 41, 401-406.
Merzenich, M., Jenkins, W., Johnston, P., Schreiner, C., Miller, S., & Tallal, P. (1996).
Temporal processing deficits of language-learning impaired children ameliorated by
training [see comments]. Science, 271, 77-81.
Moffat, S. D., Hampson, E., & Lee, D. H. (1998). Morphology of the planum temporale and
corpus callosum in left handers with evidence of left and right hemisphere speech representation. Brain, 121, 2369-2379.
Nelson, W. J. (2003). Adaptation of core mechanisms to generate cell polarity. Nature,
422(6933), 766-774.
Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97-113.
O’Rahilly, R., & Muller, F. (1987). Developmental stages in human embryos. Washington, DC:
Carnegie Institute of Washington.
Papadatou-Pastou, M., Martin, M., Munafó, M. R., & Jones, G. V. (2008). Sex differences in
left-handedness: A meta-analysis of 144 studies. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 677-699.
Papadatou-Pastou, M., Martin, M., Munafó, M. R., & Jones, G. V. (2010). Incidence of hand-
264
M. Papadatou-Pastou
edness: A systematic review of laterality among 1.8M individuals. Manuscript in preparation.
Previc, F. H. (1991). A general theory concerning the prenatal origins of cerebral lateralization in humans. Psychological Review, 98(3), 299-334.
Pujol, J., Deaus, J., Losilla, J. M., & Capdevila, A. (1999). Cerebral lateralization of language
in normal left-handed people studied by functional MRI. Neurology, 52, 1038-1043.
Purves, D., White, L. E., & Andrews, T. J. (1994). Manual asymmetries and handedness.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 91, 5030-5032.
Quack, M. (1989). Structure and Dynamics of Chiral Molecules. Angewandte Chemie
International Edition in English, 28(5), 571-586.
Ratcliff, G., Dila, C., Taylor, L., & Milner, B. (1980). The morphological asymmetry of the
hemispheres and cerebral dominance for speech: A possible relationship. Brain and
Language, 11, 87-98.
Rizzolatti, G., & Arbibi, M. A. (1998). Language within our grasp. Trends in Neurosciences
22(4), 151-152.
Sakharov, A. D. (1991). Baryon asymmetry of the universe. Soviet Physics Uspekhi, 34(5), 417-421.
Seddon, B. M., & McManus, I. C. (1993). The incidence of left-handedness: A meta-analysis.
Unpublished manuscript.
Spenneman, D. R. (1984). Handedness data on the European Neolithic. Neuropsychologia,
22, 613-615.
Springer, J. A., Binder, J. R., Hammeke, T. A., Swanson, S. J., Frost, J. A., Bellgowan, P. S.
F., Brewer, C. C., Perry, H. M., Morris, G. L. & Mueller, W. M. (1999). Language dominance in neurologically normal and epilepsy subjects: A functional MRI study. Brain,
122, 2033-2046.
Szaflarski, J. P., Binder, J. R., Possing, E. T., McKierman, K. A., Ward, B. D., & Hammeke,
T. A. (2002). Language lateralization in left-handed and ambidextrous people: fMRI
data. Neurology, 59, 238-244.
Tallal, P., Miller, S., Bedi, G., Byma, G., Wang, X., Nagarajan, S., Schreiner, C., Jenkins, W.
M., & Merzenich, M. M. (1996). Language comprehension in language-learning impaired
children improved with acoustically modified speech [see comments]. Science, 271, 81-84.
Tallal, P., & Piercy, M. (1973). Defects of non-verbal auditory perception in children with
developmental aphasia. Nature, 241, 468-469.
Toth, N. (1985). Archaeological evidence for preferential right handedness in the lower and
middle Pleistocene and its possible implications. Journal of Human Evolution, 14, 607-614.
Tzourio, N., Crivello, F., Mellet, E., Nkanga-Ngila, B., & Mazoyer, B. (1998). Functional
anatomy of dominance for speech comprehension in left handers vs. right handers.
NeuroImage, 8, 1-16.
Uguru-Okorie, D. C., & Arbuthnott, G. W. (1981). Altered paw preference after unilateral
6-hydroxydopamine injections into lateral hypothalamus. Neuropsychologia, 19, 463-467.
Vallortigara, G., & Rogers, L. J. (2005). Survival with an asymmetrical brain: Advantages and
disadvantages of cerebral lateralization. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 575-633.
Vallortigara, G., Rogers, L. J., & Bisazza, A. (1999). Possible evolutionary origins of cogni-
Handedness and language lateralization
265
tive brain lateralization. Brain Research Review, 30(2), 164-175.
Van Biervleit, J. J. (1899). L’homme gauche et l’homme droit. Revue Philosophique, 47, 113.
Varney, N. R., & Vilensky, J. A. (1980). Neuropsychological implications for pre-adaptation
and language evolution. Journal of Human Evolution, 9, 223-226.
Wada, J., & Rasmussen, T. (1960). Intracarotid injection of sodium amytal for the lateralization of cerebral speech dominance. Journal of Neurosurgery, 17, 266-282.
Wernicke, C. (1984). Der aphasische Symptomenkomplex; Eine psychologische Studie auf
anatomischer Basis [The symptoms of aphasia: A psychological study on an anatomical
basis]. Breslau, Germany: Cohn and Welgert.