Download csa5011_frequency

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts
no text concepts found
Transcript
Corpora and Statistical Methods
Albert Gatt
Zipf’s law and the Zipfian distribution
Identifying words
Words
 Levels of identification:
 Graphical word (a token)
 Dependent on surface properties of text
 Underlying word (stem, root…)
 Dependent on some form of morphological analysis
 Practical definition: A word…
 is an indivisible (!) sequence of characters
 carries elementary meaning
 is reusable in different contexts
Indivisibility
 Words can have compositional meaning from parts that are
either words themselves, or prefixes and suffixes
 colour + -less = colourless (derivation)
 data + base = database (compounding)
 The notion of “atomicity” or “indivisibility” is a matter of
degree.
Problems with indivisibility
 Definite article in Maltese
 il-kelb
DEF-dog
“the dog”
 phonologically dependent on word
 German componding
 Lebensversicherunggesellschaftsangestellter
“life insurance company employee”
 Arabic conjunctions:
 waliy
 One possible gloss: and I follow (w- is “and”)
Resuability
 Words become part of the lexicon of a language, and can be
reused.
 But some words can be formed on the fly using productive
morphological processes.
 Many words are used very rarely
 A large majority of the lexicon is inaccessible to native speakers
 Approximately 50% of the words in a novel will be used only
once within that novel (hapax legomena)
The graphic definition
 Many corpora, starting with Brown, use a definition of a
graphic word:
 sequence of letters/numbers
 possibly some other symbols
 separated by whitespace or punctuation
 But even here, there are exceptions.
 Not much use for tokenisation of languages like Arabic.
Non-alphanumeric characters
 Numbers such as 22.5
 in word frequency counts, typically mapped to a single type ##
 Other characters:
 Abbreviations: U.S.A.
 Apostrophes: O’Hara vs. John’s
 Whitespace: New Delhi
 A problem for tokenisation
 Hyphenated compounds:
 so-called, A-1-plus vs. aluminum-export industry
 How many words do we have here?
Tokenisation
 Task of breaking up running text into component words.
 Crucial for most NLP tasks, as parameters typically estimated based
on words.
 Can be statistical or rule-based. Often, simple regular
expressions will go a long way.
 Some practical problems:
 Whitespace: very useful in Indo-European languages. In others (e.g.
East Asian languages, ancient Greek) no space is used.
 Non-alphanumeric symbols: need to decide if these are part of a word
or not.
Types and tokens
Running example
 Throughout this lecture, data is taken from a corpus of
Maltese texts:
 ca. 51,000 words
 all from Maltese-language newspapers
 various topics and article types
 Compared to data from English corpora taken from Baroni
2007
Definitions (I)
 token = any word in the corpus
 (also counting words that occur more than once)
 type = all the individual, different words in the corpus
 (grouping words together as representatives of a single type)
 Example:
 I spoke to the chap who spoke to the child
 10 tokens
 7 types (I, spoke, to, the, chap, who, child)
Definitions (II)
 The number of tokens in the corpus is an estimate of overall
corpus size
 Maltese corpus: 51,000 tokens
 The number of types is an estimate of vocabulary size
 gives an idea of the lexical richness of the corpus
 Maltese corpus: 8193 types
Relative measures of frequency
 Type-token ratio:
 no. occurrences of a type / corpus size
 essentially relative frequency
 In very large corpora, this is typically multiplied by a
constant
 e.g. multiplying by 1 million gives frequency per million
Type/token ratio
 Ratio varies enormously depending on corpus size!
 If the corpus is 1000 words, it’s easy to see a TTR of, say,
40%.
 With 4 million words, it’s more likely to be in the region of
2%.
 Reasons:
 vocab size grows with corpus size but
 large corpora will contain a lot of types that occur many times
Frequency lists (BNC)
 A simple list, pairing each word with its frequency
type
frequency
the
6054231
in
1931797
time
149487
year
73167
man
57699
…
monarch
744
cumin
51
prestidigitation
3
Frequency lists (MT)
type
aħħar (“last”)
jkun (“be.IMPERF.3SG”)
ukoll (“also”)
bħala (“as”)
dak (“that.SGM”)
tat- (“of.DEF”)
frequency
97
96
93
91
86
86
Frequency ranks
 Word counts can get very big.
 most frequent word in the Maltese corpus occurs 2195 times (and the
corpus is small)
 Raw frequency lists can be hard to process.
 Useful to represent words in terms of rank:
 count the words
 sort by frequency (most frequent first)
 assign a rank to the words:
 rank 1 = most frequent
 rank 2 = next most frequent
 …
Rank/frequency profile (BNC)
 rank 1 goes to the most frequent type
 all ranks are unique
 ties in frequency are given arbitrary rank
rank (r)
1
2
3
freq (f)
6054231
1931797
149487
…
Note the
large
differences
in frequency
from one
rank to
another
Rank-frequency profile (MT)
Rank (r)
Frequency (f)
1
2195
2
2080
3
1277
4
1264
Differences
in frequency
from one
rank to
another are
smaller
than in
BNC.
Frequency spectrum (MT)
 A representation that shows,
for each frequency value, the
number of different types
that occur with that
frequency.
frequency types
1
4382
2
1253
3
661
4
356
Word distributions (few giants, many midgets)
Non-linguistic case study
 Suppose we are interested in measuring people’s height.
 population = adult, male/female, European
 sample: N people from the relevant population
 measure height of each person in the sample
 Results:
 person 1: 1.6 m
 person 2: 1.5 m
 …
Measures of central tendency
 Given the height of individuals in our sample, we can
calculate some summary statistics:
 mean (“average”): sum of all heights in sample, divided by N
 mode: most frequent value
 What are your expectations?
 will most people be extremely tall?
 extremely short?
 more or less average?
Plotting height/frequency
Observations:
1. Extreme values
are less
frequent.
2. Most people fall
on the mean
3. Mode is
approximately
same as mean
4. Bell-shaped
curve
(“normal”
distribution)
Distributions of words
 Out of 51,000 tokens in the Maltese corpus:
 8016 tokens belong to just the 5 most frequent types (the types at ranks 1 --
5)
 ca. 15% of our corpus size is made up of only 5 different words!
 Out of 8193 types:
 4382 are hapax legomena, occurring only once (bottom ranks)
 1253 occur only twice
 …
 In this data, the mean won’t tell us very much.
 it hides huge variations!
Ranks and frequencies (MT)
1.
2.
3.
2195
2080
1277
Among top ranks, frequency drops
very dramatically (but depends on corpus size)
…
2298. 1
2299. 1
…
Among bottom ranks, frequency drops very
gradually
General observations
 There are always a few very high-frequency words, and many
low-frequency words.
 Among the top ranks, frequency differences are big.
 Among bottom ranks, frequency differences are very small.
So what are the high-frequency words?
 Top 5 ranked words in the Maltese data:
 li (“that”), l- (DEF), il- (DEF), u (“and”), ta’ (“of ”), tal- (“of the”)
 Bottom ranked words:

żona (“zone”) f = 1
 yankee f = 1
 żwieten (“Zejtun residents”) f = 1
 xortih (“luck.3SGM”) f = 1
 widnejhom (“ear.POSS.3PL”) f = 1
Frequency distributions in corpora
 The top few frequency ranks are taken up by function words.
 In the Brown corpus, the 10 top-ranked words make up 23% of total
corpus size (Baroni, 2007)
 Bottom-ranked words display lots of ties in frequency.
 Lots of words occurring only once (hapax legomena)
 In Brown, ca. ½ of vocabulary size is made up of words that occur
only once.
Implications
 The mean or average frequency hides huge deviations.
 In Brown, average frequency of a type is 19 tokens. But:
 the mean is inflated by a few very frequent types
 most words will have frequency well below the mean
 Mean will therefore be higher than median (the middle value)
 not a very meaningful indicator of central tendency
 Mode (most frequent frequency value) is usually 1.
 This is typical of most large corpora. Same happens if we look at n-grams rather
than words.
Typical shape of a rank/frequency curve
Actual example (MT)
A few high frequency,
low-rank words
Hundreds of low-frequency,
frequency
high-rank words
2500
frequency
2000
1500
frequency
1000
500
0
0
1000
2000
3000
4000
rank
5000
6000
7000
8000
9000
Zipf’s law
 Observation: Frequency decreases non-linearly with rank.
C
f ( w) 
r ( w) a
a constant, determined from data,
roughly the frequency of the most
frequent word
a constant, determined from data
 Suppose a = 1, and C = 60,000.
 The model predicts:
 2nd most frequent word will be C/2 = 30,000
 3rd most frequent: C/3 = 20,000
 20th most frequent = C/20 = 3000
 So frequency decreases very rapidly (exponentially) as rank increases.
Things to note
 The law doesn’t predict frequency ties
 there are no ties among ranks
 The law is a power law: frequency is a function of negative
power of rank
 Taking the log of both sides gives us a linear function:
log f (w)  log C  a log r (w)
 Basically a straight line plot.
Log-log plot for MT data (a=1)
Deviation from prediction for
high frequencies
Deviation from prediction
for low frequencies
Log-log plot for data from Baroni 2007
Some observations
 Empirical work has shown that the law doesn’t perfectly
predict frequencies:
 at the bottom ranks (low frequencies), actual frequency drops
more rapidly than predicted
 at the top ranks (high frequencies), the model predicts higher
frequencies than actually attested
Mandelbrot’s law
 Mandelbrot proposed a version of Zipf’s law as follows:
C
f ( w) 
a
(r ( w)  b)
 (Note: Zipf’s original law is Mandelbrot’s law with b = 0)
 If b is a small value, it will make frequency of items ranked at the top
(rank 1, 2, etc) significantly smaller, but won’t affect the lower ranks.
Comparison
 Let C = 60,000, a = 1 and b = 1
 Then, for a word of rank 1:
 Zipf’s law predicts f(w) = 60,000/1 = 60,000
 Mandelbrot’s law predicts f(w) = 60,000/(1+1) = 30,000
 For a word of rank 1000:
 Zipf predicts: f(w) = 60,000/1000 = 60
 Mandelbrot: f(w) = 60,000/1001 = 59.94
 So differences are bigger at the top than at the bottom.
Linear version of Mandelbrot
log f (w)  log C  a log(r (w)  b)
 Note: this is no longer a linear curve, so should fit our data
better.
Consequences of the law
 Data sparseness: no matter how big your corpus, most of the
words in it will be of very low frequency.
 You can’t exhaust the vocabulary of a language: new words
will crop up as corpus size increases.
 implication: you can’t compare vocabulary richness of corpora
of different sizes
Explanation for Zipfian distributions
 Zipf’s own explanation (“least effort” principle):
 Speaker’s goal is to minimise effort by using a few distinct
words as frequently as possible
 Hearer’s goal is to maximise clarity by having as large a
vocabulary as possible
Other Zipfian distributions
 Zipf’s law crops up in other domains (e.g. distribution of
incomes)
 Even randomly generated character strings show the same
pattern!
 short strings will be few, but likely to crop up by chance
 more long strings, but each one less likely individually