Download PowerPoint

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Arabic grammar wikipedia , lookup

Georgian grammar wikipedia , lookup

Sanskrit grammar wikipedia , lookup

Old Irish grammar wikipedia , lookup

Morphology (linguistics) wikipedia , lookup

Modern Greek grammar wikipedia , lookup

Macedonian grammar wikipedia , lookup

Ojibwe grammar wikipedia , lookup

Germanic strong verb wikipedia , lookup

English clause syntax wikipedia , lookup

Malay grammar wikipedia , lookup

Distributed morphology wikipedia , lookup

Old Norse morphology wikipedia , lookup

Lexical semantics wikipedia , lookup

Latin syntax wikipedia , lookup

Inflection wikipedia , lookup

Kannada grammar wikipedia , lookup

Ukrainian grammar wikipedia , lookup

Udmurt grammar wikipedia , lookup

Portuguese grammar wikipedia , lookup

Italian grammar wikipedia , lookup

Turkish grammar wikipedia , lookup

Old English grammar wikipedia , lookup

Scottish Gaelic grammar wikipedia , lookup

Spanish verbs wikipedia , lookup

Ancient Greek grammar wikipedia , lookup

Spanish grammar wikipedia , lookup

Hungarian verbs wikipedia , lookup

Pipil grammar wikipedia , lookup

Lithuanian grammar wikipedia , lookup

French grammar wikipedia , lookup

Russian grammar wikipedia , lookup

Swedish grammar wikipedia , lookup

Bulgarian verbs wikipedia , lookup

Serbo-Croatian grammar wikipedia , lookup

English verbs wikipedia , lookup

Yiddish grammar wikipedia , lookup

Polish grammar wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
CAS LX 522
Syntax I
Week 2a. Morphosyntactic
features, part II.
Ch. 2, 4.2-
Lexical items


Recall that part of our language
knowledge is the knowledge of the
lexicon.
The lexicon is a list of the “words”
More accurately, it is a list of the things
sentences are made of.
 It is traditionally considered to be where
“unpredictable” information is stored. The
sound, the meaning, the grammatical category
and other features.

Features of lexical items


We represent these properties as features—any
given lexical item has



Semantic features
Phonological features
Syntactic features


A lexical item is a bundle of properties. It is a meaning, paired with
instructions for pronunciation, paired with syntactic properties like
category.
When it comes to syntax, syntactic features certainly matter. But no
language seems to arrange its sentences such that words that start
with t are first.
Hypothesis: Syntax can only “see” syntactic
features.
English pronouns



The English pronouns make several
distinctions over and above a singular/plural
distinction.
One distinction is in person, which is sensitive
to who is talking and to whom.
English (and most languages) distinguish
three persons.
first person
second person
third person
singular
I
plural
we
you
he/she/it
you
they
English pronouns


We do not want model this with three independent
person features [1], [2], and [3], since that would
predict eight persons (e.g., [1,3], [1,2,3]). With two
features, we only predict four.
By eliminating [3], we predict the system below, plus
the [1,2] combination that is not morphologically
distinguished in English.
first person [1]
second person [2]
third person [ ]
singular
I
plural
we
you
he/she/it
you
they
Fourth person

If [1] indicates the person speaking and [2]
indicates the person spoken to, what should
[1,2] indicate?



[1,2,pl] = we (including you).
[1,pl] = we (not including you).
Some languages make this distinction
morphologically, e.g., Dakota. No
languages seem to distinguish 8 persons.
Gender

Many languages distinguish nouns on
the basis of “gender” as well.


English: he/she/it (3rd person pronouns)
Gender often comes in 2-3 flavors
(masculine, feminine, neuter) which
often corresponds roughly to
biological gender where applicable.
Phi-features (f-features)


Collectively, person, number, and
gender features are referred to as ffeatures.
These are the features that are
generally involved in subject-verb
agreement.
Case features



English pronouns also change form
depending on where they are in the
sentence, what their syntactic role is.
He left. I saw him. He saw me.
The information about syntactic position is
encoded by case features.


In English, case is only visible on pronouns.
In many other languages, case is visible on all nouns
(and sometimes on words modifying nouns, like
adjectives or determiners)
Case names

In English, we distinguish nominative
(on subjects), genitive (on possessors),
and accusative (elsewhere).
Singular
Plural
Nom
I
you
he
Acc
me
you
him
Gen
my
your
his
Nom
we
you
they
Acc
us
you
them
Gen
our
your
their
she
it
her
it
her
its
they
they
them
them
their
their
Features and pronunciation

Recall that lexical
items are bundles of
features.



Like [Acc, 1, sg, PRN]
The syntactic system
arranges these
lexical items into
sentences, and
then hands the
result off to the A-P
and C-I systems (at
the interfaces).
At the A-P interface,
[Acc, 1, sg, PRN] is
interpreted as “me”.
Singular
Plural
Nom
Acc
Gen
Nom
Acc
Gen
I
me
my
we
us
our
you
you
your
you
you
your
he
him
his
they
them
their
she
her
her
they
them
their
it
it
its
they
them
their
Features and pronunciation

Notice that the
pronoun paradigm
does not make every
possible distinction.



Only 3rd person singular
distinguishes gender forms.
2nd person does not
distinguish number or
between Nom and Acc.
3rd person singular
feminine doesn’t
distinguish between Acc
and Gen.

This structure can give us a
hint about how the
interface rules work—more
on this in a moment.
Singular
Plural
Nom
Acc
Gen
Nom
Acc
Gen
I
me
my
we
us
our
you
you
your
you
you
your
he
him
his
they
them
their
she
her
her
they
them
their
it
it
its
they
them
their
Verbal features




Some features are specific to verbs…
[past], for example, differentiating write from
wrote, kick from kicked. This is a tense feature.
Some languages have a special form of the
verb for future as well, [future].
We can characterize present tense as being
non-past, non-future.


In English, future is expressed in other ways, with a
modal (will) or with the verb go. English does not seem to
make use of the [future] feature; in English we have just
past and non-past.
(cf. duals and the use of the [sg] feature on nouns)
Participles


English verbs can also take on a participle
form: writing, written.
These don’t express tense, but rather aspect.




The -ing form is the “present participle” and appears
after the auxiliary verb be, indicating a continuing event.
The -en form is the “past participle” and appears after the
auxiliary verb have, indicating a completed event.
Tense can still be expressed—on the auxiliary: I have
written, I had written, I am writing, I was writing.
Adger’s proposal:


Present participle:
Past participle:
[V, part]
[V, part, past]
(writing)
(written)
Bare verb/infinitive




I want to win the lottery.
The bare form of the verb (often appearing
after to) is the infinitive.
We will assign infinitive forms the feature
[Inf].
The fact that the infinitive is a bare verb (no
suffixes or other inflection) in English may be
something of a coincidence. Other
languages mark the infinitive with a special
verb form, on a par with participles or
tensed verbs.
Verb agreement

Verbs very often (across languages) agree
with the subject in f-features as well.


I eat bagels. He eats bagels. They eat bagels.
However, eat isn’t really “plural” in any
sense. Plurality is a property of the subject,
but it is reflected in the morphology of the
verb.

This may be the clearest example of the distinction
between interpretable and uninterpretable
features. The f-features are interpretable on the noun,
but uninterpretable on the verb. (We’ll continue to
discuss this distinction)
Verb agreement

In English, only finite verbs show agreement
(those that are not infinitives or participles).


In fact, only present tense verbs do, with the single
exception of the copula (be).
In other languages, agreement sometimes
appears on other forms. Participles, for
example, sometimes agree with their object.
Infinitives very rarely agree with anything.
A brief excursion



We’ve determined that English differentiates
past and nonpast, and Adger suggests
looking at this as a privative distinction,
between having the feature [past] and not
having it.
So far, this makes the same combinatorial
predictions as a binary feature [±past]
would.
Is there any way to decide which is better?
The morphology of be


 Suppose that our
Suppose our features
pronunciation rules at
are privative and we
the interface look at
want to lay out some
the feature bundle
pronunciation rules for
the A-P interface for
and determine the
the verb be.
pronunciation.
There are only five
different
[past]
pronunciations for the
[pl]
[pl]
12 cells in the
[1]
1
am
are
was
were
paradigm.
[2]
2
are
are
were
were
3
is
are
was
were
The morphology of be

Pronunciation rules:





[pl, past] = were
[pl] = are

The way this works is
that the most specific
rule that matches the
features takes priority.
Features not
mentioned don’t
matter.
[1]
1
[2]
2
3


[1, pl, past] yields “were”
[1, pl] yields “are”
[2, pl, past] yields “were”
…
[past]
am
are
is
[pl]
are
are
are
was
were
was
[pl]
were
were
were
The morphology of be

Pronunciation rules:



[pl, past] = were
[pl] = are
So let’s try to work out
the rest of the rules.

Notice that am and is only
appear in one cell; they are
the most specific. Was
appears in 2, are appears
in 3, were appears in 4.
[1]
[2]
[past]
1
2
3
am
are
is
[pl]
are
are
are
was
were
was
[pl]
were
were
were
The morphology of be

Pronunciation rules:









[pl, past] = were
[1, past] = was
[2, past] = were
[3, past] = was
[pl] = are
[1] = am
[2] = are
[3] = is

We find that we have more
rules than pronunciations— two
rules each for were, was, and
are.
But what if we could refer to the
absence of [pl]?
[past]
[1]
[2]
1
2
3
am
are
is
[pl]
are
are
are
was
were
was
[pl]
were
were
were
The morphology of be

If our features are
binary, we can
come up with a
much more
economical set of
pronunciation rules,
one per
pronunciation.
[+1,-2]
[-1,+2]
[-1,-2]
1
2
3
[-past]
[-pl] [+pl]
am
are
[+past]
[-pl]
[-pl]
was
were
are
is
were
was
are
are
were
were
The morphology of be

Notice also that were,
which occupies the
most cells in the
paradigm, is treated as
a default in these rules.
You pronounce were if
no other rule matches.
[+1,-2]
[-1,+2]
[-1,-2]





1
2
3
[+1, -past, -pl] = am
[+1, -past] = is
[-1] = was
[-past] = are
[] = were
[-past]
[-pl] [+pl]
am
are
[+past]
[-pl]
[-pl]
was
were
are
is
were
was
are
are
were
were
The morphology of be


 [+1, -past, -pl] = am
This fact can be taken in as
 [+1, -past] = is
support for viewing these
 [-1] = was
features as binary valued,
 [-past] = are
rather than privative.
 [] = were
You can write pronunciation
rules using either system, but
one system yields
significantly more elegant
[-past]
[+past]
results.
[-pl] [+pl] [-pl]
[-pl]
[+1,-2]
1
am
are
was
were
[-1,+2]
[-1,-2]
2
3
are
is
are
are
were
was
were
were
Bibliographical note and
comment about the future

This view of the syntax-morphology interface,
when you get out to the literature, generally goes
by the name “Distributed Morphology” so named
because the pronunciation rules are relatively
separate from the syntactic rules. The primary
source for this is Halle & Marantz (1993) (in Adger’s
bibliography).

For our purposes in this class, we will actually not spend much more
time analyzing pronunciation rules or even worrying about whether
features should be privative or binary— we will usually simply label
feature bundles like [+N,-V] as [N], [-pl] as [sg]. But this is a
convenience, there are interesting questions to explore at this lower
level as well— outside of this class, we have plenty of other things to
do.









