Download This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Causes of mental disorders wikipedia , lookup

Separation anxiety disorder wikipedia , lookup

Diagnosis of Asperger syndrome wikipedia , lookup

Asperger syndrome wikipedia , lookup

Pro-ana wikipedia , lookup

Spectrum disorder wikipedia , lookup

Glossary of psychiatry wikipedia , lookup

Child psychopathology wikipedia , lookup

Generalized anxiety disorder wikipedia , lookup

Dissociative identity disorder wikipedia , lookup

Biology of depression wikipedia , lookup

Externalizing disorders wikipedia , lookup

Death anxiety (psychology) wikipedia , lookup

Social anxiety disorder wikipedia , lookup

Prejudice wikipedia , lookup

Depression in childhood and adolescence wikipedia , lookup

Schizophrenia wikipedia , lookup

Behavioral theories of depression wikipedia , lookup

Anhedonia wikipedia , lookup

Schizotypy wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached
copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research
and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution
and sharing with colleagues.
Other uses, including reproduction and distribution, or selling or
licensing copies, or posting to personal, institutional or third party
websites are prohibited.
In most cases authors are permitted to post their version of the
article (e.g. in Word or Tex form) to their personal website or
institutional repository. Authors requiring further information
regarding Elsevier’s archiving and manuscript policies are
encouraged to visit:
http://www.elsevier.com/copyright
Author's personal copy
Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 419–424
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Personality and Individual Differences
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid
Primary and secondary negative schizotypal traits in a large non-clinical sample
Alex S. Cohen *, Russell A. Matthews
Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University, United States
a r t i c l e
i n f o
Article history:
Received 10 June 2009
Received in revised form 24 March 2010
Accepted 13 April 2010
Available online 5 May 2010
Keywords:
Schizotypy
Negative
Schizophrenia
Deficit
Apathy
Anhedonia
Social
Anxiety
Depression
Functioning
a b s t r a c t
The negative symptoms of schizophrenia reflect behavioral and affective deficits and are etiologically heterogeneous, reflecting either ‘‘primary” (i.e., apathy) or ‘‘secondary” sources (e.g., depression, anxiety,
medication side effects). This distinction is critical for understanding treatment response, illness course
and a host of neurocognitive, neurobiological and functioning variables in schizophrenia. Negative
schizotypy, defined in terms of subclinical negative traits, occurs in a sizeable portion of the adult population and is associated with increased risk for developing schizophrenia-spectrum disorders as well as
a host of neurocognitive and functional anomalies. It is, as yet, unclear whether primary and secondary
sources characterize negative schizotypy in a similar manner as schizophrenia. The present study contrasted putative ‘‘primary” (i.e., apathy) and ‘‘secondary” (i.e., depression and anxiety) causes in their
relationships to negative schizotypy and quality of life in a sample of 1356 non-clinical adults. Our data
suggests that negative schizotypy reflects two distinct ‘‘mechanisms”, one involving a putatively primary
source (i.e., social anhedonia) and the other reflecting a putatively secondary one (i.e., depression). These
primary and secondary mechanisms were separately important for understanding quality of life impoverishments. Implications of a ‘‘two-process” model of negative schizotypy are discussed.
Ó 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction
Negative symptoms, defined in terms of expressive and experiential deficits, reflect integral features of schizophrenia-spectrum
pathology in that they are associated with poor prognosis and a
range of neurocognitive, pathophysiological, and functional maladies (Blanchard & Cohen, 2006). Subclinical ‘‘negative symptoms”
that occur in non-clinical populations are also important for understanding the disorder as they often predate onset of psychosis by
years (Walker, Grimes, Davis, & Smith, 1993) and are predictive
of future schizophrenia-spectrum pathology (Gooding, Tallent, &
Matts, 2005; Kwapil, 1998). Accordingly, these negative traits are
thought to reflect a latent vulnerability marker of the genetic liability to schizophrenia-spectrum disorders – also known as ‘‘schizotypy” (e.g., Docherty & Sponheim, 2008; Meehl, 1962 but see
Meehl, 1990). In patients with schizophrenia, negative symptoms
reflect behaviors and subjective states that are etiologically heterogeneous – a fact that has greatly complicated our understanding
of them (Carpenter, Heinrichs, & Wagman, 1988). The present project examined the degree to which heterogeneity is also a feature
of psychometrically-identified negative schizotypy.
* Corresponding author. Address: Department of Psychology, Louisiana State
University, 236 Audubon Hall, Baton Rouge, LA 70808, United States. Tel.: +1 225
578 7017; fax: +1 225 578 4125.
E-mail address: [email protected] (A.S. Cohen).
0191-8869/$ - see front matter Ó 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.04.010
Carpenter et al. (1988) outline two general classes of negative
symptoms in schizophrenia patients: those that are ‘‘primary”
(i.e. ‘‘core” or ‘‘deficit” symptoms) and those related to depression,
anxiety, medication side effects, chronic social isolation and other
‘‘secondary” causes (Carpenter et al., 1988; Kirkpatrick, Buchanan,
Ross, & Carpenter, 2001). Secondary negative symptoms, which
may present similarly to primary ones during cross-sectional
assessment, are generally thought to be more transient, or exogenous in nature. Conversely, core negative symptoms are meant to
capture Kraepelin’s dementia praecox construct (Carpenter et al.,
1988) reflecting a ‘‘weakening of those emotional activities which
permanently form the mainspring of volition” and characterized by
‘‘extinction of affection for relatives and friends, of satisfaction in
their work and vocation. . . [that is] the first and most striking
symptom of the onset of the disease.” (Kraepelin, 1971). In this regard, the weakening of emotional activities, or ‘‘apathy” (Cohen,
Minor, & Najolia, in press) as it has been referred to, is postulated
to be a critical and enduring feature of the disorder (Kirkpatrick
et al., 2001). To date, well over a hundred peer-reviewed studies
support the importance of primary negative symptoms in schizophrenia (e.g., Kirkpatrick et al., 2001).
There is good reason to suspect that both primary and secondary causes contribute to negative schizotypy in a similar manner as
in schizophrenia. Although it is unlikely that negative schizotypy
reflects side effects from antipsychotic medications or chronic
institutionalization, symptoms of clinical distress such as
Author's personal copy
420
A.S. Cohen, R.A. Matthews / Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 419–424
depression and anxiety are common in schizotypy (Cohen, Leung,
Saperstein, & Blanchard, 2006; Lenzenweger & Loranger, 1989)
and could conceivably manifest in negative schizotypal traits
(e.g., ‘‘interacting with others is too anxiety provoking”, ‘‘I am too
depressed to smile”). Conversely, schizotypal negative traits might
also resemble primary/‘‘core” negative symptoms in that they reflect apathy, particularly in social situations. Social anhedonia – defined as a diminished capacity to experience pleasant emotions in
social interactions, has long been thought central to negative
schizotypy (Meehl, 1962 but see Meehl, 1990). In support of the
distinction between primary and clinical distress-related symptoms within schizotypy, several recent studies report that anxiety/depression and social anhedonia reflect separable constructs
in healthy adults (Brown, Silvia, Myin-Germeys, Lewandowski, &
Kwapil, 2008; Chmielewski & Watson, 2008; Lewandowski et al.,
2006). However, their respective contributions to the broader construct of negative schizotypy have yet to be elucidated. In this paper, we postulate that the structure of negative schizotypal traits is
similar to that seen in schizophrenia more generally, – defined in
terms of separate ‘‘primary” and ‘‘secondary” pathways.
The ‘‘apathy” postulated to reflect primary negative symptoms
warrants elaboration as its nature has yet to be fully elucidated.
A notable ambiguity concerns the degree to which apathy extends
to both pleasant attitudinal domains (e.g., ‘‘I don’t receive pleasure
from interacting with others”) and unpleasant ones (e.g., ‘‘It
doesn’t bother me to interact with others during conflicts or arguments”) as is explicitly stated in the deficit syndrome definition
(Kirkpatrick et al., 2001).1 Studies employing symptom and trait
affectivity instruments to explore this issue generally find decreased
pleasant and increased unpleasant experiences (Horan, Blanchard,
Clark, & Green, 2008) in both schizophrenia and schizotypy as a
group. However, the question of whether a subgroup of patients
shows a unilateral declination in emotional experience is more complicated. There is evidence suggesting that deficit patients show
higher levels of self-reported social anhedonia (Horan & Blanchard,
2003) and lower levels of suspiciousness, social anxiety and other
unpleasant social-based emotions (Kirkpatrick et al., 2001). As yet,
the degree to which negative schizotypy reflects diminished emotional range in both pleasant and unpleasant domains remains unclear – a secondary issue examined in the present study.
Finally, it is important to understand negative schizotypy within the context of ‘‘real world” functioning and quality of life. Negative symptoms, compared to other symptoms of the disorder, are
integral for understanding both schizophrenia and schizotypy because they are linked to impaired social functioning and impoverished quality of life (Malla et al., 2004). Interestingly, examinations
of deficit symptoms (Horan & Blanchard, 2003) and clinical distress
(Reine, Lançon, Tucci, Sapin, & Auquier, 2003) in patients with
schizophrenia have revealed that they are both associated with
poorer functioning and impoverished quality of life. Insofar as deficit symptoms and clinical distress are mutually exclusive, this
raises questions about whether their effects on ‘‘real world” variables reflect distinct pathways. We are aware of no studies to separately evaluate the potential differential contributions of primary
versus secondary negative schizotypal traits to quality of life – another knowledge gap addressed in the present study.
The present study examined affective, symptom, and functional
variables in a sample of 1356 young adults recruited from a college
1
Our use of the term apathy here is meant to include both pleasant and unpleasant
domains. We conceptualize pleasant social apathy as being identical to social
anhedonia, and for purposes of this article, will use the terms interchangeably. We do
note that social anhedonia is not synonymous with negative schizotypy, rather, that it
is one potential indicator of negative schizotypy. This is important to note as we
evaluate the social anhedonia literature which often employs social anhedonia as a
proxy for negative schizotypy.
setting. Examination of a non-clinical sample allows for understanding latent schizophrenia vulnerabilities in a population
whose insight is, by and large, not demonstrably impaired. Moreover, as noted above, the potential confounds of some secondary
causes of negative symptoms, such as environmental deprivation
and medication side effects, are reduced allowing for a clearer view
of clinical distress symptoms. College samples are commonly used
for schizotypy research, and it is worth noting that nearly a quarter
of college students with psychometrically-identified schizotypy recruited as part of a ten-year longitudinal study met criteria for a
schizophrenia-spectrum disorder by study’s end (Kwapil, 1998).
Thus, schizotypy, as measured in a college sample, is by no means
benign. Our first aim was to examine potential primary (i.e., apathy) and secondary causes (i.e., clinical distress) of negative schizotypal traits. Second, we determined the relative contributions that
primary and secondary negative schizotypal traits make to quality
of life. Through both of these analyses, we sought to disentangle
the relative importance of pleasant versus unpleasant apathy as
components of primary negative schizotypal traits.
2. Methods
2.1. Participants
Potential participants were undergraduate students (n = 8591)
who were contacted via email to participate in an on-line survey
and offered a chance to win monetary compensation as part of a
lottery (10 prizes of $25us). Embedded within this survey were a
consent form, basic demographic questions, and measures of
schizotypal symptomatology, clinical symptomatology, apathy,
and quality of life. The response rate was 17% (n = 1507). Of these
responses, 9% (n = 138) of the questionnaires was discarded because of incomplete responses (n = 141) or questionable validity
(n = 10; detailed below). The final dataset included 1356 participants. Demographic and descriptive variables are included in Table 1. This study was approved by the appropriate Human
Subject Review Board and all subjects offered informed consent
prior to completing the surveys.
2.2. Measures
2.2.1. Schizotypal symptoms
Schizotypal symptoms were assessed using the brief
Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (Raine & Benishay, 1995), a
Table 1
Means and standard deviations for the demographic and descriptive statistics
(n = 1356).
Demographic variables
Data
Possible range
Age
% Female
Ethnicity
% Caucasian
% African American
% Asian-American
% Hispanic
% Other
Negative schizotypal traits
Potential secondary sources
Depression
Anxiety
Potential primary sources
Pleasant apathy
Unpleasant apathy
Quality of life
Objective quality of life
Subjective quality of life
19.18 ± 2.03
65%
18–51
–
–
81.7%
8.4%
3.1%
3.2%
3.6%
6.18 ± 3.57
0–16
10.71 ± 4.82
9.78 ± 4.09
6–30
6–30
11.26 ± 4.27
21.59 ± 3.29
1–31
1–31
47.93 ± 5.04
25.46 ± 5.80
1–65
1–42
Author's personal copy
A.S. Cohen, R.A. Matthews / Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 419–424
22-item measure that has demonstrated adequate psychometric
properties in a number of prior schizotypy studies (e.g., Axelrod,
Grilo, Sanislow, & McGlashan, 2001). The SPQ items are organized
into one of nine subscales that mirror the diagnostic criteria of
DSM-IV schizotypal personality disorder (American Psychiatric
Association, 1994). The original brief-SPQ employs a forced choice
‘‘yes” or ‘‘no” response format. To address concerns that dichotomous response formats are insensitive to degrees of symptom
severity, we adopted a 5-point Likert scale system that has been
employed in recent research using the Full SPQ (Wuthrich & Bates,
2005). Response options ranged from ‘‘Strongly Disagree” to ‘‘Neutral” to ‘‘Strongly Agree”. The Likert scale version of the full SPQ has
shown high convergence and improved internal reliability (a = .95)
when administered in either computer or standard paper and pencil versions compared to the original version (Wuthrich & Bates,
2005).
Informed by a recent item-level factor analysis of the full SPQ
(Chmielewski & Watson, 2008), we defined negative schizotypy
in terms of the ‘‘No Close Friends” and ‘‘Constricted Affect” items
(a = .76). Although some prior research has indicated that these
scales load on a more general ‘‘Interpersonal” factor along with
‘‘Social anxiety” and ‘‘Suspiciousness” subscales, (e.g., (Raine, Reynolds, Lencz, & Scerbo, 1994), we excluded these subscales in an attempt to capture traits not overtly contaminated by depression,
social anxiety, or other obvious secondary psychopathological
states. Internal consistency was good for each of the subscales
(i.e., Cronbach’s a for the Cognitive-Perceptual factor = .79, for
the Interpersonal factor = .86, and for the Disorganization factor = .83), Prior studies have demonstrated high convergence with
the full SPQ measure, convergence with other measures of schizotypal traits, and good reliability (Cohen, Matthews, Najolia, &
Brown, in press; Raine & Benishay, 1995). The psychometric properties of this scale from this study are presented as part of a larger
investigation into the SPQ (Cohen, Matthews et al., in press).
2.2.2. Quality of life
A modified version of Lehman’s Quality of Life brief interview
(Lehman, 1995), covering the prior month, was used. Presented
in this study is a summary score reflecting 23 objective quality of
life items covering seven domains (i.e., home, daily activities, family, social, financial, health and legal concerns). These items cover
objectively anchored assessments of behavior (i.e., ‘‘How often do
you make plans ahead of time to do something with a friend?”)
and access to resources (i.e., ‘‘In the past month, did you have enough money for fun activities?”). A separate summary score
reflecting nine items assessing subjective satisfaction across these
seven domains (using a standardized 7-point Likert scale) is included in the revised brief QOL. The brief QOL has been used in
over a 100 peer-reviewed studies to date, and has demonstrated
acceptable psychometric properties (Lehman, 1995; Cohen & Davis, 2009). The newer modified version of the QOL has also been
used in prior research (e.g., Bellack, Bennett, Gearon, Brown, &
Yang, 2006). Although these measures are typically used in patient
studies, prior research has supported their use in university samples (Cohen & Davis, 2009). Internal consistency for the subjective
scale from this study was good (Cronbach’s a = .82), and adequate
for the objective scale (Cronbach’s a = .53). Increasing scores reflect
better functioning.
2.2.3. Potential secondary negative trait sources
As asserted by the manifest schizophrenia literature (Kirkpatrick et al., 2001), potential secondary negative schizotypy trait
sources were measured by depression and anxiety. The ‘‘Depression” and ‘‘Anxiety” subscales from the Brief Symptom Inventory
(Derogatis & Melisaratos, 1983), a self-report measure of psychiatric symptomatology with extensive use in prior studies, were
421
employed. Symptoms were rated on a 4-point Likert scale reflecting the prior month epoch. Increasing scores reflect increasing
symptom severity. The BSI has been used in hundreds of published
research studies, and has demonstrated good reliability and convergent validity (see Derogatis & Melisaratos, 1983). Internal consistency for these scales from our study was good (i.e., Cronbach’s a
for anxiety = .88, depression = .83).
2.2.4. Potential primary negative trait sources
Social apathy, a construct considered primary in theories of
negative symptoms (Kirkpatrick et al., 2001), was employed here
as a measure of primary negative trait sources. Unfortunately, we
were unable to identify a suitable existing measure of social apathy
that included matched measures of pleasant and unpleasant domains, thus we developed a measure for the purpose of this study,
called the Explicit Social Attitudes Scale (Cohen, Beck, Najolia, &
Brown, 2010). Participants were asked to separately rate pleasant
(i.e., ‘‘How pleasant do you feel towards these people?”; a = .72)
and unpleasant (i.e., ‘‘How unpleasant do you feel towards these
people?”; a = .70) trait apathy to six social domains (i.e., parents,
authority figures, friends, intimate significant others, strangers,
passing associates using a 6-point Likert scale from ‘‘very pleasant/unpleasant” (coded ‘‘1”) to not at all pleasant/unpleasant”
(coded ‘‘5”) to ‘‘n/a – I don’t have any of these individuals in my
life” (coded ‘‘6”). For the present study, we report separate total
scores of pleasant and unpleasant items with increasing scores
reflecting increasing apathy. Good reliability and convergent validity for this measure has been demonstrated in a larger sample of
non-clinical adults (Cohen et al., 2010).2
2.2.5. Infrequency scale
To screen out responders who provided random or grossly invalid responses, we included four questions from the Infrequency
Scale (Chapman & Chapman, 1983). Individuals who endorsed
two or more infrequency items were excluded from this study.
2.3. Analyses
The analyses were conducted in three steps. First, we sought to
ascertain the relationships between primary sources (i.e., apathy),
secondary sources (i.e., depression/anxiety), negative schizotypy
(from the SPQ), and quality of life scores by inspecting a zero-order
correlation matrix. Second and third, we determined the relative
contributions of primary and secondary sources to schizotypy negative traits and to subjective and objective quality of life by examining the standardized beta weights from linear regressions. As
part of these analyses, pleasant and unpleasant social apathy,
two potential primary sources, were considered separately.
3. Results
3.1. Zero-order correlations
Zero-order correlations are presented in Table 2. There are four
noteworthy findings. First, negative schizotypy scores significantly
corresponded to increasing severity of depression and anxiety
symptoms (two secondary negative trait sources) at a medium effect size level (using Cohen, 1988). Second, negative schizotypy
scores were significantly related to both pleasant and unpleasant
2
A portion of subjects (n = 314) indicated they did not have any significant-others
in their lives. These individuals had significantly more negative symptoms (t’s > 3.48,
p’s < .01), depression (t[1354] = 2.21, p < .05), and worse social functioning
(t’s > 14.15, p’s < .001) but not more anxiety (t[1354] = .75, p = .45) than the rest of
the sample. None of the results of this study changed in any meaningful way when
these subjects were excluded from the analyses.
Author's personal copy
422
A.S. Cohen, R.A. Matthews / Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 419–424
1.
2.
3
4
5
6
Unpleasant social apathy was associated with better subjective
quality of life. The total variance explained by these variables
was 19% for objective and 49% for subjective quality of life.
1.00
–
–
–
–
–
4. Discussion
.52
.42
1.00
.69
–
1.00
–
–
–
–
–
–
.48
.31
.39
.39
.25
.37
1.00
.19
–
1.00
–
–
.40
.52
.31
.59
.18
.50
.40
.53
.16
.40
1.00
.39
Table 2
Zero-order correlation matrix for the variables examined in this study (n = 1356).
Schizotypy Traits
1. Negative Traits
Potential secondary sources
2. Depression
3. Anxiety
Potential primary sources
4. Pleasant Apathy
5. Unpleasant Apathy
Quality of life
6. Objective
7. Subjective
The present data provides insight into the nature of negative
schizotypy. There are three important findings from this study.
First, similar to what is observed in schizophrenia more generally,
negative schizotypy appears to reflect at least two separate mechanisms, one characterized by primary sources, defined in terms of a
trait-like social apathy/anhedonia, and secondary sources, defined
in terms of clinical distress. The constructs of social anhedonia and
depression appear to be particularly important for understanding
negative schizotypy. Insofar as they made distinct contributions
to negative schizotypy, they appear to be distinct. Second, negative
schizotypy does not appear to be associated with a unilateral declination in social experience, but rather appears to be characterized
by social anhedonia and increased unpleasant emotions. Finally, a
range of primary and secondary negative schizotypy sources are
each important for understanding both objective and subjective
quality of life. This finding is consistent with others (Cohen & Davis, 2009) in highlighting the deleterious effects of negative symptoms even at the subclinical level.
Given that clinical distress, as a secondary source, and social
anhedonia, as a primary source, demarcate distinct pathways to
negative schizotypy, an important unknown issue regards the degree to which they reflect different illness courses. We would
hypothesize that social anhedonia reflects a more ‘‘primary” stable
trait (Blanchard, Horan, & Brown, 2001) such that social anhedonia
reflects a risk marker specific to deficit schizophrenia – characterized by enduring and idiopathic negative symptoms. In support of
this, individuals high in social anhedonia show more pronounced
schizoid symptoms at three (Gooding et al., 2005) and 10 year
(Kwapil, 1998) follow-up assessments compared to individuals
high in positive schizotypy. Similarly, individuals identified as having deficit symptoms early in the course of their illness tend to
show chronic deficit symptoms over a 20-year epoch (Strauss, Harrow, Grossman, & Rosen, in press). We further hypothesize that
secondary negative traits, those related to depression or anxiety
are more transient in nature and tend to demarcate a dramatically
different illness expression – one with a more paranoid, schizoaffective or otherwise non-deficit psychotic flavor. If correct, these
hypotheses suggest that distinguishing between primary and secondary negative schizotypal traits, just as in schizophrenia, is
important for understanding the type of illness that may manifest.
In short, primary versus secondary traits may signal fundamentally
distinct conditions.
All values statistically significant p < .05.
apathy scores (both primary negative trait sources), with the correlation magnitude being stronger for pleasant than unpleasant
scores (Fisher r-to-z comparisons; all z’s > 30.34. p’s < .001). Third,
negative schizotypy scores (from the SPQ) were associated with
poorer subjective and objective quality of life at a medium to large
effect size. Fourth, each of the potential primary (i.e., apathy) and
secondary (i.e., depression/anxiety) negative schizotypy scores
were significantly associated with poorer objective and subjective
quality of life, although these effect sizes ranged from small to
large.
3.2. Primary versus secondary underpinnings of negative schizotypal
traits
Table 3 contains the results of the regressions employing negative schizotypy scores as the dependent variables. The largest independent contributions to the variance in negative schizotypy were
made by depression symptoms (a secondary source) and pleasant
social apathy (a primary source), with anxiety symptoms and
unpleasant social apathy explaining a statistically significant but
more modest amount of independent variance. Overall, the four
predictor variables explained 38% of the variance in negative
symptom scores.
3.3. Primary versus secondary underpinnings of quality of life
The regressions employing quality of life as the dependent variable are also included in Table 3. The variance in both objective
and subjective quality of life scores were most explained by pleasant social apathy (a primary source) and depression (a secondary
source), with anxiety (a secondary source) and unpleasant social
apathy (a primary source) contributing a more modest amount.
Table 3
Regression analyses examining the relative contributions of unpleasant affect and apathy to negative and psychotic schizotypy symptoms and quality of life. Standardized beta
weights (b) and partial correlation values (rpartial) are reported here.
Predictor variables
Dependent variables
Negative schizotypy
R2 = .38
F [4, 1351] = 206.61***
**
***
Objective quality
R2 = .19
F [4, 1351] = 77.58***
Subjective quality
R2 = .49
F [4, 1351] = 329.70***
b
rpartial
b
rpartial
Potential secondary sources
Depression
Anxiety
.21***
.09**
.24
.09
.20***
.05
.15
.04
.28***
.16***
.26
.15
Potential primary sources
Pleasant apathy
Unpleasant apathy
.27***
.11***
.36
.11
.36***
.04
.32
.04
.35***
.17***
.41
.21
p < .01.
p < .001.
rpartial
b
Author's personal copy
A.S. Cohen, R.A. Matthews / Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 419–424
In terms of the primary negative traits examined here, social
apathy was not global in nature but specific to pleasant domains.
Unpleasant social apathy, which is theorized to be a feature of deficit schizophrenia (Kirkpatrick et al., 2001), was actually inversely
related to negative schizotypy. This highlights the importance of
social anhedonia, as opposed to a more general social apathy, as
a constituent of negative schizotypy. At the neurobiological level,
this would suggest that circuits responsible for unpleasant social
experience are preserved whereas those involving hedonic emotions from social situations are compromised in some capacity. It
is worth noting that although patients with schizophrenia, as a
group, also show high levels of social anhedonia on trait questionnaires and symptom-rating scales, they do not show commiserate
hedonic deficits during laboratory mood induction procedures (Cohen & Minor, 2010). Evidence for ‘‘in the moment” hedonic deficits
in schizotypy is mixed with one laboratory study reporting normal
emotional modulation in individuals with social anhedonia (e.g.,
Gooding, Davidson, Putnam, & Tallent, 2002) although an experience sampling methodology study in this same population reported hedonic deficits (Brown et al., 2008). It will be critical for
future research to employ multi-modal assessments to clarify the
affective underpinnings of negative schizotypy, particularly as it
pertains to social domains.
The present study was limited in some respects. First concerns
its reliance on self-report measures, which are problematic in that
they provide information on only subjective-perceived attitudes.
Clinical ratings, although costly for a sample size this large, would
be an important adjunct for use in further research. Second, the
data examined here were cross-sectional in nature and thus limited for understanding how various symptoms unfold over time.
Longitudinal assessment, particularly as individuals progress
through the window of risk, are essential for future research enterprises. Third, the sample was based solely on college students. As
noted above, this is common in schizotypy research and there is
a large body of research documenting that college students with
schizotypy show a host of ‘‘schizophrenia-like” maladies. Nonetheless, it is possible that the results do not generalize to all individuals with schizotypal traits. Finally, generalizability concerns are
raised in that only a minority of individuals approached to complete the survey actually completed it. It is unclear how our sample
is similar to the broader population. It could be the case that individuals with more psychopathology were more likely to participate
and are thusly, overrepresented in our sample. Alternatively, individuals with underlying psychopathology may have been reluctant
to complete the study due to paranoia, depression or other conditions. These possibilities do not detract from the results, which are
that both primary and secondary causes appear to underlie negative schizotypal traits.
In sum, the present project sheds light on the mechanism by
which negative symptoms manifest in individuals with schizotypal
traits. Our results highlight a ‘‘two-process” model wherein schizotypy symptoms reflect either primary (i.e., social anhedonia) or
secondary (i.e., depression) sources. The next step in this line of research would be to further clarify how these pathways manifest in
different pathological expressions, ideally through longitudinal
analysis.
References
American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (DSM-IV) (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Axelrod, S. R., Grilo, C. M., Sanislow, C., & McGlashan, T. H. (2001). Schizotypal
Personality Questionnaire-Brief: Factor structure and convergent validity in
inpatient adolescents. Journal of Personality Disorders, 15(2), 168–179.
Bellack, A. S., Bennett, M. E., Gearon, J. S., Brown, C. H., & Yang, Ye. (2006). A
randomized clinical trial of a new behavioral treatment for drug abuse in people
with severe and persistent mental illness. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63(4),
426–432.
423
Blanchard, J. J., & Cohen, A. S. (2006). The structure of negative symptoms in
schizophrenia: Implications for assessment. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 32–3,
238–245.
Blanchard, J. J., Horan, W. P., & Brown, S. A. (2001). Diagnostic differences in social
anhedonia: A longitudinal study of schizophrenia and major depressive
disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110(3), 363–371.
Brown, L. H., Silvia, P. J., Myin-Germeys, I., Lewandowski, K. E., & Kwapil, T. R.
(2008). The relationship of social anxiety and social anhedonia to
psychometrically identified schizotypy. Journal of Social and Clinical
Psychology, 27(2), 127–149.
Carpenter, W. T., Jr., Heinrichs, D. W., & Wagman, A. M. (1988). Deficit and
nondeficit forms of schizophrenia: The concept. American Journal of Psychiatry,
145(5), 578–583.
Chapman, L. J., & Chapman, J. P. (1983). Infrequency Scale. Madison, WI:
Unpublished test.
Chmielewski, M., & Watson, D. (2008). The heterogeneous structure of schizotypal
personality disorder: Item-level factors of the schizotypal personality
questionnaire and their associations with obsessive-compulsive disorder
symptoms, dissociative tendencies, and normal personality. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 117(2), 364–376.
Cohen, A. S., Beck, M. R., Najolia, G. M., & Brown, L. A. (2010). Decoupling implicit
measures of pleasant and unpleasant social attitudes. Journal of Behavior
Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 41(1), 24–30.
Cohen, A. S., & Davis, T. E. I. (2009). Schizotypal symptoms and quality of life in a
large nonclinical sample. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 50(5), 408–414.
Cohen, A. S., Leung, W. W., Saperstein, A. M., & Blanchard, J. J. (2006).
Neuropsychological functioning and social anhedonia: Results from
a
community
high-risk
study.
Schizophrenia
Research,
85(1–3),
132–141.
Cohen, A. S., Matthews, R., Najolia, G. M., & Brown, L. A. (in press). A
psychometrically-sound and sensitive brief measure of schizotypy: Improving
the SPQ-B. Journal of Personality Disorders.
Cohen, A. S., & Minor, K. S. (2010). Emotional experience in patients with
schizophrenia revisited: Meta-analysis of laboratory studies. Schizophrenia
Bulletin, 36(1), 143–150.
Cohen, A. S., Minor, K. S., & Najolia, G. (in press). A framework for understanding
experiential deficits in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Derogatis, L. R., & Melisaratos, N. (1983). The brief symptom inventory: An
introductory report. Psychological Medicine, 13(3), 595–605.
Docherty, A. R., & Sponheim, S. R. (2008). Anhedonia as a phenotype
for the ValÂ1-sup-5-sup-8Met COMT polymorphism in relatives of
patients with schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117(4),
788–798.
Gooding, D. C., Davidson, R. J., Putnam, K. M., & Tallent, K. A. (2002). Normative
emotion-modulated startle response in individuals at risk for schizophreniaspectrum disorders. Schizophrenia Research, 57(1), 109–120.
Gooding, D. C., Tallent, K. A., & Matts, C. W. (2005). Clinical status of at-risk
individuals 5 years later: Further validation of the psychometric high-risk
strategy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(1), 170–175.
Horan, W. P., & Blanchard, J. J. (2003). Neurocognitive, social, and emotional
dysfunction in deficit syndrome schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 65(2–3),
125–137.
Horan, W. P., Blanchard, J. J., Clark, L. A., & Green, M. F. (2008). Affective traits in
schizophrenia and schizotypy. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 34(5), 856–874.
Kirkpatrick, B., Buchanan, R. W., Ross, D. E., & Carpenter, W. T. Jr., (2001). A separate
disease within the syndrome of schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry,
58(2), 165–171.
Kraepelin, E. (1971). Dementia praecox and paraphrenia. Huntington, NY: Robert E.
Krieger Publishing Co., Inc.
Kwapil, T. R. (1998). Social anhedonia as a predictor of the development of
schizophrenia-spectrum disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107(4),
558–565.
Lehman, A. (1995). Evaluating quality of life for persons with severe mental illness:
Assessment toolkit. Cambridge, Mass.: The Evaluation Center at Health Services
Research Institute.
Lenzenweger, M. F., & Loranger, A. W. (1989). Psychosis proneness and clinical
psychopathology: Examination of the correlates of schizotypy. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 98(1), 3–8.
Lewandowski, K. E., Barrantes-Vidal, N., Nelson-Gray, R. O., Clancy, C., Kepley, H. O.,
& Kwapil, T. R. (2006). Anxiety and depression symptoms in psychometrically
identified schizotypy. Schizophrenia Research, 83(2), 225–235.
Malla, A. K., Norman, R. M. G., McLean, T. S., MacDonald, C., McIntosh, E., DeanLashley, F., et al. (2004). Determinants of quality of life in first-episode
psychosis. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 109(1), 46–54.
Meehl, P. (1962). Schizotaxia, schizotypy, schizophrenia. American Psychologist, 17,
827–838.
Meehl, P. E. (1990). Toward an integrated theory of schizotaxia, schizotypy, and
schizophrenia. Journal of Personality Disorders, 4(1), 1.
Raine, A., & Benishay, D. (1995). The SPQ-B: A brief screening instrument for
schizotypal personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 9(4),
346–355.
Raine, A., Reynolds, C., Lencz, T., & Scerbo, A. (1994). Cognitive-perceptual,
interpersonal, and disorganized features of schizotypal personality.
Schizophrenia Bulletin, 20(1), 191–201.
Author's personal copy
424
A.S. Cohen, R.A. Matthews / Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 419–424
Reine, G., Lançon, C., Tucci, S. D., Sapin, C., & Auquier, P. (2003). Depression and
subjective quality of life in chronic phase schizophrenic patients. Acta
Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 108(4), 297–303.
Strauss, G. P., Harrow, M., Grossman, L. S., & Rosen, C. (in press). Periods of recovery
in deficit syndrome schizophrenia: A 20-year multi-follow-up longitudinal
study. Schizophrenia Bulletin.
Walker, E. F., Grimes, K. E., Davis, D. N., & Smith, A. J. (1993). Childhood precursors of
schizophrenia: Facial expressions of emotion. American Journal of Psychiatry,
150, 1654–1660.
Wuthrich, V., & Bates, T. C. (2005). Reliability and validity of two Likert versions of
the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ). Personality and Individual
Differences, 38(7), 1543–1548.